Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Jaw Laterality and Related Handedness in the Hunting Behavior of a Scale-Eating Characin, Exodon paradoxus

Abstract

Background

Asymmetry in animal bodies and behavior has evolved several times, but our knowledge of their linkage is limited. Tanganyikan scale-eating cichlids have well-known antisymmetry in their bodies and behavior; individuals open their mouths leftward (righty) or rightward (lefty), and righties always attack the right flank of the prey, whereas lefties attack the left. This study analyzed the morphological asymmetry in a scale-eating characiform, Exodon paradoxus, and its behavioral handedness.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Each eight E. paradoxus was observed for 1-h with a prey goldfish in an aquarium to detect the behavioral handedness. Following the experiment, the lateral differences in the mandibles and head-inclination of these eight and ten additional specimens were analyzed. Both measurements on the morphology showed a bimodal distribution, and the laterality identified by these two methods was always consistent within a given individual, indicating that the characin has morphological antisymmetry. Furthermore, this laterality significantly corresponded to behavioral handedness; that is, lefties more often rasped scales from the right flank of the prey and vice versa. However, the correlation between laterality and handedness is the opposite of that in the cichlids. This is due to differences in the feeding apparatus and technique. The characin has cuspids pointing forward on the external side of the premaxilla, and it thrusts its dominant body side outward from its body axis on the flank of the prey to tear off scales. By contrast, the cichlids draw their dominant body side inward toward the axis or rotate it to scrape or wrench off scales with the teeth lined in the opened mouth.

Conclusions/Significance

This study demonstrated that the antisymmetry in external morphology and the corresponding behavioral handedness have evolved in two lineages of scale-eating fishes independently, and these fishes adopt different utilization of their body asymmetry to tear off scales.

Introduction

Antisymmetry is a phenomenon in which a population of a species are consist of right-sided and left-sided, or dextral and sinistral, forms with even frequencies [1]. Lateral asymmetry in animal can be classified by the shape of histogram into 3 categories; fluctuating asymmetry (FA) with unimodal and symmetric distribution, directional asymmetry (DA) with unimodal distribution shifted from symmetry, and anti-symmetry (AS) with bimodal distribution. Antisymmetry in external morphology is widely known in nature and has evolved several times in more than 450 species from 67 families in eight phyla, including lobsters, crossbill birds, and scale-eating cichlids [1]. The bimodal laterality in the jaw morphology and head inclination of a scale-eating cichlid, Perissodus microlepis [2], [3], an herbivorous Tanganyikan cichlid, Neolamprologus moorii [3], a Japanese riverine goby, Rhinogobius flumineus [4], the Japanese medaka Oryzias latipes [5], the zebrafish Danio rerio [6] and a Tanganyikan cichlid, Julidochromis ornatus [5], are suggested to be genetically determined.

Behavioral asymmetry, in which every individual has either left- or right-biased behavior, is revealed in the detour behavior and eye use of fishes [7], mouth use of fishes [8], [9], and paw/hand use of toads [10], mice [11], cats [12], and chimpanzees [13]. Recent studies have revealed that some behavioral handedness is highly correlated with antisymmetry in external morphology. For example, handedness in foraging behavior is correlated with laterality in mouth opening in scale-eating cichlids [2], [14], the shrimp-eating cichlid, Neolamprologus fasciatus [9], and large-mouth bass, Micropterus salmoides [8]. Furthermore, handedness during lateral display in male–male competition is correlated with head inclination laterality in the Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens [15].

The scale-eating cichlid is one of the best-known examples of both morphological and behavioral antisymmetry. Scale eating is highly specialized foraging in which the prey is usually bigger than the predator, so the scale eater needs to adapt to the high motility and possible counterattack by its prey [16]. Furthermore, since lepidophagy does not kill the prey, prey fishes avoid predators by learning, as well as by intrinsic behavior [2], [17]. Therefore, scale-eating fish have both specialized feeding apparatus and behavior, and they have evolved descaling teeth [18], [19], multiple approaching strategies [20], [21], relevant concealing colorations [22], and even aggressive mimicry [23], [24]. Some fishes adopt a remarkable adaptation for scale-eating, that is, laterality. In all seven species of scale-eating cichlid in Lake Tanganyika, every individual opens its mouth either rightward (lefty) or leftward (righty) as a result of the asymmetric position of the joints of the mandibles and the suspensorium [18], [25], [26]. Note that the definition of laterality used in recent studies differs from the definition used in the early papers [2], [25]. These earlier papers defined individuals with the mouth opening to the right as “right-handed”, or “dextral”. The terminology used in the present study, “lefty”, reflects the fact that the left mandible of such “right-handed” fish is larger than their right mandibles [3], [9], [27] and that their left eye is the dominant eye [9], [15]. The direction in this laterality is determined genetically by a one-locus two-allele Mendelian system, with the lefty dominant over the righty [2], [3]. Furthermore, in the cichlids Perissodus microlepis and P. straeleni, under natural conditions, righty individuals always attack the right flank of their prey, and lefties attack the left flank [2], [3]. This biased attack causes the prey fishes being vigilant to one side of their body that is attacked more frequently, and therefore numerically dominant morph (either lefty or righty) of P. microlepis decreases their predation success [2]. This is a case of the negative frequency-dependent selection, and the frequency of lefty and righty morphs oscillates around unity [2]. An experimental study of the laterality of P. microlepis demonstrated some morphological and behavioral plasticity in an artificial environment [28].

Scale-eating habits are known in five freshwater and eight marine fish families (Table S1), and have evolved at least 12 times independently [16], [29]. In addition to Tanganyikan cichlids, only the scale-eating triacanthodid Macrorhamphosodes uradoi is recognized as having either a leftward- or rightward-twisted mouth [29]. Morphological investigation reveals that the scales in the stomachs of M. uradoi are stolen from the caudal fin and the base of prey fishes, and therefore, M. uradoi is supposed to attack their prey from behind. In that attacking behavior asymmetric mouth may have a function, but it remains unknown how it actually works in deep water.

Another diverse, well-studied group exhibiting lepidophagy is the freshwater characins of South America. Six genera, including Exodon, are scale eaters [16], [30]. Fish scales fill 88% of the stomach contents in E. paradoxus collected in the wild [31]. Characiformes is a relatively older teleostean group that lacks the apparatus for upper jaw premaxilla protrusion that the newer scale-eating fishes have achieved [32]. Instead, scale-eating characins have enlarged cuspidate teeth that point forward on the labial sides of the jaws [33]. E. paradoxus has a pair of cuspidate teeth on the premaxilla [34] and rushes at the flank of its prey with its mouth either open or closed [16].

This study examined the mechanism by which characin scale eaters remove scales compared with other scale-eating fishes and whether the antisymmetry in mouth morphology and related behavioral handedness are ubiquitous in scale-eating fishes. Therefore, we observed the feeding behavior of a wild-caught scale-eating characin, E. paradoxus, and examined its morphological laterality, its behavioral handedness in hunting.

Materials and Methods

Behavioral observations

This study was performed in accordance with the Regulation on Animal Experimentation at Kyoto University. Approval is not needed for fishes under Japanese law, Act on Welfare and Management of Animals. Eight wild-caught adult Exodon paradoxus [52.2±5.7-mm standard length (SL), average ± standard deviation (SD)], imported from Colombia, were obtained from a commercial vendor. E. paradoxus feed on scales with 3–5 mm in diameter [30], therefore, live goldfish, Carassius auratus auratus (63.2±5.5-mm SL), were used as prey that have the similar scale size. After starvation for 24 hours, each E. paradox individual was transferred to a 58-L aquarium with one prey individual, with the two separated by a partition. After 15 min of acclimation, the partition was removed, and foraging events were recorded for 1-h using a digital video camera (Panasonic SDR-H80). The attacked flank of the prey, which direction (anterior or posterior of the prey) E. paradoxus turned its jaws when tearing off scales, and the moment when the feeding behavior occurred during the 1-hour observation were recorded. Each E. paradoxus and goldfish pair was observed once.

Measurement of morphological asymmetries

Following the observations, the fish were sacrificed using an overdose of 2-phenoxyethanol and fixed in 10% formalin solution. Ten additional E. paradoxus (49.9±4.1-mm SL) were obtained from the same vendor and fixed in the same way.

To evaluate morphological laterality, two measurements were examined (Fig. 1): (1) the difference between the heights of the right and left posterior end of the mandible (HMPE), measured as the distance between the socket bottom of the suspensorial articulation facet of the anguloarticular and the most inferior point of the retroarticular process [3], and (2) head inclination, i.e., the angle (θ) between a line from the center of the parasphenoid to the center of the basioccipital and another line from the center of the basioccipital to the center of the third spinal segment in the ventral view [15], [27]. The right-left difference of the HMPE means the following: the HMPE acts as a line between the effort point (where the ligament is attached) and the fulcrum point (the articulation part of the mandible) of the lever [35], [36]. Thus, the difference between right and left HMPE itself may produce differential opening force and speed between right and left mandible, which necessarily cause the twisted mouth-opening. The morphological implication of the head-vertebrae angle is understood as a differential development of the right and left sides, the more developed side should be convex. Consequently the dominant side of the head faces front. Specimens were dissected to extract the mandibles and expose data points, which were marked and kept horizontal using a level-scope and then measured using a digital microscope (VHX-100, Keyence). Three measurements were made for each individual to reduce the observation error. The measurement error (ME) for our measurements were estimated as the proportion of within-individual variation to total variation, i.e., ME = MSwithin/(S2A+MSwititin)×100 (%), where MS is the mean square value and S2A is the added variance component. S2A was estimated as (MSamongMSwithin)/n from a one-way ANOVA with individuals as the fixed factor, where n is the number of repeated measures per individual, which in this study is 3 [37], [38]. The measurement errors were small (for the height of the posterior mandible ends, one-way ANOVA: F35,72 = 8110, p<0.001, ME = 0.04%; for head inclination, one-way ANOVA: F17,36 = 26701, p<0.001, ME = 0.01%). Therefore, mean values were used for the analysis.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Measurement points of laterality in Exodon paradoxus for the mandible height and head inclination.

Photographs of (a) the left and right mandibles and the height measurements (L and R) between the socket bottom and ventral corner of the retroarticular, (b) the head and vertebrae in the ventral views and their angle (θ), and (c) transparent specimens of lefty and righty individuals in the ventral view. Note that in (c), the lines between the right and left ventral corners of the retroarticular do not cross perpendicularly to the vertical lines that indicate the midlines of the bodies, but rather slant to the left or right in the lefty and righty, respectively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.g001

An index of asymmetry (IAS) was calculated as follows: [2×(RL)/(R+L)]×100, where R and L are the heights of the right and left mandibles, respectively [3]. Individuals with R>L were defined as righty because their right sides dominated over the left, and the IAS was assigned a positive value. By contrast, individuals with R<L were lefty, and IAS was negative. For the angle (θ), righty was defined as an individual whose right side of the head faced front, and the neurocranium bent rightward in the ventral view (Fig. 1b), and θ was given a positive value. By contrast, lefty, in which the left side of the head faced front and the neurocranium bent leftward in the ventral view, had a negative θ.

Statistical analysis

The distributions of IAS and θ were fitted to the following three models: FA, with a normal distribution, mean 0, and SD of data; DA, with a normal distribution, mean≠0, and SD of data; and AS, with two normal distributions ± mean and SD calculated by the maximum likelihood estimation. The Akaike information criterion (AIC) was calculated for each model. The correlation between morphological and behavioral laterality was tested using a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with Poisson distribution using the attacked flank as a dependent variable (“0” was provided for the right flank, “1” was provided for the left flank), the morphological laterality (IAS or θ) and the moment of attacks as fixed factors, and the individual as a random factor. The frequencies of tear-off directions (anterior or posterior of the prey) toward which E. paradoxus turned its jaws when it attacked were compared using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. All model fitting and analyses were performed using R2.11.0 [39].

Results

Scale-eating techniques of Exodon paradoxus

The scale-eating behavior was observed 5–96 (average 34.4±29.1 SD) times/hour in eight E. paradoxus individuals. E. paradoxus dashed forward to the flank of the prey from the side, pressed its snout against the flank, and then turned to the posterior of the prey (Video S1). This tear off direction was seen in 73.6±16.3% (average ± SD) attacks in eight individuals, and the opposite direction, i.e., to the anterior direction of the prey, in 19.1±14.8%. In the remaining 7.3±13.9% of attacks, E. paradoxus did not turn and instead hit the prey straight on. E. paradoxus tore off scales in a posterior direction of the prey significantly more often than the anterior direction (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, Z = 2.52, p<0.05). Such strikes caused scales to detach from the flank of the prey and float in the water column or settle to the substrate; the attacking E. paradoxus then fed on the scales.

Laterality in morphology

Individual differences in the height of the mandibles between left and right sides and in the direction of inclination of the head were detected (Table 1, Fig. 2). In all individuals, the dominant sides were concordant between the mandibles and head angle. The model selection using the AIC showed that the distributions of the IAS of the mandibles and head angles were best fitted to the AS model (Table 2).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Frequency distributions of (a) the IAS of the mandibles and (b) head angle of Exodon paradoxus.

The lines quantified by the second y-axis show the probability curves derived from the three models: the FA-model (dotted line), DA-model (broken line), and AS-model (solid line). Dark-grey bars indicate mandible lefties, and light-grey bars indicate righties.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Jaw laterality and frequency of attacks on the left flank or the right flank of the prey goldfish during the one-hour observation of eight E. paradoxus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. AIC values for the three models to discriminate the type of asymmetry based on the two measures.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.t002

Handedness in scale-eating behavior

The flank of the prey that E. paradoxus attacked was correlated significantly with the morphological laterality, both for the IAS of the mandible and the head angle (Fig. 3, both GLMMs, p<0.05, Tables 3,4). Righty individuals attacked left flanks more frequently than right flanks, and vice versa. This tendency was consistent during the 1-h observations (both GLMMs, NS).

thumbnail
Figure 3. The correlation between morphological laterality and behavioral handedness in Exodon paradoxus.

Scatter plots of the proportion of right-flank attacks by each individual and the IAS of the mandible (a) or head angle (b). Dark-grey points indicate lefties, and light-grey plots indicate righties. The solid lines are the fitted lines for the GLMMs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.g003

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of GLMMs with morphological laterality, IAS and the timings when the attacks occurred as two fixed factors, the individual as a random factor, and which flank of the prey was attacked as a dependent variable.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Results of GLMMs with morphological laterality, θ, and the timings when the attacks occurred as two fixed factors, the individual as a random factor, and which flank of the prey was attacked as a dependent variable.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.t004

Discussion

The scale-eating characin Exodon paradoxus exhibits morphological antisymmetry in its mandibles and concordant head inclination. Consequently, every individual has either a left- or right-dominant side of the mandible in parallel with head inclination, which places the dominant side forward. Furthermore, the morphological laterality correlates significantly with behavioral handedness, i.e., lefty individuals have a tendency to tear scales from the right flank of the prey, and vice versa. However, the correlation between laterality and handedness is the opposite of that in the scale-eating cichlids Perissodus microlepis and P. straeleni in which lefty attacks left flank [2], [14]. This discrepancy seems to be caused by the variation in feeding apparatus and technique; that is, characin scale eaters thrust the dominant side of the mouth outward from the body axis on the flank of the prey to tear off scales, whereas the cichlid scale eaters draw the dominant side inward to the axis or rotate it instead (Fig. 4). E. paradoxus rushes at the flank of the prey perpendicularly with its mouth open or closed [16], presses the external teeth pointed forward against the scales, and then turns to the posterior of the prey to tear off scales. The maxilla is fixed to the neurocranium in characins and provides effective force transfer at the hit, mediated by the external teeth [40]. Conversely, scale-eating cichlids dash at their prey from behind with the mouth opened wide because of their protruding jaws. They tear scales off in two ways [19]: they move their mouths laterally along the flank of the prey to scrape scales off using the edges of recurved laminar teeth arrayed in the jaws (e.g., P. straeleni), or they press their mouths against the flank of the prey and then rotate to wrench scales off with broad-based truncated teeth arrayed in their jaws (e.g., P. microlepis). The latter technique has evolved twice from the former [41]. In this way, characins and cichlids have independently acquired specialized antisymmetric apparatuses for scale eating, as well as the opposite correspondence between jaw laterality and behavioral handedness, with apparent phylogenetic constraints.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Behavioral handedness in scale-eating of (a) righty Exodon paradoxus and (b) lefty Perissodus straeleni.

Arrows indicate the direction in which the scale-eaters hit their prey with its dominant jaw (indicated with gray bar) to scrape scales. Dashed line indicates the body axis of the scale-eaters. Note that the upper jaw of P. straeleni is protruded forward, but the upper jaw of E. paradoxus is fixed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.g004

In P. microlepis and P. straeleni, righty individuals always attack the right flank of the prey and lefties attack the left flanks in nature [2], [3], and even in an aquarium [42]. In another aquarium, however, P. microlepis shows only weak correspondence between the morphological laterality and behavioral handedness possibly because the habituation of the fish to the artificial environment and prey [28]. Therefore, field observation on feeding behavior of E. paradoxus is needed to confirm the actual intensity of the morphology-behavior correspondence.

The conspicuous antisymmetry in external morphology and behavioral handedness and their correspondence have evolved independently in the two lineages of scale-eating fishes. Scale-eating has evolved in at least twelve families of fishes (Table S1), and only E. paradoxus of Characidae, P. microlepis and P. straeleni of Cichlidae are now known to have the both morphological laterality and the related handedness in scale-eating behavior [2], [3]. The other five Tanganyikan Perissodus species [1], [25], [26] and Macrorhamphosodes uradoi of Triacanthoidae [29] have the same morphological laterality, and therefore it is highly possible that they have the behavioral handedness corresponding to their morphological laterality.

In East Africa, one cichlid species, Haplochromis welcommei, in Lake Victoria [43], four cichlids, Genyochromis mento, Corematodus toeniatus, C. shiranus, and Docimodus evelynae, in Lake Malawi [44], are known as scale eaters. On the other hand, at least nine species of six genera of South American characins are scale-eaters [16], [45]. These African cichlids and South American characins are both apparently polyphyletic. Interestingly, the jaw apparatus of these scale-eaters are diverse even within the same family and imply several trophic origins [16], [43]. Further studies on these cichlid and characin scale-eaters and comparison between them will shed light on the origin of morphological novelties such as jaw asymmetry as well as specialized teeth, and the exploitation of a novel food source.

On the other hand, several fishes have the same morphological laterality working in their asymmetric feeding behavior. In largemouse bass, Micropterus salmoides, the righty individuals make counterclockwise attack on the prey fish from the behind, and the lefties do the opposite [8], [27]. In a Tanganyikan cichlid, Neolamprologus fasciatus, righty aims at prey shrimp with the right side of the body abutting a rock and darts to the prey rightward, and lefty does the opposite [9]. This may imply that lateral asymmetry in fishes is not limited in scale-eaters, but are more common, and the laterality may have a significant function in individual-based prey-predator interactions. Further research is needed to clarify how this antisymmetry is ubiquitous in fishes, how the antisymmetry in external morphology correlates with behavioral handedness, and whether the morphological laterality of these fishes shares the genetic background.

Asymmetries are ubiquitous in animals, including humans [46], [47], [48]. The asymmetry in animals involves three aspects, that is, asymmetry in the brain and viscera, that in external morphology, and that in behavior. Our knowledge of their linkages remains limited. For example, behavioral handedness is thought to be a result of cerebral asymmetry [48] or a fortuitous consequence of fluctuating asymmetry, that is, the inability to develop symmetrically [49]. In fishes, however, the structural asymmetry in the brain is concordant with the visceral asymmetry in more than 95% [50], showing directional asymmetry, and the laterality is consistent in species [7], [51]. Therefore, this cerebral asymmetry cannot explain behavioral handedness at an individual level in several fishes, such as Jenynsia lineata [52], Betta splendens [53], and Gambusia holbrooki [54], whose populations share the directional cerebral asymmetry but contain both lefty and righty individuals in behavior. Although functional lateralization of the brain can play a role in these fishes, our study indicates that the external antisymmetry in head inclination and mandibles can be a determinant of behavioral handedness. Alternatively it is possible that the behavioral handedness can alter morphological handedness [55]. Perissodus microlepis were fed with a bilaterally biased dummy prey, “soft-bait dummy fish wrapped in trout skin and with spikes preventing foraging from the forbidden flank [28]” in an aquarium. However, no obvious result is seen in the difference between before and after the six-month experiment. More study is needed to define whether behavior or morphology determines the other asymmetry, or they interact with each other.

The present study adds to the understanding of animal asymmetry by describing another species of scale eater with laterality that has evolved independently and is comparable to the cichlid system. Further studies of these cichlid and characin scale eaters and comparisons between them will shed light on animal asymmetries in external morphology, behavior, and even cerebral and visceral systems, as well as the linkages among these.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Scale-eating fishes in freshwater and marine habitats.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.s001

(DOC)

Video S1.

Scale eating behavior by Exodon paradoxus. This individual was righty in jaw morphology and attacked the left flank of the prey gold fish.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029349.s002

(WMV)

Acknowledgments

We thank Mr. Tetsu Nakahashi for assisting behavioral observation and Dr. Tetsumi Takahashi for morphological observation. We are also grateful to Dr. Satoshi Takahashi for statistical analysis. We deeply appreciate three anonymous referees for improving this manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: HH MH. Performed the experiments: HH MY. Analyzed the data: MY HH. Wrote the paper: HH MH.

References

  1. 1. Palmer AR (2005) Antisymmetry. In: Hallgrimmson B, Hall BK, editors. Variation. Amsterdam: Elsevier. pp. 359–397.
  2. 2. Hori M (1993) Frequency-dependent natural selection in the handedness of scale-eating cichlid fish. Science 260: 216–219.
  3. 3. Hori M, Ochi H, Kohda M (2007) Inheritance pattern of lateral dimorphism in two cichlids (a scale eater, Perissodus microlepis, and an herbivore, Neolamprologus moorii) in Lake Tanganyika. Zool Sci 24: 486–492.
  4. 4. Seki S, Kohda M, Hori M (2000) Asymmetry of mouth morph of a freshwater goby, Rhinogobius flumineus. Zool Sci 17: 1321–1325.
  5. 5. Hata H, Takahashi R, Ashiwa H, Awata S, Takeyama T, et al. (2012) Inheritance patterns of lateral dimorphism examined by breeding experiments with the Tanganyikan cichlid (Julidochromis transcriptus) and the Japanese Medaka (Oryzias latipes). Zool Sci 29: In press.
  6. 6. Hata H, Hori M (2011) Inheritance patterns of morphological laterality in mouth opening of zebrafish, Danio rerio. Laterality. In press.
  7. 7. Bisazza A, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G (1998) The origins of cerebral asymmetry: a review of evidence of behavioural and brain lateralization in fishes, reptiles and amphibians. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22: 411–426.
  8. 8. Nakajima M, Yodo T, Katano O (2007) Righty fish are hooked on the right side of their mouths – observations from an angling experiment with largemouth bass, Micropterus salmoides. Zool Sci 24: 855–859.
  9. 9. Takeuchi Y, Hori M (2008) Behavioural laterality in the shrimp-eating cichlid fish Neolamprologus fasciatus in Lake Tanganyika. Anim Behav 75: 1359–1366.
  10. 10. Bisazza A, Cantalupo C, Robins A, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G (1996) Right-pawedness in toads. Nature 379: 408–408.
  11. 11. Collins RL (1985) On the inheritance of the direction and degree of asymmetry. In: Glick SD, editor. Cerebral lateralization in nonhuman species. New York: Academic Press. pp. 41–71.
  12. 12. Fabre-Thorpe M, Fagot J, Lorincz E, Levesque F, Vauclair J (1993) Laterality in cats: paw preference and performance in a visuomotor activity. Cortex 29: 15–24.
  13. 13. McGrew W, Marchant L (1999) Laterality of hand use pays off in foraging success for wild chimpanzees. Primates 40: 509–513.
  14. 14. Hori M (2007) Dynamics of fish communities and laterality in Lake Tanganyika. In: Matsumoto T, Hasegawa M, editors. Ecology and environment. Tokyo: Baihukan Press. pp. 51–95.
  15. 15. Takeuchi Y, Hori M, Myint O, Kohda M (2010) Lateral bias of agonistic responses to mirror images and morphological asymmetry in the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Behav Brain Res 208: 106–111.
  16. 16. Sazima I (1983) Scale-eating in characoids and other fishes. Environ Biol Fishes 9: 87–101.
  17. 17. Nshombo M, Yanagisawa Y, Nagoshi M (1985) Scale-eating in Perissodus microlepis (Cichlidae) and change of its food habits with growth. JPN J Ichthyol 32: 66–73.
  18. 18. Liem K, Stewart D (1976) Evolution of the scale-eating cichlid fishes of Lake Tanganyika: a generic revision with a description of a new species. Bulletin for the Museum of Comparative Zoology 147: 313–350.
  19. 19. Takahashi R, Moriwaki T, Hori M (2007) Foraging behaviour and functional morphology of two scale-eating cichlids from Lake Tanganyika. J Fish Biol 70: 1458–1469.
  20. 20. Hori M (1987) Mutualism and commensalism in a fish community in Lake Tanganyika. In: Kawano S, Connell J, Hidaka T, editors. Evolution and coadaptation in biotic communities. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press. pp. 219–239.
  21. 21. Nshombo M (1994) Foraging behavior of the scale-eater Plecodus straeleni (Cichlidae, Teleostei) in Lake Tanganyika, Africa. Environ Biol Fishes 39: 59–72.
  22. 22. Nshombo M (1994) Polychromatism of the scale-eater Perissodus microlepis (Cichlidae, Teleostei) in relation to foraging behavior. J Ethol 12: 141–161.
  23. 23. Sazima I (1977) Possible case of aggressive mimicry in a neotropical scale-eating fish. Nature 270: 510–512.
  24. 24. Hori M, Watanabe K (2000) Aggressive mimicry in the intra-populational color variation of the Tanganyikan scale-eater Perissodus microlepis (Cichlidae). Environ Biol Fishes 59: 111–115.
  25. 25. Hori M (1991) Feeding relationships among cichlid fishes in Lake Tanganyika: effects of intra- and interspecific variation of feeding behavior on their coexistence. INTECOL Bulletin 19: 89–102.
  26. 26. Stewart T, Albertson RC (2010) Evolution of a unique predatory feeding apparatus: functional anatomy, development and a genetic locus for jaw laterality in Lake Tanganyika scale-eating cichlids. BMC Biol 8: 8.
  27. 27. Yasugi M, Hori M (2011) Predominance of cross-predation between lateral morphs in a largemouth bass and a freshwater goby. Zool Sci 28: In press.
  28. 28. Van Dooren TJM, Van Goor HA, Van Putten M (2010) Handedness and asymmetry in scale-eating cichlids: antisymmetries of different strength. Evolution 64: 2159–2165.
  29. 29. Nakae M, Sasaki K (2002) A scale-eating triacanthodid, Macrorhamphosodes uradoi: prey fishes and mouth “handedness” (Tetraodontiformes, Triacanthoidei). Ichthyol Res 49: 7–14.
  30. 30. Roberts T (1970) Scale-eating American characoid fishes, with special reference to Probolodus heterostomus. Proc California Acad Sci 37: 383–390.
  31. 31. Poliana RP, Carlos SA, Rafael JO, Elineide EM (2007) Trophic guilds of fishes in sandbank habitats of a Neotropical river. Neotrop Ichtyol 5: 399–404.
  32. 32. Westneat MW (2004) Evolution of levers and linkages in the feeding mechanisms of fishes. Integr Comp Biol 44: 378–389.
  33. 33. Novakowski GC, Fugi R, Hahn NS (2004) Diet and dental development of three species of Roeboides (Characiformes: Characidae). Neotrop Ichthyol 2: 157–162.
  34. 34. Müller J, Troschel F (1845) Die familie der characinen. Horae Ichthyologicae I & II: 1–40.
  35. 35. Westneat MW (1990) Feeding mechanics of teleost fishes (Labridae, Perciformes) - a test of 4-bar linkage models. J Morph 205: 269–295.
  36. 36. Albertson R, Streelman J, Kocher T, Yelick P (2005) Integration and evolution of the cichlid mandible: the molecular basis of alternate feeding strategies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 16287–16292.
  37. 37. Bailey RC, Byrnes J (1990) A new, old method for assessing measurement error in both univariate and multivariate morphometric studies. Syst Biol 39: 124–130.
  38. 38. Merilä J, Björklund M (1995) Fluctuating asymmetry and measurement error. Syst Biol 44: 97–101.
  39. 39. R Development Core Team (2011) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0. Available: http://www.R-project.org/. Accessed 2011 Apr. 1.
  40. 40. Janovetz J (2005) Functional morphology of feeding in the scale-eating specialist Catoprion mento. J Exp Biol 208: 4757–4768.
  41. 41. Takahashi R, Watanabe K, Nishida M, Hori M (2007) Evolution of feeding specialization in Tanganyikan scale-eating cichlids: a molecular phylogenetic approach. BMC Evol Biol 7: 195.
  42. 42. Lee HJ, Pittlik S, Jones JC, Salzburger W, Barluenga M, et al. (2010) Genetic support for random mating between left and right-mouth morphs in the dimorphic scale-eating cichlid fish Perissodus microlepis from Lake Tanganyika. J Fish Biol 76: 1940–1957.
  43. 43. Fryer G, Iles TD (1972) The cichlid fishes of the Great Lakes of Africa. Neptune: TFH Publications.
  44. 44. Eccles DH, Lewis DSC (1976) A revision of the genus Docimodus Boulenger (Pisces: Cichlidae), a group of fishes with unusual feeding habits from Lake Malawi. Zool J Linnean Soc 58: 165–172.
  45. 45. Froese R, Pauly D (2011) FishBase. World Wide Web electronic publication. Available: http://www.fishbase.org/. Accessed: 2011 Aug. 1.
  46. 46. Corballis MC (2009) The evolution and genetics of cerebral asymmetry. Phil Trans R Soc B 364: 867–879.
  47. 47. Schaafsma SM, Riedstra BJ, Pfannkuche KA, Bouma A, Groothuis TGG (2009) Epigenesis of behavioural lateralization in humans and other animals. Phil Trans R Soc B 364: 915–927.
  48. 48. Tommasi L (2009) Mechanisms and functions of brain and behavioural asymmetries. Phil Trans R Soc B 364: 855–859.
  49. 49. Collins R, Sargent E, Neumann P (1993) Genetic and behavioral tests of the Mcmanus hypothesis relating response to selection for lateralization of handedness in mice to degree of heterozygosity. Behav Genet 23: 413–421.
  50. 50. Facchin L, Burgess HA, Siddiqi M, Granato M, Halpern ME (2009) Determining the function of zebrafish epithalamic asymmetry. Phil Trans R Soc B 364: 1021–1032.
  51. 51. Concha ML, Wilson SW (2001) Asymmetry in the epithalamus of vertebrates. J Anat 199: 63–84.
  52. 52. Bisazza A, Cantalupo C, Vallortigara G (1997) Lateral asymmetries during escape behavior in a species of teleost fish (Jenynsia lineata). Physiol Behav 61: 31–35.
  53. 53. Cantalupo C, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (1996) Lateralization of displays during aggressive and courtship behaviour in the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Physiol Behav 60: 249–252.
  54. 54. Bisazza A (1996) Rotational bias in mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki): The role of laterality and sun-compass navigation. Laterality 1: 161–175.
  55. 55. Palmer AR (2010) Scale-eating cichlids: from hand(ed) to mouth. J Biol 9: 11.