Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Crustacean Hyperglycemic Hormone (cHH) as a Modulator of Aggression in Crustacean Decapods

Abstract

Biogenic amines, particularly serotonin, are recognised to play an important role in controlling the aggression of invertebrates, whereas the effect of neurohormones is still underexplored. The crustacean Hyperglycemic Hormone (cHH) is a multifunctional member of the eyestalk neuropeptide family. We expect that this neuropeptide influences aggression either directly, by controlling its expression, or indirectly, by mobilizing the energetic stores needed for the increased activity of an animal. Our study aims at testing such an influence and the possible reversion of hierarchies in the red swamp crayfish, Procambarus clarkii, as a model organism. Three types of pairs of similarly sized males were formed: (1) ‘control pairs’ (CP, n = 8): both individuals were injected with a phosphate saline solution (PBS); (2) ‘reinforced pairs’ (RP, n = 9): the alpha alone was injected with native cHH, and the beta with PBS; (3) ‘inverted pairs’ (IP, n = 9): the opposite of (2). We found that, independently of the crayfish’s prior social experience, cHH injections induced (i) the expression of dominance behaviour, (ii) higher glycemic levels, and (iii) lower time spent motionless. In CP and RP, fight intensity decreased with the establishment of dominance. On the contrary, in IP, betas became increasingly likely to initiate and escalate fights and, consequently, increased their dominance till a temporary reversal of the hierarchy. Our results demonstrate, for the first time, that, similarly to serotonin, cHH enhances individual aggression, up to reverse, although transitorily, the hierarchical rank. New research perspectives are thus opened in our intriguing effort of understanding the role of cHH in the modulation of agonistic behaviour in crustaceans.

Introduction

Given the nearly ubiquity of social hierarchy across animal species (reviewed in [1]), an intriguing purpose of behavioural studies is to understand how adaptive mechanisms underlay the formation and maintenance of social hierarchies and which chemical substances are involved in aggression.

Crayfish are excellent model organisms to study the proximate mechanisms that invertebrates adopt to establish and maintain dominance hierarchies [2]. They exhibit easily identifiable behavioural patterns that escalate giving rise to fights of increased severity until dominance hierarchies are formed [3][7]. At that point, the number and intensity of fights decrease and crayfish behave consistently with the social status achieved: the dominant displays raised postures, is the initiator of most attacks and gains first access to limited resources, whereas the subordinate individual displays submissive postures, escapes from the dominant’s attacks, and has limited access to resources [8][9].

The maintenance of stable hierarchies is certainly adaptive [10], since both fighting costs and risks of injuries are reduced [10][11], but the mechanisms underlying dominance stability is still under debate. As suggested by [12], the formation of stable dominance relationships in crayfish is driven by extrinsic factors (e.g. previous history and communication) and intrinsic chemical processes (e.g. the neurochemical state). Hence, a deeper understanding of the role of neuropeptides in triggering agonistic behaviour is crucial to solve the still heated debate around the mechanisms that maintain hierarchies.

To date, serotonin (5-HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine) is the main neuromodulator recognized to play an important role in controlling aggression in several crustacean decapods (the lobster Homarus americanus, [13][15]; the shore crab Carcinus maenas, [16]; the crab Chasmagnathus granulatus, [17]; the squat lobster Munida quadrispina, [18]), crayfish included (Procambarus clarkii, [19]; Astacus astacus, [20][21]). In particular, if compared to subordinate individuals, serotonin-treated crayfish fight for longer and more strongly, and less often retreat [20][21] [15] [19]. Serotonin has also a marked effect in elevating glucose level in hemolymph, as an adaptive response to the forthcoming fights [22]. Hyperglycaemia results from the mobilization of glycogen in target tissues (e.g. midgut glands and abdominal muscles), due to the activation of phosphorylase and the inhibition of glycogen synthase via the crustacean Hyperglycemic Hormone (cHH) [23].

Crustacean HH is a member of a family of eyestalk neuropeptides [24][25], which includes the Moult Inhibiting Hormone (MIH) and the Gonad Inhibiting Hormone (GIH): the cHH/MIH/GIH family. These neuropeptides are released through exocytosis from the sinus gland (SG), a neurohemal organ located in the eyestalk of decapod crustaceans. The main function of cHH is the regulation of glucose levels in the hemolymph. It is also involved in reproduction [26][27], moulting [28][29], lipid metabolism [30], and stress responses [31][33]. About 80 cHH-superfamily peptides have thus far been fully identified from several crustacean species.

Notwithstanding the high number of papers on the cHH chemical nature, studies on its biological activity remain still scanty [34] and, up to date, no data from the literature are available on the putative role of cHH as a modulator of aggression.

To fill this gap in knowledge, here we investigate the possible influence that cHH exerts on the agonistic behaviour of the red swamp crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Specifically, we hypothesized that cHH, similarly to serotonin, could affect crayfish behaviour to the extent of reversing the hierarchical rank in combating pairs. To test this hypothesis, we manipulated the agonistic level of males in size-matched pairs through the injection of a dose of native cHH or phosphate saline solution (PBS) into the crayfish circulation. Our aims were to (1) describe the possible effect of cHH on the agonistic behaviour of crayfish and its duration, (2) assess the increased glycaemic level due to cHH injections, and (3) test whether possible changes in aggression associated with cHH injections are sufficient to reverse an established dominance hierarchy. Our general purpose is to quantify the possible effects of cHH on crayfish agonistic behaviour and to discuss the relative importance of other intrinsic/extrinsic factors in maintaining dominance hierarchies.

Materials and Methods

Collection and Holding Conditions

About 200 male crayfish were collected using baited traps from Lake Trasimeno (Umbria, central Italy) in July 2011 by professional fishermen. Once in the laboratory, each crayfish was individually marked onto its carapace with a waterproof paint and its cephalothorax length (from the tip of the rostrum to the posterior edge of the carapace) was measured using a vernier calliper (accuracy: ±0.1 mm).

Crayfish were kept for at least two weeks at the density of 15 m−2 in plastic tanks (80×60×60 cm) containing clay pots in excess as shelter and at a natural light-dark cycle at room temperature (24°C). They were fed ad libitum with live Calliphora sp. larvae. Water was changed weekly.

Criteria for Choosing Experimental Crayfish

Only hard-shelled, intact, and sexually mature males were used for the experiment. A total of 80 individuals (cephalothorax length: 47.5±0.6 mm) were thus selected: 20 for the extraction of cHH and 60 for behavioural observations. Since dominance increases with body size in crayfish [3], the experimental pairs of fighting males were size matched (maximum difference in cephalothorax length: ±2 mm) to eliminate any factor that could induce an obvious bias to our experiments. Before the beginning of the experiment, crayfish were kept in isolation in opaque plastic aquaria (25×15×25 cm) for at least two weeks, which is a sufficient time to reset any previous social experience [35]. In no case did the crayfish meet each other prior to the experiment, so any effect of previous social experience can be excluded [36]. All crayfish were used only once to avoid pseudo-replication.

Extraction of Native cHH

Twenty animals were anesthetized for 5 min on ice before eyestalk ablation. From 40 eyestalks the crude extract of dissected sinus glands was collected by adding 200 µL of extraction solution (90% MetOH, 9% acetic acid, 1% H2O). After sonication, the sample was centrifuged at 12 000× g for 10 min at 4°C and the supernatant was collected. The pellet was suspended in 200 µL of the extraction solution, sonicated and centrifuged again, and the two supernatants were mixed together. The extract was purified on an RP-HPLC system (Gilson) equipped with a Zorbax SB-C18 4.6×150 mm column from Agilent Technologies Inc. (DE, USA) thermostated at 25°C. Mobile phase A was 0.1% TFA in water, mobile phase B was 0.1% TFA in acetonitrile. The separation was done using a gradient of 0–100% B in 60 min at 1 mL/min. The resulting chromatogram is shown in Figure 1.

thumbnail
Figure 1. RP-HPLC profile of the crude extract of sinus glands.

Mobile Phase A: 0.1% TFA in water. Mobile Phase B: 0.1% TFA in acetonitrile. Gradient: 0–100% B over 60 min at 1 mL min−1. Column: Zorbax SB-C18 4.6 × 150 mm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050047.g001

The collected fractions were analyzed on an API150EX single quadrupole mass spectrometer (ABSciex), and those fractions containing the expected molecular mass of 8386 Da [37] were pooled and lyophilized. Peptide concentration was determined by UV absorbance at 280 nm using calculated ε values of 9315 M−1 cm−1 for the peptide oxidized form. The extinction coefficient was computed using the ProtParam programme on the ExPASy server [38].

Experimental Design (Fig. 2)

Behavioural experiments were conducted in the laboratory from 0800 to 1400 h during August 2011 to reduce possible interference due to circadian changes in blood glucose level [39]. During observations, we recorded the effects through time of the injected native cHH extract on crayfish behaviour and examined whether such extract might induce a change in the hierarchy. The experiment was planned in five phases in sequence, as described below.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Horizontal time sequence of experimental design.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050047.g002

Phase 1: Hemolymph sampling and determination of initial glycemia.

The animals were blotted dry and hemolymph (about 50 µl) was drawn from the pericardial sinus into sterile 1 mL syringes fitted with 25 g needles. All the animals were bled between 0800 and 0900 h and left undisturbed for 2 h. The sample was preserved on ice for 5 min to avoid coagulation and then centrifuged at 12 000× g for 2 min at 4°C to pellet the hemocytes. The supernatant was then collected. Glucose concentration (mg dL−1) in the hemolymph was assessed using the glucose oxidase method of a commercial kit (Hospitex Diagnostics).

Phase 2: Familiarization (T0).

The two opponents were kept in an experimental aquarium (a circular opaque PVC container, diameter: 30 cm) separated by an opaque PVC divider for 10-min acclimatization. The familiarization started with the removal of the divider and lasted 20 min (T0), during which time crayfish behaviour was recorded by a digital camera (Samsung VP-L800) for subsequent blind analysis (see below). Simultaneously, an experienced observer (LA) recorded the winner of each fight so that, at the end of familiarization, we could determine the dominant –alpha crayfish (and consequently the subordinate –beta crayfish) for each pair, that is, the winner (and the loser) of more than 60% of the total fights [40]. The ‘winner’ was defined as the crayfish that did not retreat or that retreated after the opponent showed a motionless posture, which is typical of a subordinate [4]. Trials where dominance was not clearly established were excluded from the analysis. A total of 26 (out of 30) size-matched pairs were observed and alpha and beta crayfish were assessed.

Phase 3: Experimental treatments.

The above selected pairs were randomly assigned to one of the following treatments: (1) ‘control pairs’ (CP, n = 8): both males were injected with 100 µL of phosphate buffered saline (PBS); (2) ‘reinforced pairs’ (RP, n = 9): the alpha was injected with 100 µL of native cHH solution, and the beta with 100 µL of PBS; (3) ‘inverted pairs’ (IP, n = 9): the opposite of the previous treatment.

To obtain cHH solutions, lyophilised native cHH was diluted with PBS to a final concentration of 5 µg mL−1, using 100 µL of such solution per individual, corresponding to 0.5 µg of cHH. The amount of the cHH injected into each crayfish was set from the results of a preliminary experiment showing that the injection of 0.5 µg of cHH determined a significant increase of the glycemic level in the hemolymph. Injections were made through the dorsal abdo-cephalothoracic membrane into the pericardial sinus using a 25 gauge × 5/8′ needle fitted to a 1 mL syringe.

Treated crayfish were left isolated and undisturbed for 30 min. Differently from serotonin and octopamine [13] [19], crayfish did not exhibit any rigid posture immediately after cHH injections.

Phase 4: Fighting bouts after injections (T1, T2, T3).

The original pairs were reconstituted and observed following the same procedures as T0. After 10-min acclimatization, the divider was removed and crayfish behaviour was video-recorded for three fighting bouts in sequence of 20 min each (T1, T2, T3). The experiment was timed to record the possible behavioural alterations due to cHH injections as a consequence of a major glucose release expected to occur in T2. In fact, from the literature (e.g. [34]) we know that cHH determines increased glycemia about 1 h after the injection.

Videotapes were then blindly analysed by an unbiased observer (a PhD student), who was well experienced in crayfish behaviour but unaware of the experimental design and predictions. During T0 and the three fighting bouts we recorded:

  1. The number and total duration (in s) of fights. A fight began when one opponent approached the other and ended when one of the two individuals ran away, backed off or tail flipped away from the other for at least 10 s at a distance longer than one body length [41]. Tail flipping away is the typical backward swimming response of crayfish. From these parameters we computed the mean duration of fight (in s);
  2. Percentage of dominance (%). The number of fights won by an individual as a percentage over the total fights battled. The winner was the individual that did not retreat or that retreated after the opponent had assumed a body down posture or remained motionless. As in familiarization, the alpha was the individual that won more than 60% of the fights battled [40];
  3. Fight intensity (measured as the mode of the totalized scores). To each fight, classified as avoidance, threat, week and strong physical interactions, and unrestrained fights (as modified from [4]), a score was assigned from 1 (avoidance) to 5 (unrestrained fight);
  4. The fights started by alphas;
  5. The time spent motionless (in s).

Phase 5: Hemolymph sampling and determination of the final glycemia.

To compare variations of glycemia among treatments, immediately after the end of the experiment about 50 µL of hemolymph was drawn from each crayfish from 1300 to 1400 h, as described above.

Statistical Analyses

Data were first checked for normality and homogeneity of variance using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Levene tests, respectively. All data met the assumptions for the parametric tests and were thus analysed accordingly. To test the effect of cHH injections on glycemia, we first computed the difference between glycemic levels after and before the injection, and then we applied a one-way ANOVA (statistic: F), in which the three treatments (CP, RP, and IP) and the two ranks (alpha and beta) were the between-subject factors and the difference in glycemic levels was the variable. The difference among treatments was explored by Tukey post hoc tests; in the case of significance, a comparison between alphas and betas was made by independent samples Student’s t test (statistic: t). To analyse the effect of cHH injections on the agonistic behaviour, we applied General Linear Models for repeated measures (GLMs, statistic: F), followed by Tukey post hoc tests, where the three treatments (CP, RP, and IP) were between-subject factors and the four fighting bouts (T0, T1, T2, T3) were within-subject factors [42]. If the difference among bouts was significant, we applied one-way ANOVAs (statistic: F) to determine which treatments differed significantly. The level of significance is α = 0.05. The text reports means ± SE.

Ethical Note

The experiments comply with the current laws of Italy, the country in which they were done. No specific permits were required for the described field studies that did not involve endangered or protected species. The collection of animals did not affect the population density. Individuals were maintained in appropriate laboratory conditions to guarantee their welfare and responsiveness. After the experiments were completed, crayfish were killed by hypothermia because law forbids the release of invasive species into natural water bodies (L.R. 7/2005).

Results

Effect of Native cHH on Glycemia (Fig. 3)

As expected, the injection of cHH significantly increased glycemic levels in the crayfish hemolymph (F = 32.874, df = 2,52, P = 0.0001), independently of the hierarchical status of treated individuals (F = 0.0001, df = 1,52, P = 0.996). In fact, after cHH injections, glycemia significantly increased in a similar way (t = 0.57, df = 16, P = 0.995) in both the alphas of RP (t = −10.320, df = 16, P = 0.0001) and the betas of IP (t = 7.668, df = 16, P = 0.0001). Glycemic levels also increased in CP (in alphas: 17.6±6.3 mg dL−1, in betas: 16.6±3.2 mg dL−1) in response to fighting, but the recorded increment in the crayfish treated with cHH was about 10 times higher (alpha in RP: 178.6±20.4 mg dL−1; beta in IP: 177.1±15.0 mg dL−1).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Difference in glycemic levels after (1300–1400 h) and before (0800–0900 h) cHH injections (Mean+SE) in Control Pairs (CP), Reinforced Pairs (RP), and Inverted Pairs (IP), and in alphas (white bars) and betas (black bars).

Three asterisks denote significant differences at P<0.001 after Student's t-tests.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050047.g003

Effect of Native cHH on Agonistic Behaviour and Dominance

The total duration (F = 11.414, df = 3,69, P = 0.0001) and the number (F = 7.061, df = 3,69, P = 0.0001) of fights tended to decrease from T1 to T3 without any difference among treatments (F = 0.356, df = 2,23, P = 0.704 and F = 9.598, df = 6, 69, P = 0.748, respectively). As a consequence, the mean duration of fights was progressively shorter (F = 3.166, df = 3,69, P = 0.032) even if, immediately after the cHH injection, RP pairs combated longer than CP (F = 3.982, df = 2,25, P = 0.033; IP = CP<RP) (Fig. 4a). Alphas increased dominance across fighting bouts (F = 7.817, df = 3,69, P = 0.0001) independently of the treatment (F = 5.111, df = 6,69, P = 0.0001). The increase in the agonistic behaviour of betas immediately after the cHH injection reduced the dominance of alphas in IP (F = 7.058, df = 2,69, P = 0.004; IP<RP = CP), leading to a temporary reversal of hierarchy in T1 (Fig. 4b). Crustacean HH injections also affected the intensity of fights (F = 3.536, df = 2,23, P = 0.046). In particular, beginning from T2, IP pairs interacted stronger than the other pairs (F = 4.281, df = 2,25, P = 0.026; CP = RP<IP), with fight intensity remaining high until the end of the experiment (Fig. 4c). More often, fights were started by the alphas (F = 5.765, df = 3,69, P = 0.001), although a difference was found with treatments (F = 3.571, df = 2,23, P = 0.045). While no difference was found between CP and RP, in IP the number of fights that the initial alpha started significantly decreased in T1 (F = 5.371, df = 2,25, P = 0.012; IP<CP = RP), as a consequence of cHH injections on betas. Alphas started a lower number of fights than betas also in T2 (F = 3.285, df = 2,25, P = 0.056) but this number tended to increase progressively through the experiment (F = 1.939, df = 2,25, P = 0.167) (Fig. 4d). Overall the time spent motionless increased with time (F = 2.992, df = 3,92, P = 0.035), with betas remaining motionless for longer than alphas (t = −2.355, df = 31, P = 0.025). The treatment had also an effect on activity (F = 3.594, df = 2,92, P = 0.031): cHH injections induced a more intense locomotion, so treated betas in IP spent the same time motionless as alphas (t = −0.355, df = 35, P = 0.725), whereas the different activity between treated alphas in RP and the respective beta increased (t = −4.797, df = 35, P = 0.0001) (Fig. 5).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Behavioural parameters across fighting bouts (T0, T1, T2, T3) in Control Pairs (CP), Reinforced Pairs (RP), and Inverted Pairs (IP).

Before T0, the initial glycemia was determined; during T0, alpha and beta crayfish were assessed; between T0 and T1, crayfish were subject to the injection of either PBS solution (both alphas and betas in CP, alphas in IP, and betas in RP) or cHH solution (betas in IP and alphas in RP); from T1 to T3, crayfish behaviour was recorded and, then, the final glycemia was determined. Means (± SE) of: (a) duration of fights; (b) percentage of dominance; (c) fight intensity level; (d) number of fights started by alphas. One and two asterisks denote significant difference at P<0.05 and P<0.01, respectively, after one-way ANOVAs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050047.g004

thumbnail
Figure 5. Mean time spent motionless (+SE) in Control Pairs (CP), Reinforced Pairs (RP) and Inverted Pairs (IP), and in alphas (white bars) and betas (black bars).

One and three asterisks denote significant differences at P<0.05 and P<0.001, respectively, after Student’s t-tests.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050047.g005

Discussion

Our study analysed the effects of cHH on the agonistic behaviour of crayfish and demonstrated, for the first time, its role in enhancing it, up to reverse, although transitorily, the rank. The supporting evidence is: as expected, (1) in CP and RP alphas increased dominance. On the contrary, (2) in IP, betas became likely to initiate and escalate fights and, consequently, increased dominance till a temporary reversal of the hierarchy, but the original rank was re-established. (3) In comparison with control individuals, fights of treated alphas were longer and reached a higher intensity in treated betas. (4) IP betas showed reduced time spent motionless. To summarize, independently of prior social experience, cHH injections induced expression of dominance that differs in relation to the original rank of the individual. These behavioural changes were associated not only with an increased glycemia in the crayfish hemolymph, as well known in the literature [34], but also with the reduced time spent motionless. Our results here are consistent with what was previously described for exogenous serotonin on P. clarkii [19] and other crustaceans, including H. americanus [13], A. astacus [20][21], and M. quadrispina [18]. Serotonin, in fact, elicits the occurrence of dominant postures and aggression, in terms of the number of fights of high intensity executed, and determines a temporary reversal of hierarchies. However, serotonin shows a longer lasting effect than cHH: the original hierarchy in P. clarkii was reconstituted 1 h after the injection of serotonin [19] but only 30 min after the injection of cHH.

Our results show that cHH can modulate the neurons controlling the direct expression of agonistic behaviour also mobilizing the energetic stores needed for the increased fighting activity. Natural fluctuations of cHH release seem to be regulated by changes in central neuromodulation due to environmental and/or endogenous influences [43]. Several neurotransmitters and neuropeptides are involved on cHH release. Serotonin is recognized to play an important role in mediating the release of cHH [44][45]: serotonin injection is followed by a release of cHH that causes hyperglycemia [46][47] [22]. As a further confirmation of the occurrence of the serotonin-cHH-glycaemia physiological axis, immunoreactive and ultrastructural studies have demonstrated serotonergic synaptic structures on the axonal ramification of the cHH-producing cells of the X-organ of crayfish [48], P. clarkii included [49]. The involvement of the serotonin-cHH-glycemia physiological axis could explain both the mechanisms through which cHH controls agonism and the expression and timing of dominant behaviours triggered by either cHH or serotonin injections. The availability of an adequate amount of cHH by synthesising it with the correct post-translational modifications conferring a full biological activity [50] will allow further validation or rejection of this hypothesis.

Consistent with the study on the serotonin effects on P. clarkii [19], also the cHH did not lead to a permanent inversion of the dominance hierarchy. Cheating seems not to be sufficient to maintain the role of dominant in prolonged fights against stronger opponents. Intrinsic properties of crayfish other than body size, weight, chelar dimensions or circulating neuropeptides may likely determine the structure of dominance hierarchies in decapods. For instance, in the American lobster, H. americanus, the outcome of contests between size-matched individuals was predicted from hidden cues such as plasma protein level and exoskeleton calcium concentration [51]. These variables are not clearly visible to the rivals, but fighting lobsters may indirectly assess them by claw contraction forces, the resistance of the exoskeleton to pressure, and general fighting vigour [51]. Notwithstanding the neuropeptides injected, betas have neither the physical characteristics nor the experience of a dominant, and prolonged fights could result in both losing time/energy and increasing the risks of injury that eventually may lead to their death [52]. The original rank is thus quickly re-established since it allows betas to minimize the costs and risks of fighting with a superior individual. As a consequence, the relevance of both intrinsic physical characteristics and experience cannot be excluded in the dynamics of dominance hierarchies.

Undoubtedly, behavioural physiology opens new avenues for our understanding of the functioning of cHH and is expected to unravel its role in modulating invertebrate agonistic behaviour. Future researches are obviously needed to answer the exciting questions of how physiology and environment interact in regulating the neural systems underlying the formation and maintenance of social hierarchies across species.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: LA PGG FG. Performed the experiments: LA AM CG. Analyzed the data: LA. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: EF. Wrote the paper: LA.

References

  1. 1. Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology. The new Synthesis. Cambridge: The Belknap Press, Harvard University Press.
  2. 2. Gherardi F, Souty-Grosset C, Vogt G, Diéguez-Uribeondo J, Crandall KA (2010) Infraorder Astacidea Latreille, 1802 p.p.: the freshwater crayfish. In: Schram FR, von Vaupel Klein JC, editors. Treatise on Zoology, Anatomy, Taxonomy, Biology. The Crustacea, Decapoda. Leiden: Brill. 269–423.
  3. 3. Bovbjerg RV (1953) Dominance order in the crayfish Orconectes virilis (Hagen). Physiol Zool 26: 173–178.
  4. 4. Bruski C, Dunham DW (1987) The importance of vision in agonistic communication of the crayfish Orconectes rusticus: an analysis of bout dynamics. Behaviour 103: 83–107.
  5. 5. Zulandt Schneider RA, Huber R, Moore PA (2001) Individualand status recognition in the crayfish, Orconectes rusticus: the effects of urine release on fight dynamics. Behaviour 138: 137–153.
  6. 6. Bergman DA, Kozlowsky CP, McIntyre JC, Huber R, Daws AG, et al. (2003) Temporal dynamics and communication of winner-effects in the crayfish, Orconectes rusticus. Behaviour 140: 805–825.
  7. 7. Gherardi F, Pieraccini R (2004) Using information theory to assess dynamics, structure, and organization of crayfish agonistic repertoire. Behav Process 65: 163–178.
  8. 8. Herberholz J, Sen MM, Edwards DH (2003) Parallel changes in agonistic and non-agonistic behaviors during dominance hierarchy formation in crayfish. J Comp Physiol A 189: 321–325.
  9. 9. Song CK, Herberholz J, Edwards DH (2006) The effects of social experience on the behavioral response to unexpected touch in crayfish. J Exp Biol 209: 1355–1363.
  10. 10. Goessmann C, Hemerlrijk C, Huber R (2000) The formation and maintenance of crayfish hierarchies: behavioral and self-structuring properties. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48: 418–428.
  11. 11. Edwards DH, Herberholz J (2005) Crustacean models of aggression. In: Nelson RJ, editor. The biology of aggression. New York: Oxford University Press. 38–61.
  12. 12. Moore PA, Bergman DA (2005) The smell of success and failure: the role of intrinsic and extrinsic chemical signals on the social behavior of crayfish. Integr Comp Biol 45: 650–657.
  13. 13. Livingstone MS, Harris-Warrick R, Kravitz EA (1980) Serotonin and octopamine produce opposite postures in lobsters. Science 208: 76–79.
  14. 14. Harris-Warrick RM, Kravitz EA (1984) Cellular mechanisms for modulation of posture by octopamine and serotonin in the lobster. J Neurosci 4: 1976–1993.
  15. 15. Kravitz EA (2000) Serotonin and aggression: insights gained from a lobster model system and speculations on the role of amine neurons in a complex behavior. J Comp Physiol 186: 221–238.
  16. 16. Benvegut M, Clarac F (1982) Contrôle de la posture du crabe Carcinus maenas par des amines biogènes. C R Acad Sci (Paris) 295: 23–28.
  17. 17. Aggio J, Raktìn A, Maldonado AH (1996) Serotonin-induced short and long-term sensitization in the crab Chasmagnathus. Pharmacol Biochem Be 53: 441–448.
  18. 18. Antonsen BL, Paul DH (1997) Serotonin and octopamine elicit stereotypical agonistic behaviours in the squat lobsters Munida quadrispina (Anomoura, Galatheidea). J Comp Physiol A 181: 501–510.
  19. 19. Tricarico E, Gherardi F (2007) Biogenic amines influence aggressiveness in crayfish but not their force or hierarchical rank. Anim Behav 74: 1715–1724.
  20. 20. Huber R, Smith K, Delago A, Isaksson K, Kravitz EA (1997) Serotonin and aggressive motivation in crustaceans: altering the decision to retreat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94: 5939–5942.
  21. 21. Huber R, Delago A (1998) Serotonin alters decisions to withdraw in fighting crayfish, Astacus astacus: the motivational concept revisited. J Comp Physiol 182: 573–583.
  22. 22. Lorenzon S, Brezovec S, Ferrero EA (2004) Species-specific effects on hemolymph glucose control by serotonin, dopamine, and L-enkephalin and their inhibitors in Squilla mantis and Astacus leptodactylus (crustacea). J Exp Zoolog A Comp Exp Biol 301: 727–736.
  23. 23. Sedlmeier D, Dieberg G (1982) Action of the crustacean hyperglycemic hormone (CHH) on adenylate-cyclase and phosphodiesterase in crayfish abdominal muscle. Gen Comp Endocrinol 46: 384–385.
  24. 24. Bocking D, Dircksen H, Keller R (2001) The crustacean neuropeptides of the CHH/MIH/GIH family: structures and biological activities. In: Wiese K, editor. The crustacean nervous system. Berlin: Springer. 84–97.
  25. 25. Giulianini PG, Edomi P (2006) Neuropeptides controlling reproduction and growth in Crustacea: a molecular approach. In: Satake H, editor. Invertebrate Neuropeptides and Hormones: Basic Knowledge and Recent Advances. Research Signpost. 225–252.
  26. 26. De Kleijn DPV, Janssen KP, Waddy SL, Hegeman R, Lai WY, et al. (1998) Expression of the crustacean hyperglycaemic hormones and the gonad-inhibiting hormone during the reproductive cycle of the female American lobster Homarus americanus. J Endocrinol 156: 291–298.
  27. 27. De Kleijn DPV, van Herp F (1998) Involvement of the hyperglycemic neurohormone family in the control of reproduction in decapod crustaceans. Invert Reprod Develop 33: 263–272.
  28. 28. Chung JS, Dircksen H, Webster SG (1999) A remarkable, precisely timed release of hyperglycemic hormone from endocrine cells in the gut is associated with ecdysis in the crab Carcinus maenas. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 13103–13107.
  29. 29. Webster SG, Dircksen H, Chung JS (2000) Endocrine cells in the gut of the shore crab Carcinus maenas immunoreactive to crustacean hyperglycemic hormone and its precursor-related peptide. Cell Tissue Res 300: 193–205.
  30. 30. Santos EA, Nery LE, Keller R, Goncalves AA (1997) Evidence for the involvement of the crustacean hyperglycemic hormone in the regulation of the lipid metabolism. Physiol Zool 70: 415–420.
  31. 31. Chang ES, Chang SA, Keller R, Reddy PS, Snyder MJ, et al. (1999) Quantification of stress in lobsters: crustacean hyperglycemic hormone, stress proteins, and gene expression. Am Zool 39: 487–495.
  32. 32. Lorenzon S, Francese M, Ferrero EA (2000) Heavy metal toxicity and differential effects on the hyperglycemic stress response in the shrimp Palaemon elegans. Arch Environ Contam Toxicol 39: 167–176.
  33. 33. Lorenzon S, Edomi P, Giulianini PG, Mettulio R, Ferrero EA (2004) Variation of crustacean hyperglycemic hormone (cHH) level in the eyestalk and hemolymph of the shrimp Palaemon elegans following stress. J Exp Biol 207: 4205–4213.
  34. 34. Webster SG, Keller R, Dircksen H (2012) The CHH-superfamily of multifunctional peptide hormones controlling crustacean metabolism, osmoregulation, moulting, and reproduction. Gen Comp Endocr 175: 217–233.
  35. 35. Hemsworth R, Villareal W, Patullo BW, Macmillan DL (2007) Crustacean social behavioral changes in response to isolation. Biol Bull 213: 187–195.
  36. 36. Bergman DA, Moore PA (2005) Prolonged exposure to social odours alters subsequent social interaction in crayfish (Orconectes rusticus). Anim Behav 70: 311–318.
  37. 37. Yasuda A, YasudaY, Fujita T, Naya Y (1994) Characterization of crustacean hyperglycemic hormone from the crayfish (Procambarus clarkii): multiplicity of molecular forms by stereoinversion and diverse functions. Gen Comp Endocr 95: 387–398.
  38. 38. Gasteiger E, Hoogland C, Gattiker A, Duvaud S, Wilkins MR, et al.. (2005) Protein identification and analysis tools on the ExPASy server. In: Walker JM, editor. The proteomics protocols handbook. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press. 571–607.
  39. 39. Kallen JL, Abrahamse SL, Van Herp F (1990) Circadian rhythmicity of the crustacean hyperglycemic hormone (CHH) in the hemolymph of the crayfish. Biol Bull 179: 351–357.
  40. 40. Aquiloni L, Buřič M, Gherardi F (2008) Crayfish females eavesdrop on fighting males before choosing the dominant mate. Curr Biol 18: 462–463.
  41. 41. Gherardi F, Daniels WH (2003) Dominance hierarchies and status recognition in the crayfish Procambarus acutus acutus. Can J Zool 81: 1269–1281.
  42. 42. Quinn DGP, Keough MJ (2002) Experimental design and data analysis for biologists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 556 p.
  43. 43. Fanjul-Moles ML (2006) Biochemical and functional aspects of crustacean hyperglycemic hormone in decapod crustaceans: Review and update. Comp Biochem Phys C 142: 390–400.
  44. 44. Lee CY, Yang PF, Zou HS (2001) Serotonergic regulation of crustacean hyperglycemic hormone secretion in the crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Physiol Biochem Zool 74: 376–382.
  45. 45. Escamilla-Chimal EG, Hiriart M, Sanchez-Soto MC, Fanjul-Moles ML (2002) Serotonin modulation of CHH secretion by isolated cells of the crayfish retina and optic lobe. Gen Comp Endocrinol 125: 283–290.
  46. 46. Bauchau AG, Mengeot JC (1966) Serotonine et glycemie chez les crustaces. Experientia 22: 38–239.
  47. 47. Strolenberg GECM, Van Herp F (1977) Mise en èvidence du phénomène d′ exocytose dans la glande du sinus d′ Astacus leptodactylus (Nordmann) sous I′ influence d′injections de sérotonine. C R Acad Sci (Paris) D 284: 57–60.
  48. 48. Van Herp F, Kallen JL (1991) Neuropeptides and neurotransmitters in the × organ sinus gland complex: an important neuroendocrine integration center in the eyestalk of Crustacea. In: Stefano GB, Florey E, editors. Comparative Aspects of Neuropeptide Functions. Manchester: Manchester University Press. 211–221.
  49. 49. Escamilla-Chimal EG, Van Herp F, Fanjul-Moles ML (2001) Daily variations in crustacean hyperglycaemic hormone and serotonin immunoreactivity during the development of crayfish. J Exp Biol 204: 1073–1081.
  50. 50. Mosco A, Zlatev V, Guarnaccia C, Pongor S, Campanella A, et al. (2012) Novel Protocol for the Chemical Synthesis of Crustacean Hyperglycemic Hormone Analogues. An Efficient Experimental Tool for Studying Their Functions. PLoS One 7: e30052–e30052.
  51. 51. Vye C, Cobb JS, Bradleyb T, Gabbay J, Genizid A, et al. (1997) Predicting the winning or losing of symmetrical contests in the American lobster Homarus americanus (Milne- Edwards). J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 217: 19–29.
  52. 52. Hemelrijk CK (1997) Cooperation without genes, games or cognition. In: Husbands P, Harvey I, editors. Fourth European conference on artificial life. Cambridge: MIT Press. 511–520.