Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Does Pet Arrival Trigger Prosocial Behaviors in Individuals with Autism?

Abstract

Alteration of social interactions especially prosocial behaviors – an important aspect of development – is one of the characteristics of autistic disorders. Numerous strategies or therapies are used to improve communication skills or at least to reduce social impairments. Animal-assisted therapies are used widely but their relevant benefits have never been scientifically evaluated. In the present study, we evaluated the association between the presence or the arrival of pets in families with an individual with autism and the changes in his or her prosocial behaviors. Of 260 individuals with autism - on the basis of presence or absence of pets - two groups of 12 individuals and two groups of 8 individuals were assigned to: study 1 (pet arrival after age of 5 versus no pet) and study 2 (pet versus no pet), respectively. Evaluation of social impairment was assessed at two time periods using the 36-items ADI-R algorithm and a parental questionnaire about their child-pet relationships. The results showed that 2 of the 36 items changed positively between the age of 4 to 5 (t0) and time of assessment (t1) in the pet arrival group (study 1): “offering to share” and “offering comfort”. Interestingly, these two items reflect prosocial behaviors. There seemed to be no significant changes in any item for the three other groups. The interactions between individuals with autism and their pets were more – qualitatively and quantitatively - reported in the situation of pet arrival than pet presence since birth. These findings open further lines of research on the impact of pet’s presence or arrival in families with an individual with autism. Given the potential ability of individuals with autism to develop prosocial behaviors, related studies are needed to better understand the mechanisms involved in the development of such child-pet relationship.

Introduction

Impairments of social development associated with communication deficits, restricted interests and repetitive behaviors constitute the triad of autistic disorders [1], [2]. Individuals with autism have difficulty interacting with others as well as using and interpreting nonverbal communication. Social impairments have been regarded as primary deficits by several authors [3], [4] since they are among the first symptoms of autistic disorders (e.g. difficulty in participating in imitative or pretend play [5], [6]). Individuals with autism appear to have problems recognizing, understanding and expressing both feelings and intentions, which may be due to a lack of “theory of mind” [7]. These individuals fail to infer mental states and display impairment of abilities to understand and manage emotions (i.e. understand the other's feelings and display appropriate behavior or response [4], [8]).

Many strategies, supports or therapies have been aimed at improving the everyday lives and social interactions of individuals with autism [9], [10] For example, peer-mediated interventions have proved to be useful through increasing the communicative interactions and stimulating the development of joint attention [11]. Complementary and alternative interventions are also proposed: relaxation, music or activities with animals [12]. Indeed, since early findings by Levinson’s reporting that a dog could help in therapy [13], animal assisted therapies (AAT) have been used largely. Sessions with dogs, horses or dolphins are proposed, and considered overall as beneficial to improve prosocial behaviors [14][17]. However, to date, there is no scientific evaluation of their relevant benefit [18], [19]. Moreover, the context in which AAT occur must be accounted for. The impact of having a pet in a therapeutic or home setting seems to be different when encountering humans [20].

More broadly, beneficial effects of having a pet at home have been reported for improvement of health or well-being of elderly, isolated women, adults and children [21][26]. It is considered as a source of non judgmental and positive affection [27], [28]. Several studies suggest that children learn prosocial behaviors through their interactions with pets [29][31]. These prosocial behaviors constitute an important aspect of a child’s development. They are triggered by pet’s presence under certain circumstances (e.g. if a strong bond is formed, if the pet lives at home or if the human partner is younger than 6 years old [32][34]). Thus, bonding with a pet may help with developing some prosocial behaviors. This hypothesis seems to be consistent with the results of other studies about the reciprocal behavior that leads an animal to exceptional learning (e.g. Alex the parrot [35], Hoover the seal [36], Kanzi the chimpanzee [37]).

In the present study, we hypothesized that a pet at home might help individuals with autism to develop some prosocial behaviors. For this, we compared three situations: never owned a pet, owned a pet since birth (i.e. pet has been part of the individual’s environment) or owned a pet after the age of 5. The age of 4 to 5 is considered as a “key age” in autistic disorders [38] because it seems to be representative of the period when the severity of autism is the most important. Indeed, older subjects might outgrow some of the major impairments. Accordingly, there is a need to avoid focusing on the basis of behavior in childhood. Consequently, the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) explains that the most satisfactory compromise is to consider the age of 4 to 5 as the key age to evaluate the individual’s behavior.

The arrival of a pet in a family has been shown to increase the level of interactions between family members: they spend more time together and share joint attention on the new family member [39]. The new arrival of a pet potentially elicits more attention in individuals with autism thus leading to a greater chance of bonding with the pet. We further hypothesized that the arrival of a pet when the human partner was old enough to “realize this change” would increase the chances of improving the human’s prosocial behaviors. For this, we evaluated the individual’s impairments using the ADI-R [38], to compare two time periods (i.e. t0 at the age of 4 to 5 and t1 at the time of assessment), and a parental questionnaire about the child-pet relationship. Since direct questioning of individuals with autism can be complicated, we only used parental reports in this study. According to the literature, individuals with autism display delays and deficits in the acquisition of language (e.g. complete absence of functional communication, impairments in conversation) [40], [41]. Parents are a reliable source of information in regard to the evaluation of their child’s developmental problems [42], [43]. For example, in a previous study, Siegel et al. [44] found that parental reports about typical daily behaviors of their children with autism confirmed observations made during diagnostic play sessions by trained professionals. In addition, parental reports concerning both their pets and their child's behaviors are more reliable than children's interviews [45].

Methods

Participants

All the individuals with autism (n = 260; 59♀/191♂; mean age, 15±7.5 years old, range from 6 to 34 years old) in this study, came from the “Centre de Ressources sur l’Autisme de Bretagne” (Bohars, France) or the child day-care facilities controlled by the Bicêtre and Reims University Hospitals (France). The cognitive and behavioral assessments were approved by the ethics committee of Bicêtre hospital (the committee was not specific to this study). It is worth mentioning that the present research was non-invasive and did not involve pharmacological interventions. Hence, in accordance to the ethics committee, parents (or guardians) gave a simple verbal consent. All individuals met DSM-IV criteria for autistic disorders [1]. As part of a routine follow-up of individuals with autism, the same psychiatrists did the diagnosis and the ADI-R [38] assessment to confirm the diagnosis.

Cognitive and Behavioral Assessments

The cognitive functioning of individuals with autism from child day-care facilities of the University Hospitals of Bicêtre and Reims (n = 70) was assessed by two psychologists using the age-appropriate Weschler intelligence scale and the Kaufman K-ABC [46]. All assessed individuals with autism were cognitively impaired (mean full scale IQ ± S.D: 42.1±3.4, with a range of 40–58; mean verbal IQ ± S.D: 45.2±2.3, with a range of 45–57; mean performance IQ ± S.D: 45.2±4.4, with a range of 45–80).

ADI-R was used to assess the behavior of 260 participants with autism [38]. ADI-R, an extensive, semi-structured parental interview, was conducted by trained psychiatrists (EL, ST). The structuring lies in the details of the predetermined codings for each behavioral item. The interview schedule specifies a variety of screening questions, the purpose of which is to guide the interviewer on the content of the response (yes or no responses from the informant, i.e. parents or guardians, were inadequate). Behavioral descriptions are coded. The codings have been devised with the aim of differentiating developmental delay from deviance. Thus, for each section of the interview, there is an initial compulsory probe printing. The interviewer should then continue to ask further questions until he/she is able to make the coding for each item, for example, using different supplementary probes proposed in the ADI-R. The ADI-R scale assessed the three major domains of autistic impairments: (1) reciprocal social interactions, (2) verbal and non-verbal communication and (3) stereotyped behavior and restricted interests. The presence of verbal language is defined as daily, functional and comprehensible use of spontaneous phrases of at least three words, including at least sometimes, a verb [38].

The ADI-R algorithm is validated to assess the behavior and is based on the 4-to-5-year-old period of life. To reveal possible variations, we compared the ratings at the current period (t1) of the subset of ADI-R to those at the age of 4 to 5 (t0) [47]. The severity of behavioral impairments was scored using the subset of ADI-R items included in the ADI-R algorithm, following the procedure previously described [48]. We give below the mean score for each main domain: (1) total reciprocal social interaction (15 items), (2) total verbal communication and total non-verbal communication (13 items for non verbal patients, the score was based on 9 items), (3) total stereotypies (8 items). A score for the combined domain (social/communication/stereotypies) was calculated and regarded as a global score of autism severity (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic and behavioral characteristics of study groups (G0A and G0B never owned pet; Galw always owned a pet; Gpet didn’t own a pet before the age of 5, but owned at least one at the time of assessment).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041739.t001

Based on direct clinical observation for each participant by an independent psychiatrist, a diagnosis of autistic disorder was made according to DSM-IV [1] and ICD-10 [49] criteria and was confirmed by the ADI-R ratings. We didn’t perform an Autistic Diagnostic Observation Schedule [50] assessment. It has not been a routine practice in France before 2008 [51].

Questionnaires on Human-pet Relationships

Parents were interviewed by phone by one of the investigators (MG) not involved in the ADI-R scoring (i.e. was not aware of the data values). They were asked to answer a short standardised questionnaire about the child-pet relationship. No further information was given before the beginning of the questionnaire. Verbal informed consent was given by the parents (or guardians) when the questionnaire on human-pet relationships was filled in. The consent form explained that the questionnaire and ADI-R data will be used together. ADI-R evaluation was performed by the psychiatrist who was not aware of our project. Therefore, neither parents nor evaluators were influenced by the potential expectations of the pet’s impact. The interval between the ADI-R assessment and the questionnaire phase was less than one year. The data from parental questionnaire were collected between winter 2006 and winter 2007.

The questionnaire was about the presence (or absence) of pets in the family at t0 (i.e. at the individual’s age of 4 to 5) and at t1 (i.e. at the time of ADI-R assessment). If one or more pets were present, parents gave information on the species and the pet ownership duration, as well as their child-pet relationship. The following data were gathered (yes or no answers): tactile interactions, visual interactions, play, care (e.g. feeding, walking with the pet, brushing the pet), time spent with and any privileged relationship. The above data helped us to evaluate the individual-pet bond. Moreover, parents specified whether the pet was specially acquired for their child with autism. Pets were dogs, cats and/or little furry animals. Half of the pets were acquired for the individuals with autism.

Study 1: Arrival of a Pet between the Age of 4 to 5 and the Time of ADI-R Assessment

From the initial pool of 260 participants, we selected two groups. The first group, Gpet, did not own a pet before t0 but owned at least one afterwards (n = 12; pets were dogs, cats and one hamster). The Gpet individuals were matched with control individuals – who never owned a pet (G0A, n = 12) - for sex, age, overall level of language (absence/presence of verbal language as defined by ADI-R criteria in the following section) and history of epilepsy (Table 1; all chi-square tests and Mann Whitney U-tests p>0.05). Both the total score and the sub-scores of the ADI-R were not significantly different (all Mann-Whitney U-tests, p>0.05; Table 1). The Gpet and G0A mean age was 10.8±2.3 years old at t1. On the average, we obtained the Gpet parents responses to the questionnaire 79±29 months after the pet's arrival.

Study 2: Owned a Pet since Birth

We investigated whether the arrival (or presence) per se of pets was associated with changes in any of the ADI-R social items. We selected two groups from the initial pool of 260 participants. The first group, Galw, owned at least one pet at home since birth (n = 8; pets were dogs, cats and one rabbit). Among the Galw individuals, three owned two pets. These Galw individuals were matched with control individuals - who never owned a pet (G0B, n = 8) - for the same individual’s characteristics as in study 1 (all chi-square and Mann Whitney U-tests p>0.05; Table 1). Both the total score and the sub-scores of the ADI-R were not significantly different (all Mann-Whitney U-tests, p>0.05; Table 1). The Galw and G0B mean age was 11.1±1.9 years old at t1.

Statistical Analyses

Changes between item scores at t0 and at t1 in each group (Gpet, Galw, G0A and G0B) were evaluated using Wilcoxon’s matched-pairs signed rank test. When a significant effect was observed, Mann-Whitney test was then applied to evaluate whether or not the change could be associated with the following variables:

  1. • individual’s gender
  2. • reasons for obtaining the pet(s)
  3. • presence of different pets
  4. • type of human-pet interactions (including privileged relationship)
  5. • life setting (i.e. urban or rural)

Spearman’s rank order correlation assessed the correlation between the individual’s age or IQ score and his or her ADI-R item score. Since 36 tests were performed at both t0 and t1, in order to avoid false positive due to chance, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparison was applied systematically (p<0.0014).

Results

Study 1

Comparison of ADI-R assessment between t0 and t1 revealed significant changes in two of the 36 items in the Gpet. Thus, Gpet had a lower deficit score for the items “offering to share”, e.g. sharing food or toys with parents or other children (Wilcoxon test: ZGpet = 21 p<0.0014; Fig. 1) and “offering comfort”, e.g. reassuring parents or peers who were sad or hurt (Wilcoxon test: ZGpet = 21 p<0.0014; Fig. 2). No changes were observed for the control individuals (Wilcoxon tests: ZG0A = 3, ZG0A = 6 p>0.05 in both cases; Fig. 1, Fig. 2). In Gpet and G0A, neither the total scores of ADI-R at t0 and t1 (Wilcoxon tests: ZGpet = 4 p = 0.011; ZG0A = 15 p = 0.065) nor the sub-scores in the main domains (all Wilcoxon tests: p>0.0014) were statistically different at p<0.0014.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Item scores of “offering to share” at t0 (4-to-5-years old; in grey) and t1 (current period; mean age: 129.9±55.8 months old; in black) for G0A (group with no pet in the family), Gpet (group with a pet arriving after the child’s 5th birthday), G0B (group with no pet in the family) and Galw (group always with at least one pet at home since birth).

Higher the score, more significant was the “offering to share” (e.g. sharing food or toys with parents or other children).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041739.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Item scores of “offering comfort” at t0 (4-to-5-years old; in grey) and t1 (current period, mean age: 129.9±55.8 months old; in black) for G0A (group with no pet in the family), Gpet (group with a pet arriving after the child’s 5th birthday), G0B (group with no pet in the family) and Galw (group always with at least one pet at home since birth).

Higher the score, more significant was the impairment “offering comfort” (e.g. reassuring parents or peers who were sad or hurt). Comparisons were performed using Wilcoxon’s matched-pairs signed ranks tests (Significant threshold: p<0.0014).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041739.g002

Score differences between t0 and t1 were neither correlated with individual’s age (all Spearman’s rank order correlation p>0.05) nor affected by gender, life setting, presence of different pets, and type of human-pet interaction (all Mann Whitney U-tests p>0.05). Interestingly, whether the parents had acquired the animal for their child or for the family revealed no significant difference in ADI-R scores (Mann Whitney U-test = 53.5 p>0.05), indicating that the results were not influenced by the parents expectations on the pet’s impact. In addition, communication and non-social aspects (e.g. scores for repetitive behavior and stereotyped patterns) were not affected by the pet’s arrival (all Wilcoxon tests p>0.05). No significant correlation (Spearman’s rank order correlation, p>0.05) between the items “offering comfort” or “offering to share” and IQ scores (verbal IQ, performance IQ and full IQ) was observed.

Parental questionnaire offered some information about the interaction type Gpet individual had with his or her pet (Table 2). Tactile interactions were the most reported (i.e. 75%; n = 9), followed by time spent with the pet (n = 8), play (n = 7) and visual interactions (n = 7). Care was the least reported item (n = 6). Thus, seven Gpet individuals were considered by their parents as having a privileged relationship with their pets. Among the five remaining individuals, three owned a cat and two owned a dog.

thumbnail
Table 2. Number of individuals with autism who display different types of relationships with their pet according to parents.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041739.t002

Study 2

No significant change was observed for individuals with autism who owned a pet since birth or for control individuals (Galw and GOB; all Wilcoxon test p>0.05; Fig. 1, Fig. 2). In Galw and GOB, neither the total scores of ADI-R at t0 and t1 (Wilcoxon tests: ZGalw = 9 p = 0.447; ZG0B = 11 p = 0.363) nor the sub-scores in the four domains (all Wilcoxon tests: p>0.05) were statistically different at p<0.0014.

Here again, an exploration of the parental questionnaire offered some information about the interaction type Galw individual had with his or her pet. Few individuals were reported as interacting with their pets (Table 2). Care and play were not mentioned. Two individuals spent time with their pet, four had tactile interactions and five had visual interactions. Only three Galw individuals were considered by their parents as having privileged relationships with their pets (i.e. three dogs). However, two of the three individuals who owned the same pet since birth, neither interacted nor bonded with it (i.e. all items were reported as absent).

Discussion

Comparison of ADI-R assessment between Gpet and Galw at two different time periods revealed significant changes in ADI-R scores only in the group experiencing the pet arrival in their homes. However, these changes were limited to two ADI-R items, “offering to share” and “offering comfort”. These findings suggest an improvement in prosocial behaviors of the individuals with autism. These prosocial behaviors are mainly impaired in individuals with autism [1], [52]. The absence of a significant correlation with IQ scores might imply that these changes were not related to the level of cognitive functioning. Interestingly, the individual-pet interactions (i.e. bonding) were more - qualitatively and quantitatively - reported in the case of pet arrival than pet presence since birth. To our knowledge, this is the first study showing an association between pet arrival and changes in prosocial behaviors. Our study follows the footsteps of the human-pet reports on the improvement of prosocial behaviors in individuals with typical development [53], [54].

On the Significance of Changes

On the one hand, two main possible explanations could account for these findings.

First, parents may have acquired a pet because they believed that it would improve the prosocial behaviors of their children with autism. In this case, their responses to the ADI-R could be biased. The following findings strongly suggest that this was not the case:

  1. Only 6 pets (of the 15 pets in Gpet) were acquired especially for the individuals with autism; the others were acquired for another family member. Changes in the prosocial behaviors were observed in both cases. Thus, these changes were not related to parental expectations.
  2. This “pet study” (and its related questionnaire) began after the ADI-R completion. This suggests that the parents were not aware of the possible pet impact at t1.
  3. Improvement was found only for two of the 36 items, further indicating the non-bias character of parent's responses.

The second explanation is that the arrival of a pet may have triggered a change in the individuals’ “perception of the social world”. Pets are supposed to enhance different skills in children with typical development such as self-esteem, socio-emotional development and empathy [32], [33], [54]. According to several authors, children with typical development seem to learn prosocial behaviors through their interactions with pets (e.g. sharing with and stroking the pet) [30], [55]. Could this also be the case for individuals with autism? Only observational studies can reveal how individuals with autism interact with their pet and whether somehow they develop skills to understand pet’s behaviors or needs [56].

On the other hand, it is not very surprising that other ADI-R items did not change, not even those related to the prosocial behaviors. Since verbal exchanges with pets are excluded, we would expect no changes in language skills whereas parents can indeed influence such skills [57]. Moreover, other studies confirm that animals neither influence motor skills nor reduce restricted behaviors in children with autism [16].

Potential Mechanisms

Numerous theories have been proposed to explain the pet’s influence on human life (for a review, see [58]). Animals are animates, thus differ from inanimates in regard to many biological characteristics such as motion or sensory properties. Specifically, animates are beings that know, perceive, learn and think. These abilities make them appealing ([59] in [60]).

Friedman et al. [61] proposed the bio-psycho-social model that considers pets could reduce loneliness and thus could also be considered as “transitional objects” especially for the children [31], [62]. Pets may also be considered as “distracters”. Brickel [63] and more recently Odendaal [64] proposed to explain this phenomenon by the attention-shift theory. They stated that when a human is in a stressful situation, a pet seems to distract him/her from the anxiogenic stimulus (e.g. unknown situations in the case of people with autism). Animal’s presence triggers human’s attention-shift. Attention-shift offered by a pet under repeated exposure to a stressful situation, leads to a decrease in anxiety. Therefore, a family pet may also become a source and a center of attention that could be useful in individual’s learning.

On the one hand, the presence of a pet can have a direct influence. When a human and a pet are interacting, each partner uses signals emitted by the other to adjust their behavior: the behavior of one influences the response of the other (e.g. between a dog and a child [65][67]). A bond or a relationship emerges from these series of interactions where both partners have expectations on the next interaction on the basis of the previous ones [68]. Thus, as stated by Filiatre et al. [65] the pet’s behavior “could contribute to the acquisition by the child of a more structured and more socially efficient behavioral repertoire”. Moreover, the attitudes that children display towards pets have an impact on their prosocial and social behaviors [34], [69]. On the other hand, a pet can have an indirect influence on children through the family. Indeed numerous parents state that pets can be precious tools with which they educate their children [29], [70], [71]. For example, Beck et al [72] showed that an increased knowledge about wild birds after a ten-week educational home-based program for feeding was associated with parental involvement.

People with autism have been shown to be less sensitive to human voices [73] or faces [74] than to other environmental stimuli. To our knowledge, little is known about how they perceive animals' characteristics, but they are quite able to classify their animal preferences based on pictures [75]. Using a task based on sorting by preference, Celani [76] showed that children with autism chose pictures with an animal (e.g. dog, cat) rather than the ones with objects. At last, some authors explained that the affinity of people with autism for pets comes from animal’s multisensory characteristic. In addition, according to these authors, an animal’s behavior seems to be easier to decode and to predict than that of a human partner [14], [17].

Pet Presence versus Arrival

One intriguing finding was that similar results were observed for the individuals who were in the presence of a pet from birth and those who never owned a pet. Changes were only observed in the group where the pet arrived after the age of 5. Different hypotheses are possible and are explored below.

When the pet was reported to be present since the individual’s birth, one would expect a cumulative effect of its presence. We cannot exclude this effect even if the ADI-R did not clearly explore it here (e.g. neither a too low nor a specific effect was explored by ADI-R items). However, we proposed an alternative explanation. Individuals with autism may usually avoid unfamiliar social partners and display diminished interest in novelty [1]. But under certain circumstances, children with autism prefer new stimuli rather than familiar ones [77]. The presence of a pet may be a mere “additional” element of the environment, therefore not attracting special attention. This is consistent with our parental questionnaire revealing that few of these individuals (Galw) developed a real bond with the pet in comparison to the other group (i.e. Gpet, pet arrival). For example, only a quarter of Galw individuals had a privileged relationship with their pet. The sole presence of the pet did not confer benefit for the individuals with autism. Such situation was previously reported in the children with typical development : the quality of relationship with their own pet appears to be a direct determinant of their socio-emotional development [33] and “pet bonding” is a stronger determinant of pet-associated benefits than the sole pet ownership [78]. If we take a look at the other side, the pet may also have formed a preferential bond with another member of the family and therefore been less demanding on the individual with autism.

The other non-exclusive possibility is that the arrival of a pet strengthens the cohesion of the family and increases the levels of interactions between their members. Pet’s arrival plays an even more important role in the lives of children who have inadequate or destructive family and social environments [79]. Most families acquiring a pet experienced an increase in quantity and quality of time spent together and felt happier after pet’s arrival [39]. This situation might be due to the collective attention on the new pet. This new pet arrival might induce an increased interest of the individuals towards the pet and/or their involvement in the family’s interactions. Cain [39] talked about the “triangling” process initiated by the pet (i.e. structuring and promoting interactions between two humans).

In our study, playing with the pet was reported by seven of the parents in Gpet whereas only two of the parents in Galw noticed it. This behavior is a powerful means by which children master skills that are important for their development [80]. Playing with a pet is a complex behavior, sometimes involving object manipulation as a means for practice and mastery of action schemas (i.e. sensorimotor play) or child’s ability for mental representation. Thus, it provides a child with means of practicing and understanding the events of his or her social world (i.e. pretend play) [81]. These behaviors are not only observed in humans but also in human-pet interactions [55], [82]. Such interactions may have some positive outcome: playing with a dog during pet therapy had beneficial impact on hospitalized children [83]. This implies that playing with a pet may be beneficial to individuals with autism.

Interestingly, in our study, taking care of the pet was reported by half of the parents in Gpet whereas none of the parents in Galw noticed it. Our finding infers the positive influence of pet arrival on parental support in the development of individuals with autism. Previous studies have shown that parents use pets to teach their children how to take care of pets by giving them age-appropriate tasks [29]. With parental support, the child involvement towards a pet may influence his/her socio-emotional development [84].

Finally individuals with autism may be sensitive to an overall change in their social sphere. Therefore the changes may be related merely to the overall family functioning rather than the sole pet arrival. This however would not explain why only two precise items were affected and not the others.

Conclusion

This study reveals that in individuals with autism, pet arrival in the family setting may bring about changes in specific aspects of their socio-emotional development. It suggests the improvement of some prosocial behaviors in such individuals under certain circumstances. Thus, it offers a “window of opportunity” to future longitudinal developmental studies to further confirm these findings and explain their underlying mechanisms. Given the current state of knowledge, we suggest further research exploring our hypothesis on the association between the arrival of a new pet and the change in a family dynamic to evaluate the impact of another child’s arrival.

Our study has limitations that need to be noted. Both our study design and its lack of power (40 individuals from an initial cohort of 260 participants) didn’t allow us to clarify the exact role of pets in the families who already owned pets. Nevertheless these first results open interesting lines of research exploring the efficacy of animals employed in AAT settings. Further studies with larger sample sizes (e.g. including more control groups) are needed to clarify the exact role of pets in this context.

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Dr. Ann Cloarec, researcher, Ethos laboratory and Zarrin Alavi, (for her pertinent advice) medical writer and translator, Brest University Hospital, Department of Internal Medicine and Chest Diseases; INSERM CIC 0502, to Pr Michel Botbol, CHRU Brest, to families for their participation, to the Fondation Sommer for their support and the French GIS CCS (Groupe d'Intérêt Scientifique - Comportement Cerveau et Société)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MG MH AL ST MD EL. Performed the experiments: MG MH EL. Analyzed the data: MG MH EL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MG MH EL. Wrote the paper: MG MH ST EL.

References

  1. 1. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington.
  2. 2. Wing L, Gould J (1979) Severe impairements of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 9: 11–29.
  3. 3. Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press. 197 p.
  4. 4. Mundy P (1995) Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology 7: 63–82.
  5. 5. Osterling JA, Dawson G, Munson JA (2002) Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology 14: 239–251.
  6. 6. Baranek GT (1999) Autism during infancy: A retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29: 213–224.
  7. 7. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1985) Does the autistic-child have a theory of mind. Cognition 21: 37–46.
  8. 8. Kasari C, Sigman M, Mundy P, Yirmiya N (1990) Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally-retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20: 87–100.
  9. 9. McConnell SR (2002) Interventions to facilitate social interaction for young children with autism: Review of available research and recommendations for educational intervention and future research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 32: 351–372.
  10. 10. Wong HHL, Smith RG (2006) Patterns of complementary and alternative medical therapy use in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36: 901–909.
  11. 11. Pierce K, Schreibman L (1997) Multiple peer use of pivotal response training to increase social behaviors of classmates with autism: Results from trained and untrained peers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 30: 157–160.
  12. 12. Gasalberti D (2006) Alternative Therapies for Children and Youth With Special Health Care Needs. Journal of Pediatric Health Care 20: 133–136.
  13. 13. Levinson BM (1962) The dog as a “co-therapist”. Mental Hygiene 179: 46–59.
  14. 14. Redefer LA, Goodman JF (1989) Pet-facilitated therapy with autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 19: 461–467.
  15. 15. Nathanson DE, DeFaria S (1993) Cognitive improvement of children in water with and without dolphins. Anthrozoös 65: 17–29.
  16. 16. Bass M, Duchowny C, Llabre M (2009) The Effect of Therapeutic Horseback Riding on Social Functioning in Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39: 1261–1267.
  17. 17. Martin F, Farnum J (2002) Animal-assisted therapy for children with pervasive developmental disorders. Western Journal of Nursing Research 24: 657–670.
  18. 18. Barker SB, Wolen AR (2008) The Benefits of Human-Companion Animal Interaction: A Review. West Lafayette, IN. Univ Toronto Press Inc. pp.487–495.
  19. 19. Katcher A (2000) The future of education and research on the human-animal bond and animal-assisted therapy Part B: Animal-assisted therapy and the study of human-animal relationships: Discipline or bondage? Context or transitional object? In: A F, editor. Handbook on animal-assisted therapy: Theoretical foundations for guidelines and practice. San Diego: Academic Press. pp.461–473.
  20. 20. Burrows KE, Adams CL, Spiers J (2008) Sentinels of Safety: Service Dogs Ensure Safety and Enhance Freedom and Well-Being for Families With Autistic Children. Qualitative Health Research 18: 1642–1649.
  21. 21. Anderson WP, Reid CM, Jennings GL (1992) Pet Ownership And Risk-Factors For Cardiovascular-Disease. Medical Journal of Australia 157: 298–301.
  22. 22. Friedmann E, Katcher AH, Lynch JJ, Thomas SA (1980) Animal companions and one-year survival after discharge from a coronary-care unit. Public Health Reports 95: 307–312.
  23. 23. Turner DC, Rieger G, Gygax L (2003) Spouses and cats and their effects on human mood. Anthrozoos 16: 213–228.
  24. 24. Beck AM, Meyers NM (1996) Health enhancement and companion animal ownership. Annual Review of Public Health 17: 247–257.
  25. 25. Serpell JA (1991) Beneficial-effects of pet ownership on some aspects of human health and behavior. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 84: 717–720.
  26. 26. Paul ES, Serpell JA (1996) Obtaining a new pet dog: Effects on middle childhood children and their families. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 47: 17–29.
  27. 27. Beck AM, Katcher AH (1984) A new look at pet-facilitated therapy. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 184: 414–421.
  28. 28. Bryant BK (1990) The richness of the child pet relationship: A consideration of both benefits and costs of pets to children. Anthrozoos 3: 253–261.
  29. 29. Endenburg N, Baarda B (1996) The Role of Pets in Enhancing Human Well-being: Effects on Child Development. In: Robinson I, editor. The Waltham Book of Human-Animal Interactions: Benefits and Responsibilities of Pet Ownership. pp.7–17.
  30. 30. Filiatre JC, Millot JL, Montagner H, Eckerlin A, Gagnon AC (1988) Advances in the study of the relationship between children and their pet dogs. Anthrozöos 2: 22–32.
  31. 31. George H (1988) Child therapy and animals . In: CE S, editor. Innovative interventions in child and adolescent therapy. New York: John Wiley. pp.400–418.
  32. 32. Kidd AH, Kidd RM (1985) Children’s attitudes toward their pets. Psychological Reports 57: 15–31.
  33. 33. Melson GF (1991) Children’s attachment to their pets: Links to socio-emotional development. Children’s Environments Quarterly 82: 55–65.
  34. 34. Bailey C (1988) Exposure of Preschool Children to Companion Animals: Impact on Role Taking Skills. Dissertation Abstracts International 48: 1976.
  35. 35. epperberg IM, Brezinsky MV (1991) Acquisition of a relative class concept by an african grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) - Discriminations based on relative size. Journal of Comparative Psychology 105: 286–294.
  36. 36. Ralls K, Fiorelli P, Gish S (1985) Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals, Phoca vitulina. Canadian Journal of Zoology 63: 1050–1056.
  37. 37. Savage-Rumbaugh S, Lewin R (1994) Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind: John Wiley & Sons Inc. 299 pages p.
  38. 38. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A (1994) Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 24: 659–685.
  39. 39. Cain AO (1985) Pets as family members. Marriage & Family Review 8: 5–10.
  40. 40. Tager-Flusberg H (2000) Understanding the Language and Communicative Impairments in Autism. New York: Academic Press.
  41. 41. Tager-Flusberg H, Caronna E (2007) Language disorders: Autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatric Clinics of North America 54: 469–+.
  42. 42. Glascoe FP, Foster M, Wolraich ML (1997) An economic analysis of developmental detection methods. Pediatrics 99: 830–837.
  43. 43. Glascoe FP (2005) Screening for developmental and behavioral problems. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 11: 173–179.
  44. 44. Siegel B, Anders TF, Ciaranello RD, Bienenstock B, Kraemer HC (1986) Empirically derived subclassification of the autistic syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 16: 275–293.
  45. 45. Bryant B (1986) The relevance of family and neighborhood animals to social-emotional development in middle childhood. Boston.
  46. 46. Anastasi A (1988) Psychological Testing. New York: Macmillan. 818 p.
  47. 47. Lord C, Pickles A, McLennan J, Rutter M, Bregman J, et al. (1997) Diagnosing autism: Analyses of data from the autism diagnostic interview. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 27: 501–517.
  48. 48. Tordjman S, Gutknecht L, Carlier M, Spitz E, Antoine C, et al. (2001) Role of the serotonin transporter gene in the behavioral expression of autism. Molecular Psychiatry 6: 434–439.
  49. 49. World Health Organization (1994) The composite international diagnostic interview, Version 1.1. Geneva: Researcher’s manual.
  50. 50. Lord C, Rutter M, Goode S, Heemsbergen J, Jordan H, et al. (1989) Autism diagnostic observation schedule: a standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 19: 185–212.
  51. 51. Rogé B, Fombonne E, Fremolle J, Arti E (2009) Adaptation française de l'ADOS: Echelle d'observation pour le diagnostic de l'autisme: Editions Hogrefe.
  52. 52. Travis LL, Sigman M (1998) Social deficits and interpersonal relationships in autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 4: 65–72.
  53. 53. Furman W (1989) The development of children's social networks. In: Belle D, editor. Children's Social Networks and Social Support. New-York: Wiley. 151–172.
  54. 54. Hills A (1995) Empathy and belief in the mental experience of animals. Anthrozoös 8: 132–142.
  55. 55. Melson GF (2005) Why the wild things are; animals in the lives of children: Harvard University Press. 236p.
  56. 56. Grandgeorge M (2010) Could the bond to an animal allow social and cognitive recovery in children with autism? Rennes: University Rennes 2: 357p.
  57. 57. Grandgeorge M, Hausberger M, Tordjman S, Deleau M, Lazartigues A, et al. (2009) Environmental Factors Influence Language Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. PLoS ONE 4: e4683.
  58. 58. Maurer M, Delfour F, Trudel M, Adrien JL (2011) L’enfant avec un autisme et l’animal dans un lien signifiant : des possibilités d’interventions thérapeutiques. La psychiatrie de l'enfant 54: 575–609.
  59. 59. Gelman R, Spelke ES (1981) The development of thoughts about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research on social cognition. In: JH Flavell, L Ross, editors. Social cognition. New York: Academic. pp.43–66.
  60. 60. Poulin-Dubois D, Frenkiel-Fishman S, Nayer S, Johnson S (2006) Infants’ Inductive Generalization of Bodily, Motion, and Sensory Properties to Animals and People. Journal of cognition and development 7: 431–453.
  61. 61. Friedmann E, Katcher AH, Thomas SA, Lynch JJ, Messent PR (1983) Social-Interaction And Blood-Pressure - Influence Of Animal Companions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 171: 461–465.
  62. 62. Triebenbacher SL (1998) Pets as transitional objects: Their role in children's emotional development. Psychological Reports 82: 191–200.
  63. 63. Brickel CM (1982) Pet-Facilitated Psychotherapy - A Theoretical Explanation Via Attention Shifts. Psychological Reports 50: 71–74.
  64. 64. Odendaal JSJ (2000) Animal-assisted therapy - magic or medicine? Journal of Psychosomatic Research 49: 275–280.
  65. 65. Filiatre JC, Millot JL, Montagner H (1986) New data on communication behavior between the young-child and his pet dog. Behavioural Processes 12: 33–44.
  66. 66. Millot JL, Filiatre JC (1986) The behavioral sequences in the communication-system between the child and his pet dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 16: 383–390.
  67. 67. Millot JL, Filiatre JC, Gagnon AC, Eckerlin A, Montagner H (1988) Children and their pet dogs: how they communicate. Behavioural Processes 17: 1–15.
  68. 68. Hinde R (1979) Towards Understanding Relationships. London: Academic Press.
  69. 69. Poresky RH (1996) Companion animals and others factors affecting young children's development. Anthrozoos 9: 159–168.
  70. 70. Salomon A (1981) Animals and children - The role of the pets. Canadas Mental Health 29: 9–13.
  71. 71. Macdonald A (1981) The pet dog in a home: a study of interactions. In: Fogle B, editor. Interrelations between people and pets. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C. Thomas.
  72. 72. Beck AM, Melson GF, da Costa PL, Liu T (2001) The educational benefits of a ten-week home-based wild bird feeding program for children. Anthrozoos 14: 19–28.
  73. 73. Gervais H, Belin P, Boddaert N, Leboyer M, Coez A, et al. (2004) Abnormal cortical voice processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience 7: 801–802.
  74. 74. Osterling J, Dawson G (1994) Early recognition of children with autism - a study of 1st birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24: 247–257.
  75. 75. Maurer M, Delfour F, Wolff M, Adrien JL (2010) Dogs, Cats and Horses: Their Different Representations in the Minds of Typical and Clinical Populations of Children. Anthrozoos 23: 383–395.
  76. 76. Celani G (2002) Human beings, animals and inanimate objects - What do people with autism like? Autism 6: 93–102.
  77. 77. Kenzer AL, Bishop MR (2011) Evaluating preference for familiar and novel stimuli across a large group of children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5: 819–825.
  78. 78. Poresky RH, Hendrix C (1989) Companion animal bonding, children's home environments and young children's social development. Kansas City.
  79. 79. Blue GF (1986) The value of pets in children's lives. Childhood Education 63: 84–90.
  80. 80. Bruner JS (1972) Nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist 27: 687–708.
  81. 81. Piaget J (1962) Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
  82. 82. Montagner H (1995) L'enfant, l'animal et l'école: AFIRAC. 222 p.
  83. 83. Kaminski M, Pellino T, Wish J (2002) Play and Pets: The Physical and Emotional Impact of Child-Life and Pet Therapy on Hospitalized Children. Children's health care 31: 321–335.
  84. 84. Haggerty Davis J, Gerace L, Summers J (1989) Pet-care management in child-rearing families. Anthrozoos 2: 189–193.