Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cognitive Styles and Psychotic Experiences in a Community Sample

  • Sarah Sullivan ,

    sarah.sullivan@bristol.ac.uk

    Affiliation Academic Centre for Mental Health Research, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • Richard P. Bentall,

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom

  • Charles Fernyhough,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Durham University, Durham, United Kingdom

  • Rebecca M. Pearson,

    Affiliation Academic Centre for Mental Health Research, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • Stanley Zammit

    Affiliations Academic Centre for Mental Health Research, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom

Abstract

Introduction

In clinical populations paranoid delusions are associated with making global, stable and external attributions for negative events. Paranoia is common in community samples but it is not known whether it is associated with a similar cognitive style. This study investigates the association between cognitive style and paranoia in a large community sample of young adults.

Methods

2694 young adults (mean age 17.8, SD 4.6) from the ALSPAC cohort provided data on psychotic experiences and cognitive style. Psychotic experiences were assessed using a semi-structured interview and cognitive style was assessed using the Cognitive Styles Questionnaire-Short Form (CSQ-SF) on the same occasion. Logistic regression was used to investigate associations between paranoia and CSQ-SF scores, both total and domain-related (global, stable, self, external). The role of concurrent self-reported depressive symptoms in the association was explored.

Results

Paranoia was associated with Total CSQ-SF scores (adjusted OR 1.69 95% CI 1.29, 2.22), as well as global (OR 1.56 95% CI 1.17, 2.08), stable (OR 1.56 95% CI 1.17, 2.08) and self (OR 1.37 95% CI 1.05, 1.79) domains, only Total score and global domain associations remained after additional adjustment for self-reported depression. There was no association between paranoia and external cognitive style (OR 1.10 95% CI 0.83, 1.47).

Conclusion

Paranoid ideation in a community sample is associated with a global rather than an external cognitive style. An external cognitive style may be a characteristic of more severe paranoid beliefs. Further work is required to determine the role of depression in the association between cognitive style and paranoia.

Introduction

Paranoia, “the tendency to believe that others hold malevolent intentions towards the self” [1] is common in psychosis [2-4]. For example, patients have delusions about being spied on, or persecuted. The common feature of these delusions is the belief that the self is the target of the malevolent intentions of others.

Paranoia is also common in nonclinical populations [5-7], with a prevalence of broadly-defined paranoia of 10%. However, there may be qualitative differences between paranoia in clinical and community populations. Paranoid patients, compared to non-patients, feel less control over their beliefs [8] and their symptoms are less sensitive to context [9]. Also, individuals with paranoia in community samples are much more likely to believe that persecution is deserved because of personal fault (‘bad-me’ paranoia) [10] whereas patients usually have either exclusively ‘poor-me’ paranoia when persecution is believed to be undeserved. They may also fluctuate between poor-me and bad-me beliefs [11,12].

Paranoia is associated with distress and help-seeking in both clinical [13] and general population samples [14] and therefore attempts have been made to understand it in terms of cognitive processes [15,16]. A focus on the individual’s cognitive style (i.e. making attributions or causal explanations about salient events), may be useful. The Attributional Theory of paranoia was developed following the observation that paranoid patients made excessively external (blaming others or the circumstances), stable (unchangeable) and global (assuming that the cause will affect all areas of life) attributions for hypothetical negative events [17]. Bentall et al [18] explains this in terms of latent negative self-schemata that are easily activated when faced with a threat to a preferred view of the self. In order to avoid negative self-thoughts, an external attribution is made to reduce the potential discrepancy between the ideal and actual self. The disadvantage of this process is that it may activate beliefs that others view the individual negatively. It is argued that the recurrent use of this kind of reasoning may result in elaborate suspicions about others and hence paranoia.

The Attributional Model has therapeutic implications because attributional style is modifiable by cognitive therapy and related interventions [19,20]. However evidence in its favour is at best mixed. Early studies found that paranoia was associated with an excessive self-serving bias (i.e. a bias towards attributing positive events to internal causes and negative events to external causes) which is greater than that found in healthy people [21-23], a finding that is consistent with the model. However, subsequent studies have failed to replicate these findings [24]. There are several possible explanations for this inconsistency. First, a variety of assessment tools have been used to measure attributional style, which make different assumptions about the way in which it should be measured (for example, about whether it is important to distinguish between external attributions that implicate others and those that implicate situational factors) [25] [26] [27]. Second, studies have been conducted in both clinical and general populations, which may differ in the quantitative and qualitative features of paranoia. Two studies [28] [29] found an abnormal self-serving bias in clinical but not community samples and another [11] found an abnormal attributional style in poor-me but not bad-me patients. Another study, with a student sample, only found an association between paranoia and an excessive self-serving bias in a sub-group who scored very highly for paranoia [30]. Third, much of the existing evidence has been hampered by the lack of adjustment for co-morbid symptoms such as depression. Depression is common in paranoid patients and known to influence attributional style [31], but is rarely controlled for. Hallucinations are a frequently observed co-morbid symptom in clinical populations [32], although at present there are no compelling theoretical reasons to assume that they are associated with attributional bias. No study to date has investigated the relationship between hallucinations and cognitive style. A final limitation of existing studies is sampling. The majority have used clinical and/or convenience samples rather than a representative community sample. This is important given evidence that paranoia in clinical and general population samples may be qualitatively different.

This study investigates the association between attributional style and paranoia and hallucinations in a large community sample of young adults, using a measure of attributional style [33]. Hallucinations were the most prevalent experience and rarely occurred alongside any of the other experiences. This allowed us to investigate the specificity of attributional style for paranoia by investigating its association with hallucinations, and by controlling for co-morbid depression.

We hypothesise that paranoid beliefs will be associated with an excessive tendency to make external, stable and global attributions for negative events. There are no specific predictions for hallucinations.

Materials and Methods

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC)

ALSPAC recruited 14,541 pregnant women resident in Avon, UK with expected dates of delivery 1st April 1991 to 31st December 1992. Of these initial pregnancies, there was a total of 14, 676 foetuses, resulting in 14, 062 live births and 13,988 children who were alive at 1 year of age. When the oldest children were approximately 7 years of age an additional 713 children were enrolled resulting in a total sample size of 15,458 foetuses. Of this total sample 14,701 were alive at 1 year. The cohort has been previously described[34,35] and is representative of the UK population [36].

Ethics Statement

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and the Southmead, Frenchay, UBHT and Weston Research Ethics Committees. Written consent was obtained from participants to allow use of anonymized linked data for research by bona fide scientists.

Measures: Outcome

Psychotic experiences age 18.

The Psychosis-Like Symptom interview (PLIKSi) [37] is a semi-structured instrument that uses the principles of standardised clinical examination developed for the Schedule for Clinical Assessment in Psychiatry (SCAN). It consists of 11 ‘core’ questions eliciting key psychotic experiences occurring since age 12, covering hallucinations (visual and auditory), delusions (being spied on, persecution, thoughts being read, reference, control, and grandiosity), and experiences of thought interference (broadcasting, insertion and withdrawal). Any unspecified delusions elicited were also rated. Cross-questioning was used to establish the presence of psychotic experiences, and coding of items followed the glossary definitions and rating rules for SCAN. Interviewers were psychology graduates trained in assessment using the SCAN Psychosis Section and in the PLIKSi. Interviewers rated experiences as not present, suspected, or definitely present. Unclear responses after probing were always ‘rated down’, and experiences only rated as definite when a credible example was provided. Interviewers discussed cases with a psychiatrist if it was unclear how an experience should be rated. At regular intervals interviewer ratings for a sample of recorded interviews were confirmed by a psychiatrist to ensure interviewers were rating experiences correctly. If the interviewer rated the experience as suspected or definitely psychotic they were asked whether experiences reported were always attributable to the effects of sleep (hypnopompic or hypnogogic experiences), fever, or substance use (defined as experiences occurring only within 2 hours of intoxication with drugs or alcohol). If the experiences were attributable to these causes they were not used in this analysis.

For the purposes of this analysis the outcome variables used were total psychotic experiences, hallucinations only, and persecutory delusions only. Each was used as a binary variable (no experiences versus suspected or definite experiences).

Reliability.

Test-retest agreement for any psychotic experience was 0.73 (SE=0.074), and was higher for interviews rated by the same interviewer at both time points (kappa=0.86, S.E=0.136). For a detailed description see Zammit et al 2013[38].

Exposure

Attributional Style at age 18.

A short form of the Cognitive Style Questionnaire (CSQ-SF) [33], developed from the longer Cognitive Style Questionnaire [39], was administered to the ALSPAC children at a clinic. The reliability and validity of the short version has been established in young adults [33], although the reliability of one domain (internal) is lower than the others. The measure focuses on 8 negative hypothetical events relating to failures in academic, employment and interpersonal relationships. For each event participants are asked to vividly imagine themselves in that situation and think carefully about the likely reason for the event. Participants then rate the extent to which this reason was caused by internal versus external factors (caused by themselves or others), specific versus global factors (will impact all areas of life or just this specific situation), stable versus unstable factors (the cause will persist and lead to the same outcome in the future) and reflects their self-worth (they are flawed in some way) on Likert scales of agreement from 1-5. For each scenario, two items related to each of 4 dimensions resulting in 8 items across 8 scenarios, 64 items in total. When calculating total scores, we summed the domains so that high total scores indicated high scores for stability, globality, implications for self and externality (rather than internality), because this was the pattern anticipated for paranoia based on previous research [40]. In order to derive an externality score the internality domain score was reversed. This way of totalling the scores is different from that used in depression research, where the internality dimension is scored so that a high score implies internal attributions. The range of the total score was from 64 to 320 and the domain scores from 16 to 80, with higher scores indicating a style hypothesised to be associated with psychotic experiences. Total score and domain scores were computed and showed normal distributions. Internal consistency for the total score in a comparable ALSPAC sample was α= 0.89 and for the domain scores was; stability α=0.74, self α=0.84, globality α=0.69 and externality α=0.56 [41] which is comparable to previous studies [33]. A principal-components analysis of the scores for the four dimensions indicated that a single factor with an eigenvalue of 3.25 explained 65% of the variance [41]. Three of the domains (globality, stability and self-worth) were highly correlated with each other (range r=0.56-0.63) although the externality domain was less strongly correlated (range r=0.19-0.32) [41]. These findings are consistent with findings from other attributional measures [33,42]. The measure was administered as an interviewer-assisted questionnaire, with respondents verbally providing the answer to the stem question of each scenario.

Potential Confounders

Potential confounders were selected using clinical knowledge of variables associated with community psychotic experiences and which have been used in previous research on cognitive style. The child-specific variables selected were: gender, IQ at age 15 measured by alternate items of the Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) and self-reported depression measured using the Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ)[43] at age 18. The family-specific variables selected were: maternal education and marital status at the child’s birth, home ownership status and social class (highest of mother’s and father’s) also measured at the child’s birth.

A median split of the CSQ-SF total score resulted in two groups: high and low. The distribution of each potential confounder was examined across the median split. Those with higher total CSQ-SF scores were more likely to be female with higher self-reported depression scores at 18 years and to have a mother of higher social class (see Table 1). These items were selected as potential confounding variables using the following rationale. Gender is likely to be related to psychotic experiences [44] and may also be associated with cognitive style [45]. There is existing evidence that social class is associated with the probability of psychotic experiences [46]. Finally, self-reported depression is associated both with cognitive style [41] and with psychotic experiences [47].

VariableLevel/MeasureTotal sampleHigh CSQ-sf total scores mean (SD)Low CSQ-sf total scores mean (SD)
Gender % M41.2%36.9%45.6%
F58.8%63.05%54.4%
IQ at 15 mean (SD)94.7 (12.6)94.9 (13.1)94.6 (12.1)
Self-reported depression at 18 years mean (SD)SMFQ range (0-26)6.4 (5.2)7.6 (5.4)5.2 (4.6)
Psychotic experiences at 12 years %suspected or definite psychotic experiences 12.7%13.4%12.1%
Ethnicity %White 95.6%95.0%96.1%
Maternal educational status %Low (CSE, vocational, O level)49.4%53.0%51.8%
High (A levels, degree) %50.6%47.0%48.7%
Maternal marital status %Never married 11.8%12.8%11.0%
Married presently or previously88.2%87.3%89.1%
Social class (highest of maternal and paternal) %I & II35.1%37.0%33.2%
III manual and non-manual50.3%47.6%52.9%
IV & V14.6%15.4%13.9%
Home ownership status %Mortgaged/owned 87.4%87.2%87.5%
Rented 12.6%12.8%12.5%

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of total sample (n=2694) and across high and low categories of total CSQ scores split at the median.

Abbreviations: SMFQ – Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire
CSV
Download CSV

Dataset

The dataset consists of a group of the cohort for whom data were available on the outcome, exposure and all confounders (n=2694) (see Figure 1).

Statistical Analysis

Logistic regression models were used to investigate associations between total CSQ-SF score, all CSQ-SF score domains and the presence of suspected or definite psychotic experiences. Confounding variables, including concurrent self-reported depression, were used to adjust the logistic regression models.

Although there were missing data previous studies on the ALSPAC cohort using imputed data have shown that the results are not materially different and therefore bias resulting from loss to follow-up has not affected the findings[48]. Consequently a complete cases analysis (i.e. all cases with data on the outcome, exposure and all confounding variables) was conducted.

Two sensitivity analyses were also performed. The first investigated whether any association was influenced differentially by those with extremely high or low CSQ-SF scores. In order to investigate this possibility the analyses above were repeated but excluded those with CSQ-SF total (number excluded = 17) and CSQ-SF domain scores (number excluded stable n=8, self n=22, global n=17 and external n=13) which were 3 SD or more above or below the mean. It is possible that early psychotic experiences may result in an altered cognitive style at 18 years, therefore the second sensitivity analysis investigated the influence of early psychotic experiences by repeating the analysis excluding participants who had also experienced suspected or definite psychotic experiences at age 12 (n=309).

Results

The participants included in this study were more likely to have parents in social classes I and II and to be female than the rest of the cohort. They were also less likely to have had psychotic experiences including paranoia (see Table 2).

VariableTime point (years)Range/categoriesCohortDataset
ProportionMean (SD)ProportionMean (SD)
Total psychotic experiences18Suspected and definite/no9.13%8.61%
Hallucinations188.76%8.28%
Paranoia182.36%1.89%
Cognitive style questionnaire - total score1864-320161.97 (20.03)162.05 (20.07)
Cognitive style questionnaire - stable score1816-8039.81 (6.68)39.85 (6.62)
Cognitive style questionnaire - self score1816-8035.91 (8.11)35.93 (8.11)
Cognitive style questionnaire - global score1816-8038.53 (6.19)38.44 (6.21)
Cognitive style questionnaire - external score1816-8055.27 (5.99)55.18 (6.00)
GenderBirthM51.36%41.26%
Parental social classBirthClasses I and II26.05%35.09%
Depressive symptoms score180-266.55 (5.23)6.44 (5.16)

Table 2. Descriptive data comparing the ALSPAC cohort (n=15602) and study dataset (n=2694).

CSV
Download CSV

Suspected or definite psychotic experiences at age 18 were detected in 232 (8.6%) participants; 20 of the 232 (8.6%) had experienced paranoia alone, 191 of the 232 (82.3%) had hallucinations alone and 21 of the 232 (9.1%) both paranoia and hallucinations. There was evidence of a statistical association between hallucinations and paranoia (χ 2 =189.64 p<0.0001) and evidence of a cross-sectional association between self-reported depressive symptoms (SMFQ scores) at 18 years and psychotic experiences (OR 1.12 95% CI 1.10, 1.15), paranoia (OR 1.12 95% CI 1.08, 1.17) and hallucinations (OR 1.12 95% CI 1.10, 1.15).

Those who had higher CSQ-SF scores were more likely to be female, have more depressive symptoms and psychotic experiences and to have parents of a lower social class (see Table 1).

The unadjusted analysis showed strong evidence of an association between paranoid delusions and CSQ-SF total score, and with all CSQ-SF sub-domains excepting the external CSQ-SF domain (see Table 3). The associations were attenuated by adjusting for gender and social class. Adjustment for concurrent self-reported depression further attenuated these associations, although there was still evidence of association between both CSQ-SF total scores and global scores and paranoid delusions.

Unadjusted paranoia Adjusted 1 Adjusted 2Unadjusted hallucinations Adjusted 1 Adjusted 2
CSQ-sf total (SD=18)1.75 (1.34, 2.27) ≤0.00011.69 (1.29, 2.22) ≤0.0001 1.34 (1.01, 1.78) 0.041.28 (1.11, 1.46) ≤0.00011.27 (1.11, 1.47) 0.0011.03 (0.89, 1.19) 0.71
CSQ-sf stable (SD=7)1.57 (1.19, 2.09) 0.0021.56 (1.17, 2.08) 0.003
  1. 1.25 (0.93, 1.68)
1.17 (1.01, 1.35) 0.031.19 (1.03, 1.38) 0.020.97 (0.83, 1.13) 0.66
CSQ-sf self (SD=7)1.43 (1.11, 1.85) 0.0051.37 (1.05, 1.79) 0.021.11 (0.86, 1.46) 0.411.09 (0.95, 1.24) 0.231.09 (0.95, 1.26) 0.200.90 (0.78, 1.04) 0.15
CSQ-sf global (SD=6)1.64 (1.28, 2.09) ≤0.00011.66 (1.30, 2.14) ≤0.00011.36 (1.05, 1.77) 0.021.30 (1.14, 1.48) 0.00011.30 (1.14, 1.49) ≤0.00011.07 (0.93, 1.23) 0.34
CSQ-sf external (SD=6)1.18 (0.89, 1.56) 0.261.10 (0.83, 1.47) 0.511.16 (0.87, 1.55) 0.221.19 (1.04, 1.37) 0.011.15 (1.00, 1.33) 0.051.21 (1.05, 1.40) 0.01

Table 3. Odds ratios, 95% confidence intervals and p values of the association between CSQ-sf scores and psychotic experiences (y/n).

Adjusted 1 – gender and social class (highest of mother’s and father’s)
Adjusted 2 – gender and social class (highest of mother’s and father’s)+ self reported depression at 18
Key – CSQ-sf Cognitive Styles Questionnaire – short form
Results expressed in terms of SD increase in CSQ-sf scores. Complete cases n=2694
CSV
Download CSV

Hallucinations were associated with CSQ-SF total scores and with the stable, global and external CSQ-SF domains in the unadjusted analysis. These remained after adjustment for gender and social class, but after further adjustment for concurrent depressive symptoms only the association with the external domain remained.

When the outcomes of paranoid delusions and hallucinations were directly compared there was evidence that the association between paranoia and CSQ-SF scores is stronger than the association between hallucinations and CSQ-SF scores for total CSQ-SF and all domains of the CSQ-SF, except for the external domain (see Table S1).

The association between paranoia and the CSQ-SF total and global domain scores may have been driven by those at the extreme end of the CSQ-SF score distribution. When the analyses above were repeated excluding any participants who scored more than three SDs above or below the mean, the associations between experience of paranoia and the CSQ-SF total score and the CSQ-SF global score were attenuated: OR 1.25 95% CI 0.92, 1.70 and OR 1.23 95% CI 0.91, 1.67 respectively. However the association between experience of hallucinations and CSQ-SF external domain score remained even after removing those with extreme scores; OR 1.26 95% CI 1.08, 1.46 (see Table S2).

Repeating the analyses excluding those who had experienced psychotic experiences at 12 years (n=309), the associations between paranoia and total and global domain CSQ-SF scores were attenuated (OR 1.22 95% CI 0.93, 1.73 and OR 1.17 95% CI 0.81 and 1.68) (see Table S3).

Discussion

Main findings

Our hypothesis that paranoid delusions would be associated with higher global, stable and external CSQ-SF domain scores was only partially supported. Although we found associations between CSQ-SF total and global scores and paranoia, we did not find an association with external or stable CSQ-SF domain scores. Moreover, unexpectedly, hallucinations were associated with the external CSQ-SF domain scores.

The findings for paranoia are not in accordance with earlier studies in clinical samples [21,22,40]. However they are in accordance with more recent studies with community samples. One study [28] found an association between an externalising bias and paranoia in a clinical group but not in a community sample at high risk of psychosis (n=88). Another, using student participants (n=114) [49], found no evidence of an association between attributional bias and paranoia across the whole sample, however in a subgroup with less depression, moderate anxiety and high levels of self-esteem, (a group which it was suggested may be a poor-me sub-type), there was evidence of a personalising bias (a tendency to excessively attribute negative events to external-personal causes). A third study [30] found only found evidence of an association between an externalising style and paranoia in a sub-group with severe paranoia.

Differences between the Present Study Findings and Previous Studies

This is the first large study of cognitive style in relation to paranoia using a representative community sample.

Previous studies have not adopted a consistent approach to minimising confounding, particularly for mood. A failure to adjust for depression may under-estimate the association between CSQ externalising scores and paranoia and may over-estimate associations between paranoia and global, stable and self CSQ domains. In our analyses, the associations between externalising and both hallucinations and paranoia were increased by adjusting for depression.

Hallucinations

Unexpected associations were observed between CSQ-SF scores and hallucinations in this study. Although both paranoia and hallucinations load on the same positive factor in analyses of patients’ symptoms [32], the relationship between the two types of experiences is probably complex. Some have argued that delusions and perceptual abnormalities result from the same cognitive deficits [50]. Others have pointed to evidence that delusional beliefs can influence source monitoring abnormalities thought to underlie hallucinations [51] but also that anomalous experiences can provoke delusional thinking [52]. A recent study of first-episode patients reported that delusions and hallucinations began within the same month in 45% of cases, delusions preceded hallucinations in 19% of cases, hallucinations preceded delusions in 16% of cases and 20% developed delusions in the absence of hallucinations [53]. In the ALSPAC sample the proportion of those experiencing both hallucinations and delusions at 18 years of age was surprisingly low. Together, these findings suggest that the association between hallucinations and delusions may be different at different points in the evolution of psychosis.

Contrary to prediction, the presence of hallucinations, rather than paranoia, was associated with an excessively external attributional style for negative events. One possibility is that the experience of hallucinations leads individuals to suppose that negative events are caused by external forces.

Limitations

The CSQ-SF only measures attributions for hypothetical negative events, in contrast to other measures which assess causal explanations for positive and negative events. Additionally the external domain of the CSQ-SF has relatively poor psychometric qualities and may have been affected by measurement error which would reduce the probability of detecting an association.

In the study dataset only 1.3% (n=35) had a psychotic disorder. These people would be more likely to have an extreme attributional style and may be a reason why an association was not found with some CSQ-SF domains.

At present it is not known what proportion of young people with psychotic experiences at 18 years will develop a psychotic illness and it is likely that many will only experience sub-syndromal psychotic states during their lifetimes.

Although designed to reduce measurement error our measure of psychotic experiences was used as a binary variable and therefore does not reflect important factors such as frequency and severity.

There was a marked reduction in the strength of the relationships between CSQ-SF scores and paranoia at 18 years when those who had experienced paranoia or hallucinations at 12 years were excluded. One possible explanation is that psychotic experiences in early adolescence cause an abnormal cognitive style or that an abnormal cognitive style in childhood is more strongly associated with early adolescent psychotic experiences than with those occurring in early adulthood. Unfortunately we do not have earlier measures of cognitive style to enable us to examine either of these possibilities. It is also possible that the attenuated associations are due to the reduction of statistical power when the sample was reduced.

The cross-sectional design of our study does not allow us to investigate whether cognitive style causes paranoia or vice versa.

Implications

Several possible implications may be drawn from our findings. Firstly, the paranoid experiences of clinical and community populations may be qualitatively different. In our community sample paranoia was associated with a more pessimistic cognitive style i.e. that the causes of the negative event would impact on many other areas. By contrast, in clinical groups paranoia appears to be associated not only with a global and stable cognitive style but also one that attributes the cause of negative findings to others. This represents a cognitive style which many would consider to be more characteristic of paranoid behaviour. Secondly, it is possible that early on in its developmental trajectory paranoia is associated with a global cognitive style which later becomes more stable and external. Thirdly, it is possible that a stable, external and global style is associated with more severe or frequent episodes of paranoia which may be under-represented in the study dataset. The final possibility concerns the lack of certainty about the meaning of community psychotic experiences. Several recent findings in the ALSPAC cohort suggest that they may be an expression of psychological distress rather than an endophenotype of psychosis. There is some evidence (unpublished data) that a global style is also associated with depressive symptoms (another form of psychological distress) in and that psychotic experiences and depressive symptoms are strongly correlated (further work conducted by the authors, details available on request) suggesting an underlying common psychological construct.

This is a clinically relevant finding as psychological interventions for people with the psychotic symptom of paranoia typically target attributional and other reasoning processes. One of the attributional styles most actively targeted is externality. Our findings suggest that it is also key to target a global style which may be important in the development of paranoia and therefore also in reducing paranoid symptomology. Interestingly, one study found that patients treated with CBT for paranoid beliefs sometimes benefited without showing a shift in attributional style [54].

Future studies addressing the psychology of paranoid thinking need to consider subtypes of paranoia (mild vs severe; poor-me vs bad-me) and the developmental evolution of paranoid thinking (subclinical vs prodromal vs first episode vs chronic). Longitudinal studies with repeated measures are required to investigate the development of cognitive style and its relationship with the emergence of depression and psychotic experiences over time.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Odds ratios & 95% confidence intervals for multinomial logistic regression of: no experiences-reference group (0), hallucinations only (1), paranoia only (2), both (3) (may have other experiences too). Complete cases n=2693.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080055.s001

(DOCX)

Table S2.

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals of the association between CSQ-sf scores and psychotic symptom (y/n); in a sample without extreme CSQ scores (i.e. >3SDs from mean)-CSQ total 19 excluded, CSQ stable 9 excluded, CSQ self 26 excluded, CSQ global 20 excluded, CSQ internal 14 excluded.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080055.s002

(DOCX)

Table S3.

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals of the association between CSQ-sf scores and psychotic symptoms (y/n). Results expressed in terms of SD increase in CSQ-sf scores. Complete cases omitting those with PE suspected or definite at age 12 n=2385.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080055.s003

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptions and nurses

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: RPB CF. Analyzed the data: SS SZ. Wrote the manuscript: SS RPB CF RMP SZ.

References

  1. 1. Freeman D, Garety PA (2000) Comments on the content of persecutory delusions: does the definition need clarification? Br J Clin Psychol 39 (4): 407-414. doi:https://doi.org/10.1348/014466500163400.PubMed: 11107494.
  2. 2. Brakoulias V, Starcevic V (2008) A cross-sectional survey of the frequency and characteristics of delusions in acute psychiatric wards. Australas Psychiatry 16: 87-91. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/10398560701633176. PubMed: 18335363.
  3. 3. Jorgensen P, Jensen J (1994) Delusional beliefs in first admitters. Psychopathology 27.
  4. 4. Jablensky A, Sartorius N, Ernberg G, Anker M, Korten A et al. (1992) Schizophrenia - Manifestations, Incidence and Course in Different Cultures - a. World-Health-Organization 10-Country Study. Psychological Medicine: 1-97.
  5. 5. Rutten B.P.F. vOJ, Domingues M, Krabbendam L (2008) Epidemiology and social factors: findings from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (DEMESIS). Freeman D BR, Garety P, editor. Persecutory delusions: Assessment, theory, treatment. . Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 53-72.
  6. 6. Johns LC, Cannon M, Singleton N, Murray RM, Farrell M et al. (2004) Prevalence and correlates of self-reported psychotic symptoms in the British population. Br J Psychiatry 185: 298-305. doi:https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.185.4.298. PubMed: 15458989.
  7. 7. Freeman D, Garety PA, Bebbington PE, Smith B, Rollinson R et al. (2005) Psychological investigation of the structure of paranoia in a non-clinical population. Br J Psychiatry 186: 427-435. doi:https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.186.5.427. PubMed: 15863749.
  8. 8. Morrison AP, Petersen T (2003) Trauma, Metacognition and Predisposition to Hallucinations in Non-Patients. Behav Cogn Psychother 31: 235-246. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465803003011.
  9. 9. Collip I, Oorschot M, Thewissen V, Van Os J, Bentall R et al. (2011) Social world interactions: how company connects to paranoia. Psychol Med 41: 911-921. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291710001558. PubMed: 20735885.
  10. 10. Trower P, Chadwick P (1995) Pathways to Defense of the Self - a Theory of 2 Types of Paranoia. Clin Psychol_Sci Practice 2: 263-278. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1995.tb00044.x.
  11. 11. Melo S, Corcoran R, Shryane N, Bentall RP (2009) The persecution and deservedness scale. Psychol Psychother_Theory Res Practice 82: 247-260. doi:https://doi.org/10.1348/147608308X398337. PubMed: 19426584.
  12. 12. Udachina A, Varese F, Oorschot M, Myin-Germeys I, Bentall RP (2012) Dynamics of self-esteem in 'poor-me' and 'bad-me' paranoia. J Nerv Ment Dis 200: 777-783. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e318266ba57. PubMed: 22922239.
  13. 13. Sullivan S, Herzig D, Mohr C, Lewis G, Corcoran R et al. (2012) Theory of mind and social functioning in first episode psychosis. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. PubMed: 22803807.
  14. 14. Combs DR, Finn JA, Wohlfahrt W, Penn DL, Basso MR (2013) Social cognition and social functioning in nonclinical paranoia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. PubMed: 23445398.
  15. 15. Bentall RP, Corcoran R, Howard R, Blackwood N, Kinderman p (2001) Persecutory; Delusions. (2001) A Review and Theoretical Integration. Clin Psychol Rev 21: 1143-1192 doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00106-4. PubMed: 11702511.
  16. 16. Freeman D, Garety PA, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Bebbington PE (2002) A cognitive model of persecutory delusions. Br J Clin Psychol 41: 331-347. doi:https://doi.org/10.1348/014466502760387461. PubMed: 12437789.
  17. 17. Kaney S, Wolfenden M, Dewey ME, Bentall RP (1992) Persecutory Delusions and Recall of Threatening Propositions. Br J Clin Psychol 31: 85-87. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb00971.x. PubMed: 1559121.
  18. 18. Bentall RP, Kinderman P, Kaney S (1994) The Self, Attributional Processes and Abnormal Beliefs: Towards a Model of Persecutory Delusions. Behav Res Therapy 32: 331-341. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)90131-7. PubMed: 8192633.
  19. 19. Beck AT, Rector NA, Stolar NM, Grant PM (2009) Schizophrenia: Cognitive theory, research and therapy. New York: Guilford Publishing House.
  20. 20. Kinderman P, Bentall RP (1997) Causal attributions in paranoia and depression: Internal, personal, and situational attributions for negative events. J Abnorm Psychol 106: 341-345. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.2.341. PubMed: 9131855.
  21. 21. Candido CL, Romney DM (1990) Attributional Style in Paranoid Vs Depressed-Patients. Br J Med Psychol 63: 355-363. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1990.tb01630.x. PubMed: 2081160.
  22. 22. Fear C, Sharp H, Healy D (1996) Cognitive processes in delusional disorders. Br J Psychiatry 168: 61-67. doi:https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.168.1.61. PubMed: 8770430.
  23. 23. Kaney S, Bentall RP (1992) Persecutory Delusions and the Self-Serving Bias - Evidence from a Contingency Judgment Task. J Nerv Ment Dis 180: 773-780. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-199212000-00006. PubMed: 1469376.
  24. 24. Bentall RP, Udachina A (2013) Social cognition and paranoid delusions. D. R Penn D. L., editors.
  25. 25. Peterson C, Semmel A, Vonbaeyer C, Abramson LY, Metalsky GI et al. (1982) The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy Res 6: 287-299. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173577.
  26. 26. Kinderman P, Bentall RP (1996) A new measure of causal locus: The internal, personal and situational attributions questionnaire. Pers Individ Dif 20: 261-264. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00186-7.
  27. 27. Lee DA, Randall F, Beattie G, Bentall RP (2004) Delusional discourse: An investigation comparing the spontaneous causal attributions of paranoid and non-paranoid individuals. Psychol Psychother_Theory Res Practice 77: 525-540. doi:https://doi.org/10.1348/1476083042555361. PubMed: 15588459.
  28. 28. Janssen I, Versmissen D, Campo AJ, Myin-Germeys I, van Os J et al. (2006) Attribution style and psychosis: evidence for an externalising bias in patients but not in individuals at high risk. Psychol Med 36: 771-778. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291706007422. PubMed: 16563206.
  29. 29. McKay R, Langdon R, Coltheart M (2005) Paranoia, persecutory delusions and attributional biases. Psychiatry Res 136: 233-245. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.06.004. PubMed: 16125787.
  30. 30. Fornells-Ambrojo M, Garety PA (2005) Bad me paranoia in early psychosis: A relatively rare phenomenon. Br J Clin Psychol 44: 521-528. doi:https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X35326. PubMed: 16368031.
  31. 31. Abramson LY, Alloy LB, Metalsky GI (1989) Hopelessness Depression - a Theory-Based Subtype of Depression. Psychol Rev 96: 358-372. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.358.
  32. 32. van Os J, Kapur Shitij (2009) Schizophrenia. Lancet 374: 635-645. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60995-8. PubMed: 19700006.
  33. 33. Meins E, McCarthy-Jones S, Fernyhough C, Lewis G, Bentall RP et al. (2012) Assessing negative cognitive style: Development and validation of a Short-Form version of the Cognitive Style Questionnaire. Pers Individ Dif 52: 581-585. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.026. PubMed: 22389545.
  34. 34. Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A et al. ( April 162012) Cohort profile: The 'Children of the 90s' - the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol April 16: 1-17. PubMed: 22523758.
  35. 35. Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A, Golding J et al. (2013) Cohort Profile: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol 42: 97-110. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys066. PubMed: 22507742.
  36. 36. Golding J, Pembrey M, Jones R, The At (2001) ALSPAC-the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. I Study methodology. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 15: 74-87. doi:https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-3016.2001.00325.x. PubMed: 11237119.
  37. 37. Horwood J, Thomas K, Duffy L, Gunnell D, Hollis C et al. (2008) Frequency of psychosis-like symptoms in a non-clinical population of 12 year olds: Results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Eur Psychiatry 23: S282-S282. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.01.595.
  38. 38. Zammit S, Kounali D, Cannon M, David A, Gunnel D et al. (2013) Prevalence and prediction of psychotic experiences, prodromal syndromes and psychotic disorder at age 18 in a longitudinal, population-based cohort study Am J Psychiatry (in press).
  39. 39. Haeffel GJ (2010) Cognitive Vulnerability to Depressive Symptoms in College Students: A Comparison of Traditional, Weakest-Link, and Flexibility Operationalizations. Cognitive Therapy Res 34: 92-98. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-008-9224-8.
  40. 40. Kaney S, Bentall RP (1989) Persecutory Delusions and Attributional Style. Br J Med Psychol 62: 191-198. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1989.tb02826.x. PubMed: 2751948.
  41. 41. Pearson RM, Fernyhough C, Bentall R, Evans J, Heron J et al. (2012) Association between maternal depressogenic cognitive style during pregnancy and offspring cognitive style 18 years later. Am J Psychiatry (. (2013)) PubMed: 23318526.
  42. 42. Reivich K (1995) The measurement of explanatory style. In: BGM SMEP. Hillsdale, NJ: Explanatory Style: Lawrance Erlbaum.
  43. 43. Angold A, Costello EJ, Messer SC, Pickles A, Winder F et al. (1995) The development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 5: 1-12.
  44. 44. Ring N, Tantam D, Montague L, Newby D, Black D et al. (1991) Gender Differences in the Incidence of Definite Schizophrenia and Atypical Psychosis - Focus on Negative Symptoms of Schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 84: 489-496. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb03182.x. PubMed: 1792920.
  45. 45. Hankin BL, Abramson LY (2001) Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory. Psychol Bull 127: 773-796. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.773. PubMed: 11726071.
  46. 46. Harrison G, Gunnell D, Glazebrook C, Page K, Kwiecinski R (2001) Association between schizophrenia and social inequality at birth: case-control study. Br J Psychiatry 179: 346-350. doi:https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.179.4.346. PubMed: 11581116.
  47. 47. Freeman D, Garety PA (2003) Connecting neurosis and psychosis: the direct influence of emotion on delusions and hallucinations. Behav Res Ther 41: 923-947. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00104-3. PubMed: 12880647.
  48. 48. Wolke D, Waylen A, Samara M, Steer C, Goodman R et al. (2009) Selective drop-out in longitudinal studies and non-biased prediction of behaviour disorders. Br J Psychiatry 195: 249-256. doi:https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.108.053751. PubMed: 19721116.
  49. 49. Combs DR, Adams SD, Penn DL, Roberts DL, Tiegreen J et al. (2007) Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Preliminary findings. Schizophr Res 91: 112-116. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.12.010. PubMed: 17293083.
  50. 50. Fletcher PC, Frith CD (2009) Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 48-58. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2536. PubMed: 19050712.
  51. 51. Haddock G, Slade PD, Bentall RP (1995) Auditory Hallucinations and the Verbal Transformation Effect - the Role of Suggestions. Pers Individ Dif 19: 301-306. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00063-C.
  52. 52. Garety PA, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D, Bebbington PE (2001) A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychol Med 31: 189-195. PubMed: 11232907.
  53. 53. Compton M, Potts A, Wan CR, Flosnik D (2012) Which came first, delusions or hallucinations? Early Interv Psychiatry 6: 53-53. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-7893.2011.00300.x. PubMed: 21952020.
  54. 54. Brakoulias V, Langdon R, Sloss G, Coltheart M, Meares R et al. (2008) Delusions and reasoning: a study involving cognitive behavioural therapy. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 13: 148-165. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800801900587. PubMed: 18302027.