Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

BMI Not WHR Modulates BOLD fMRI Responses in a Sub-Cortical Reward Network When Participants Judge the Attractiveness of Human Female Bodies

Abstract

In perceptual terms, the human body is a complex 3d shape which has to be interpreted by the observer to judge its attractiveness. Both body mass and shape have been suggested as strong predictors of female attractiveness. Normally body mass and shape co-vary, and it is difficult to differentiate their separate effects. A recent study suggested that altering body mass does not modulate activity in the reward mechanisms of the brain, but shape does. However, using computer generated female body-shaped greyscale images, based on a Principal Component Analysis of female bodies, we were able to construct images which covary with real female body mass (indexed with BMI) and not with body shape (indexed with WHR), and vice versa. Twelve observers (6 male and 6 female) rated these images for attractiveness during an fMRI study. The attractiveness ratings were correlated with changes in BMI and not WHR. Our primary fMRI results demonstrated that in addition to activation in higher visual areas (such as the extrastriate body area), changing BMI also modulated activity in the caudate nucleus, and other parts of the brain reward system. This shows that BMI, not WHR, modulates reward mechanisms in the brain and we infer that this may have important implications for judgements of ideal body size in eating disordered individuals.

Introduction

In perceptual terms, the human body is an evolutionarily important, complex 3D shape. It potentially conveys a wide range of information, including information important for human mate selection. Behavioural studies have shown that both male and female observers are consistent and reliable in their ratings of the attractiveness of female bodies [1], [2]. It is likely that the perception of attractive bodies is linked to the reward systems in the brain. Several studies have shown that processing pleasant pictures differs from neutral pictures by activation in regions including the anterior cingulate, left precuneus, right and left insula, right inferior frontal gyrus, and left caudate nucleus [3][5]. Additionally, when observers judge facial attractiveness functional imaging shows an activation of the medial orbitofrontal cortex, left anterior frontal cortex, left frontal-temporal junction, nucleus accumbens, right caudate nucleus, and visual cortex [6][8]. This pattern of activation is likely to reflect the positive reward properties of faces. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in processing reward-based stimuli has been extensively documented [6], [9] and it is likely that an observer's preference for an attractive face is mediated by its reward value. We therefore hypothesized the attractiveness preferences for bodies would activate the same reward systems.

The two features of the body most frequently used to explain attractiveness judgments are overall body fat, indexed by the body mass index (BMI) (e.g. [1], [10], [11]) and the specific distribution of fat deposition on the lower body, indexed by the ratio of waist circumference to hip circumference — the waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) (e.g. [12], [13]. The role of BMI in attractiveness judgements is particularly important, as it is an over-estimation of body mass that systematically shifts the ideal body size of eating disordered women towards a lower body weight (i.e. they see themselves as bigger than they are, which produces a dissatisfaction with their body size), which in turn drives their restrictive dietary behaviour (e.g. [14][16]).

Under normal circumstances, BMI and WHR co-vary in Caucasian populations [17]. For example, the Health Survey for England [18], which includes directly obtained measurements from 1808 Caucasian women of reproductive age (16–45) ranging in BMI from around 15–45, shows a correlation between BMI and WHR of 0.45. That BMI and WHR and other related physical variables are correlated is not surprising. As a body adds fat, the circumference of the waist and hips also increases; BMI is strongly correlated with both waist circumference (Pearson Correlation, r = 0.87, p<0.0001) and the hip circumference (r = 0.90, p<0.0001). WHR is also correlated with weight (r = 0.40, p<0.0001) and to a lesser extent height (r = 0.12, p<0.0001) [18]. For studies of attractiveness, the correlation between features such as BMI and WHR raises the problem of collinearity amongst explanatory variables, and raises the question of whether WHR or BMI is the primary cue used in attractiveness judgements.

In an attempt to avoid this problem, a recent fMRI study used before and after photographs of the lower torsos (from the bottom of the ribcage to half way down the thigh) of 7 women who had undergone a cosmetic surgical procedure [19]. The surgery involved moving adipose tissue from the stomach to the hips and thighs, and it is reported that this changed WHR but made no statistically significant change to BMI. These images appeared to activate regions which are associated with neural reward mechanisms (such as the anterior cingulate cortex and nucleus accumbens). It was suggested that the changing WHR of these images modulated the activity in the reward systems, but by contrast, changing BMI had no effect. However, this study had two serious flaws. Firstly, there were potential problems with the images. The photographs were not standardised and vary in viewing angle (varying between a profile view and a view-point behind the body) and illumination in the before and after conditions, so although the women in these pictures are suggested not to vary significantly in BMI in the before and after surgery pictures, they may appear to alter in their BMI. Moreover, both behavioural and eye-movement studies suggest that the degree of stomach depth (i.e. the degree to which the stomach protrudes) is used as a key cue to judge BMI [10], [20], [21]. This surgical intervention, which artificially alters this part of the body, may lead observers to perceive a difference in BMI in the before and after condition. This is important because, the observers have only the visual image to go on, and if the image appears to vary in BMI (even if there is no significant change in the BMI of participants in the photographs) then the observers will react to the images as though they do alter in BMI. Thus, the apparent BMI and WHR of the pictures may co-vary and it is not clear whether the reported changes in neural activity were due to changes in WHR, apparent BMI or some mixture of the two. Secondly, it appears that no correction factor was applied to the BOLD activity reported in their paper, to compensate for multiple statistical testing in the analysis. Such an analysis is not without precedent. A number of published studies have also not used corrections factors, but from a purely statistical standpoint, without such correction factors BOLD activations cannot be said to be statistically significant [22].

It is possible to construct a set of artificial female bodies using four independent descriptors of shape derived from a principal components analysis (PCA) of real body shape [1]. The body mass of these bodies can be quantified using the perimeter-to-area ratio (PAR) [21], [23]. The PCA allows a separation of the different components of body size and shape, and allows the construction of sets of bodies which vary in BMI but not in WHR, and in WHR but not BMI. We combined the behavioural rating of these images with an fMRI study to determine whether changing BMI or WHR does modulate activity in the reward areas of the brain and to establish a foundation for studies on patients with disordered perception of body size and shape, such as patients with Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa.

Methods

Ethics Statement

Ethical approval was given for this study from the Aston University Human Sciences Ethical Committee and was conducted in accordance with the guidance given in The Declaration of Helsinki [24]. Participants received information about the study including its purpose, explaining what their participation would involve and explaining their right not to participate and to withdraw from the study at any time. Each participant gave their written consent to participation before the study they took part in commenced.

Participants

12 Participants were recruited from the student and staff population at Aston University (6 were male and 6 were female). Each participant was screened for their safety and health according to a protocol approved by Aston University Human Sciences Ethical Committee 24 Hrs before attending, and again immediately prior to, their scanning session. Before the fMRI scan, participants were shown the stimulus sequence running and used the button response to practice rating one or two images on a computer screen outside the scanner room. They were instructed to provide their initial reaction and reassured that the images were computer generated images and not photographs of real women who could potentially be upset by the ratings.

Behavioural Methods

During the experiment participants viewed grey scale images of artificial female figures presented via a back projector onto a screen mounted at the end of the MR scanner. The stimuli (N = 40) were generated from a principal components analysis of images of real figures [1], and an accurate estimate of the BMI of the bodies can be calculated using PAR [23]. This estimate is referred to as BMIPAR. In this study, BMIPAR varied from 20.9 to 23.3 (limits that are well within the normal BMI range of 18.5–24.9). We varied the WHR range from 0.65 to 0.77. Examples of two figures and the effect of varying PAR are shown in figure 1A.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Female Body Shape Stimuli and experimental design.

A): Sample images ranged from a low value of PAR (left) to a high value (right). There were 40 such images with different PAR values spanning a range corresponding to the full range of body shapes typical of the normal BMI range for young female adults. B): The stimulus sequence: Fixation cross (1, 2 or 3s); Body image appears with empty rating bar; after 2s of body attractiveness assessment the rating bar started to fill in red alerting participant to the approaching reporting phase; 200ms later the fill colour changed to green after which a rating of attractiveness could be made by pressing a response button, short green segments signalling low attractiveness ratings, and long green segments signalling high attractiveness ratings, the green bar filled completely in 2s; 7s after first appearing the stimulus offset to a blank screen followed by a random delay before the next trial started, indicated by fixation re-appearance. Total trial durations were, 16, 18 or 20 s.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027255.g001

Procedure

Refer to figure 1B showing the stimulus sequence. In an event-related design, the body images were displayed for 7 seconds, appearing after a fixation cross of variable duration (1–3 seconds). The rating phase started 2 seconds after the appearance of the stimulus body image with the appearance of an animated scale bar at the bottom of the screen. The scale bar started filling with red from the left hand end of the bar to alert the participants to prepare to deliver their attractiveness rating. Then 200 ms later, the scale bar started to fill with green, indicating that a response could be made. Participants pressed the response button when the proportion of green filling matched their attractiveness rating, and the bar stopped filling. If the bar was allowed to completely fill with green it took about 2 seconds, representing the most attractive response rating. At the end of the 7 second stimulus display time, the screen was blanked to black. The blank inter-stimulus interval (ISI) continued until the fixation cross appeared again to cue the next trial. The length of the ISI varied between 6 and 12 s.

FMRI Methods

All MRI images were collected using a 3-Tesla Siemens Magentom Trio MRI Scanner at Aston University. T2* weighted gradient echo sequences were acquired with the following parameters: TR = 3000ms, TE = 60ms, 64x64 matrix of 3x3mm in plane resolution, 44 slices of 3mm thickness per whole brain volume. 241 volumes were collected in a total experiment run-time of about 12 minutes. In addition, an anatomical volume was acquired using MP-RAGE inversion recovery sequence with GRAPPA, 256 x256 matrix, 1x1x1 mm voxels and 176 slices.

Analysis

FMRI images were analyzed using SPM2 (The Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). The images were realigned, normalised to the MNI template brain and smoothed to 8mm FWHM. The following events of interest were modelled with the canonical haemodynamic response function (HRF) for each participant for the first level analysis: fixation onset, body image onset (BODY), and the scale bar onset (JUDGE). Covariates of interest included in the model design were the participant rating (RATING), the body image BMI index (BMIpar) and the body image WHR index (WHR). The main focus of the analyses were the separate regressions of the BOLD activation associated with the BODY event and the body image shape (BMIPAR), body image waist-to-hip ratio (WHR), and participant attractiveness ratings (RATING). The statistical contrast images (T statistic images) for each participant were then entered into the second level group analysis. For the fMRI results the statistical threshold for detection of a significant response across the group of participants was set at a false discovery rate (FDR) of (p<0.05) corrected for the whole brain volume, and additionally the minimum size of cluster(k) accepted was typically K > = 8 for an uncorrected cluster significance cPu<0.05.

Results

Behavioural Results

The behavioural results (Figure 2) show a negative correlation of attractiveness ratings with BMIPAR (r = −0.84, P<0.0001). However, there was no significant correlation of attractiveness with WHR (r = −0.07, p = 0.666).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Participant attractiveness ratings: correlation with BMI and WHR.

A)Plot of average attractiveness ratings against the BMIPAR of the bodies in the photographs. B)Plot of average attractiveness ratings against the WHR of the bodies in the photographs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027255.g002

Negative Regression of BMIPAR with Body Event

Table 1 shows the results of a negative regression of the BOLD signal associated with the BODY event and the BMIPAR values. The locations of activations reported in the table (columns X, Y, Z in mm) were transformed from those reported from the SPM2 software using the procedure of [25] to obtain more accurate reference to Talairach and Tourneaux atlas co-ordinates [26]. Anatomical identification was obtained by submitting the transformed co-ordinates to the Talairach Daemon Java client [27]. The X,Y & Z co-ordinates were additionally clustered using hierarchical k-means clustering in R using a minimum variance distance measure (procedure hlust(), http://www.r-project.org/), and the resulting tree cut to yield 9 clusters, shown in table 1 in column Cl and labelled 1 to 9 . This gives broadly the same clustering as provided by SPM2, but provides some additional structuring of the activations. Within the clusters the ordering of the activations was retained as in the original SPM output. Column Sx in table 1 provides an ordered index to allow readers to re-sort the table into the original ordering from SPM2 if required (Figure 3a).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Negative Regression of BMIPAR with BOLD.

A)Cluster dendrogram for (X,Y,Z) peak BOLD activations sites for the negative regression of BMIPAR with BOLD signal intensity based on a linear distance measure computed on the (Talairach transformed) co-ordinates of significant peaks of BOLD activation. Dendrograms in the following figures were computed this way too. B)Negative regression of BMIPAR on BOLD intensity. Activations are superimposed on the Colin27 template supplied with SPM2 courtesy of the Montreal Neurological Institute. Cross hairs located at [−45, −69, −6] (untransformed MNI co-ordinates) show the putative location of the EBA in the left hemisphere; there is a corresponding activation in the right hemisphere. C)Bilateral caudate activation is visible at the cross hair position [−15, +12, −3] (untransformed MNI co-ordinates) and the corresponding position in the right hemisphere. D)A superiorly positioned slice selection shows frontal eye fields and bilateral posterior parietal activity. Crosshair location [+30, −3 ,+56] untransformed coordinates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027255.g003

Cluster 7 of table 1 shows regions of the caudate and lenticulate nucleus bilaterally where BOLD signal increased with decreasing BMIPAR. Lower BMIPAR values indicate slimmer body shapes and these were rated as more attractive by our participants, so increasing attractiveness, indexed by BMIpar, increases activity within the caudate and lenticulate nuclei.

There was also extensive activation of visual cortical regions within the occipital lobe, but of chief interest to us are the two regions underlined and italicised in table 1: clusters 1(Sx = 1) and 5(Sx = 13). These were the largest clusters of activation observed; they were located in the middle occipital gyrus. The crosshairs in figure 3B are centred at the largest activation (−45.6, −68.4, −5.7), identified in table 1 as cluster 1(Sx = 1); we believe this is the left hemispheric location of the extrastriate body area (EBA), BA37 (cf.[28], [29]). Its companion activation visible in the right hemisphere (cluster 5(Sx = 12)) was located just below the cortical surface but the activated region extends into the cortex and the second cluster sub-peak 5(Sx = 13) lies in the region of the EBA in the right occipital lobe (48.9, −65.8, −6.5), BA 37. The balance of the other brain activity revealed with this analysis reflects extensive visual activations in the occipital lobe – Clusters 1 & 5, BA 18 & 19 – and the occipito-parietal cortex – clusters 3, 6, & 8 BA40 & BA7. Frontal regions, possibly the frontal eye fields, are also engaged in the task (Figure 3D, axial slice, and cluster 2, 4 & 9, BA6- also compare this image with figure 6 from[30]) all of which may reflect the relatively high level visual judgement and visuo-motor planning needed to perform the task. The results show that brain regions responsive to body-shape and size can be modulated by artificially produced body-shaped stimuli with varying BMIPAR.

Positive Regression of BMIPAR with Body

The results of a positive regression of BODY and BMIPAR values are shown in table 2. The analysis was as for the negative BOLD regression with BMIPAR and again the k-means clustering tree of the corrected X,Y & Z co-ordinates (figure 4a) was cut to yield 9 clusters; these are shown in column Cl of table 2 , labelled 1–9. The preponderance of BOLD activity observed was in or near the posterior cingulate gyrus of the right hemisphere (figure 4B). The largest region activated (Table 2, Cl = 1,Sx = 1–3) had an extended volume of activation engulfing the parahippocampal gyrus and the posterior caudate. Cluster 4 (Sx = 8, 13, 16) seems to be a companion activation to this in the left hemisphere. Anterior cingulate activation was also evident, cluster 2 showing the close association between the left and right cortices; one location in this group was reported within the superior frontal gyrus (Cl = 2, Sx = 22, BA9). Visual association areas within the occipito- temporal route were activated too, Clusters 5, 7, 8 & 9 predominantly being reported as BA 18, 19, 20, 21, & 22; bilateral anterior temporal lobe activation within the medial temporal gyrus is shown in Figure 4C. A single relatively strong activation was seen in the superior frontal gyrus (cluster 6, Sx = 12, [−21.0, 13.5, 51.1] Talairach transformed coordinates; figure not shown), suggesting the engagement of the frontal eye fields as observed with the previous contrast.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Positive Regression of BMIPAR with BOLD.

A)Cluster dendrogram for (X,Y,Z) peak BOLD activations sites for the positive regression of BMIPAR with BOLD signal intensity. B)Bilateral cingulate gyrus activation. The cross hair was positioned between the two posterior cingulate gyrus activations at [−3, −48, 9] so both can be seen. Crosshair location in untransformed MNI co-ordinates. C)Bilateral anterior temporal lobe activation within the medial temporal gyrus (BA21). The cross hair was positioned at [−51, −12, −9] untransformed MNI coordinates, table 2 Cl5(Sx = 10).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027255.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Positive regression of BODY and BMIPAR (otherwise as in table 1).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027255.t002

Positive Regression of WHR with Body

No activations reached the predetermined criteria for detection (false discovery rate p(FRD)<0.05, cluster extent K>6, cPu<0.05), or when the FDR criterion was reduced to P<.1 ; no figure is provided. Based on uncorrected cluster statistics alone (cPu<0.05), three areas of activation were suggestive of underlying brain activity. The largest region was in the inferior parietal lobule of the left hemisphere ( (−37.86, −49, 47.45), BA 40, and (−40.65, −40.07, 51.14), BA 40) with a small corresponding region in the right hemisphere (39.79, −41.01, 57.82; BA 40).

Negative Regression of WHR with Body

A negative regression of WHR with BOLD signal found no region that met the false discovery rate and cluster level statistical criteria for detection. One activation was observed that met the cluster level criterion (cluster extent >7, p<.05) in the right superior temporal gyrus (−42, 12.1, 57.0; BA22, cluster equivalent K  = 10, p = 0.02); no figure is provided. This is suggestive of high order visual processing of WHR-specific information within the temporal lobe, but the evidence is inconclusive.

Contrast between Judge and Body

The level of BOLD response was compared in a simple main effects contrast between JUDGE and BODY. The body-shaped pictures were visible in both periods. The k-means clustering tree of the corrected X,Y & Z co-ordinates (figure 4a) was cut to yield 10 clusters. The results are dominated by visually activated regions shown in tables 3 and 4 as clusters Cl 1 & 3 (left hemisphere) Cl 2 (RH). In the frontal lobe there were some small regions within BA6 with significantly increased BOLD activity during the BODY interval compared to the JUDGE interval (Cluster 5); again we infer these were associated with activity in the frontal eye fields.

In addition to the general visual cortical activation, medial cortical and sub-cortical structures also showed significantly increased BOLD signal during the BODY interval. Clusters 4 & 6 are a group of BOLD activation loci identified by the clustering algorithm on the anterior midline. These clusters are dominated by cingulate cortex activation but they also include the underlying midbrain structures nearby: the caudate nucleus, the lentiform nucleus and the putamen, hinting that these brain regions form a network involved in this task. Figure 5 B shows the bilateral BOLD activation in the caudate nucleus; the cross-hairs are located at the right caudate activation ([9.9, −2.9, 15.0] Corrected coordinates, Cl 6, Sx = 15). Furthermore, bilateral activation of the insula cortex was detected (Cl = 7 & 8). These regions are the bilateral anterior lateral BOLD activations shown in figure 5 C axial and tangential sections.

thumbnail
Figure 5. T-Contrast of BOLD signal between the intervals JUDGE and BODY.

A)Cluster dendrogram for (X,Y,Z) peak BOLD activations sites for the contrast between the period of participant response (JUDGE) and participant body evaluation (BODY). B)Bilateral visual cortical activations in posterior parietal and lateral occipital regions. Bilateral activation in and around the caudate nucleus is present at the position of the cross hairs: [+12, 0,+12] untransformed MNI co-ordinates. C)At a lower plane, bilateral activation foci are present in the insula cortex. Cross hairs: [+42, +15, −3] untransformed MNI co-ordinates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027255.g005

Negative Regression of Body on Participant Rating

When participant rating was modelled with a negative regression on the BOLD response BODY only one region met the statistical criteria, a cluster within the cingulate gyrus BA24 ( [−15.4, −2.8, 44.3] corrected coordinates, Sx = 1, no figure is provided) (see table 5). If the false discovery criterion is relaxed so that activations with p(FDR) <0.1 then several small regions of parietal lobe activations may be inferred within BA7, BA40, BA2. On the same basis minor activations of medial frontal gyrus (−7.2, −14.6, 48.7; BA6) and precentral gyrus (48.9, 3.3, 8.12; BA44) can be seen. These activations are perhaps related to visuo-motor activity. Of more interest, one small region (23 voxels extent, Sx = 7) of activation in the insula cortex was observed under the relaxed statistical criteria ([4.08, −45.8, −23.4], corrected coordinates, Sx  = 9). As the insula is known to be involved in emotional processing of negatively valenced stimuli then the reduced BOLD activation with increasing participant rating is weak evidence that this region is responding preferentially to negatively valenced stimuli. There were no regions that showed a positive regression of RATING on BODY.

Discussion

Our behavioural results suggest that BMIPAR is closely correlated with attractiveness judgments and that WHR is not. This is consistent with previous behavioural and eye-movement studies which suggest that the primary predictor of attractiveness judgements for a female body is BMI, and that other physical features, such as WHR, have a much weaker effect on these judgements (e.g. [1], [10], [21]). Consistent with these behavioural results, our imaging results show that altering the apparent overall body mass of the bodies (as indexed by the BMIPAR) modulates activity in both the higher visual areas and in neural areas which form part of the brain reward system including the caudate nucleus. Caudate activation has been observed with a range of rewards including cocaine [31], nicotine [32], money [33], and feedback on performance on behavioural tasks [34]. By comparison, modulation of the WHR produced no significant change in BOLD activity.

In this study we used both male and female participants, whereas the Platek & Singh study used just male participants. However, previous studies have shown extremely high correlations (correlations greater than 0.95) between the ratings of male and female participants in attractiveness ratings of female bodies (e.g. [2], [35][38]). This is a degree of correlation no different than the correlation between two groups of the same gender (i.e. males versus males or females versus females), strongly suggesting that both genders assess female bodies in the same way. This is predicted by mate selection theory which postulates that individuals will not only be able to judge the attractiveness of members of the opposite sex, but also will know their own attractiveness relative to other members of the same sex (e.g., [39]). This information allows an individual to concentrate on potential partners of the same attractiveness as themselves, thus avoiding both unsuccessful courtship of a more attractive partner (potentially wasteful in time and resources) and accepting a less attractive partner (with a potentially negative impact on future reproductive success). Thus, both male and female observers should assess the bodies in the same way.

The fMRI results show that the appraisal of body shape involves a complex network of brain regions, with aspects of the task related to visual shape processing generally activating posterior regions; activation of the extra-striate body area (EBA) was seen, but not the fusiform body area (FBA). The EBA is believed to sensitive to body parts and the FBA to the whole body [40], forming part of a system which has been suggested to be analogous to the face recognition system [41]. As the stimuli used here did not include the whole body (see figure 1), but were centred on the torso, it is possible that they were not the optimal stimuli for the FBA.

Studies which have looked at judgements of attractiveness using either photographs of bodies (e.g.,[2], [35], [37], [42][45]), video clips of bodies (e.g.[20], [37]) or laser scanned 3D bodies (e.g. [11], [46]) have found that BMI is the primary predictor of attractiveness and health judgements by both male and female observers. Additionally in an eye-movement study, Cornelissen et al. [10] asked people to rate images for BMI, WHR and attractiveness. The areas of the body fixated when judging BMI were also fixated when estimating attractiveness, suggesting an assessment of BMI is part of the judgements made when rating attractiveness. The areas fixated when estimating WHR were not included in the areas fixated when estimating attractiveness, suggesting WHR is not directly assessed in attractiveness judgements. The importance of BMI is not only true of western populations, but seems to apply cross-culturally (e.g. [43], [47][49]). The importance of BMI in attractiveness judgments makes sense in an evolutionary context as it provides a reliable cue to female health (e.g. [50], [51]) and reproductive potential (e.g. [52][55]). However, this is not to say that WHR plays no role in attractiveness judgements. Several studies which have tried to separate out the relative importance of different physical features in predicting attractiveness judgements have found a weak role for WHR (e.g. [1], [37]). Additionally, several studies have explored which physical variables in their female participants predict their ratings of male faces and found significant correlations between WHR and their choices ([56][58]). This may imply that WHR may be linked to the female participant's estimate of their own attractiveness. Thus, it is quite possible that using images of female bodies with a much wider range of WHR, it would have been possible to find a correlation between this feature and our participants' attractiveness judgements and a corresponding activation of brain reward mechanisms. However, given the weaker predictive power of WHR for attractiveness judgements, it would be consistent to expect a correspondingly weaker activation of the brain's reward centres with modulating WHR even over a wide range of shapes.

Our behavioural results show a significant correlation between the BMIpar of the bodies in our images and their attractiveness ratings by our observers. Our results also show a significant correlation between BMIpar and activation of part of the brain reward areas in our observers brains, but they do not show a correlation between the ratings of attractiveness and BOLD activation of brain reward areas unless we relax our correction factors for multiple comparisons in the analysis (like [19]). This may be because the simple activation of the reward centres then has to be filtered through more complex cognitive decision making mechanisms to generate a rating response, which may weaken a simple 1 to 1 mapping of reward reaction to rating response so that it only meets a lower level of significance. It may also be that any variance in the ratings will act to reduce the detectability of the relationship between ratings and brain activation. Although images with a particular BMIPAR value may always produce the same BOLD activation, the corresponding behavioural rating of the image over the course of these presentations will fluctuate around a mean value. If this fluctuation is relatively high, then it becomes harder to model the relationship between the brain's response and the ratings even though the BOLD response itself is reliably linked to the image.

Our experimental results show that increasing apparent body mass, indexed by BMIPAR, is strongly related to decreased BOLD response within the caudate nucleus bilaterally. Other mid-brain nuclei similarly modulated but to a lesser degree are the putamen and the anterior thalamic nucleus. The contrast between JUDGE and BODY showed BOLD activation was decreased in these midbrain structures, and within the insula cortex bilaterally (Figure 4B & C), during body shape appraisal (BODY) with respect to the later rating period (JUDGE). An increasing BMI has consistently been correlated with decreasing attractiveness ratings for female bodies (e.g. [1], [2]). The decrease in activation in caudate nucleus in response to decreasing preference is consistent with the results which have suggested reduced activity in the caudate nucleus with negative reward in a decision making task [59] and reduced activity in the caudate nucleus in response to aesthetically less pleasing representational and abstract paintings [5], [60]. Additionally, there is reduced activity in caudate nucleus in depressed patients relative to normal controls [61], [62]. One feature of depression is a decrease in the ability to experience pleasure and reward (anhedonia). A comorbidity between depression and Anorexia Nervosa (AN) has long been established, indeed part of the diagnostic criteria of AN is a disturbed body image [63]. The reduced activity in the caudate nucleus might be implicated in the impaired ability to accurately evaluate an attractive and healthy body (perceptions of attractiveness and health are very highly correlated) which has been demonstrated in the Anorexic observers [14], [15]. Activation within the lentiform nucleus and the anterior thalamus was also seen and this may point to this group of related sub-cortical nuclei being involved in processing body-shape judgements.

Our results suggest that BMI modulates reward mechanisms in the brain and we infer that this may have important implications for judgements of ideal body size in eating disordered individuals. Controlling body size through restricting diet, often augmented by excessive exercise and/or purging and vomiting, is a central feature of Anorexia Nervosa (e.g.[64], [65]). Sufferers are constantly checking their body size in the mirror and their weight on the scales. Behavioural studies have shown Anorexic observers prefer a significantly lower ideal body size for both their own, and other women's bodies [14], [15] and the progressive activation of the brain's reward mechanisms as BMI decreases shown in our study provides a potential mechanism by which this activity is rewarded and reinforced.

Further work will be needed to conclusively demonstrate that the caudate is important in judgements related to body size and shape, and whether there is a more extensive sub-cortical network of brain regions within which the caudate participates. This suggests that a further investigation to study individual variability with a focus on this sub-cortical network as region of interest would be a fruitful avenue for future research. Beyond this we look towards studies that seek to determine whether differences in activity in the nuclei around the caudate head play a role in the development of eating disorders.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: IEH MJT. Performed the experiments: IEH NJT OAL MJT. Analyzed the data: IEH MJT OAL NJT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: PJBH. Wrote the paper: IEH MJT.

References

  1. 1. Smith K, Tovee M, Hancock P, Bateson M, Cox M, et al. (2007) An analysis of body shape attractiveness based on image statistics: Evidence for a dissociation between expressions of preference and shape discrimination. Visual Cognition 15: 927–953.
  2. 2. Tovee M, Hancock P, Mahmoodi S, Singleton B, Cornelissen P (2002) Human female attractiveness: waveform analysis of body shape. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 269: 2205–2213.
  3. 3. Lane R, Reiman E, Bradley M, Lang P, Ahern G, et al. (1997) Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia 35: 1437–1444.
  4. 4. Paradiso S, Johnson D, Andreasen N, O'Leary D, Watkins G, et al. (1999) Cerebral blood flow changes associated with attribution of emotional valence to pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral visual stimuli in a PET study of normal subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry 156: 1618–1629.
  5. 5. Teasdale J, Howard R, Cox S, Ha Y, Brammer M, et al. (1999) Functional MRI study of the cognitive generation of affect. American Journal of Psychiatry 156: 209–215.
  6. 6. Aharon I, Etcoff N, Ariely D, Chabris C, O'Connor E, et al. (2001) Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron 32: 537–551.
  7. 7. Nakamura K, Kawashima R, Nagumo S, Ito K, Sugiura M, et al. (1998) Neuroanatomical correlates of the assessment of facial attractiveness. Neuroreport 9: 753–757.
  8. 8. O'Doherty J, Winston J, Critchley H, Perrett D, Burt D, et al. (2003) Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia 41: 147–155.
  9. 9. Montague P, Berns G (2002) Neural economics and the biological substrates of valuation. Neuron 36: 265–284.
  10. 10. Cornelissen P, Hancock P, Kiviniemi V, George H, Tovee M (2009) Patterns of eye movements when male and female observers judge female attractiveness, body fat and waist-to-hip ratio. Evolution and Human Behavior 30: 417–428.
  11. 11. Fan J, Liu F, Wu J, Dai W (2004) Visual perception of female physical attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 271: 347–352.
  12. 12. Singh D (1993) Adaptive significance of female attractiveness: role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 293–307.
  13. 13. Streeter S, McBurney D (2003) Waist-hip ratio and attractiveness - New evidence and a critique of “a critical test.” Evolution and Human Behavior 24: 88–98.
  14. 14. Tovee M, Benson P, Emery J, Mason S, Cohen-Tovee E (2003) Measurement of body size and shape perception in eating-disordered and control observers using body-shape software. British Journal of Psychology 94: 501–516.
  15. 15. Tovee M, Emery J, Cohen-Tovee E (2000) The estimation of body mass index and physical attractiveness is dependent on the observer's own body mass index. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 267: 1987–1997.
  16. 16. George HR, Cornelissen PL, Hancock PJ, Kiviniemi VV, Toveé MJ (2011) Differences in eye-movement patterns between anorexic and control observers when judging body size and attractiveness. British Journal of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712610X524291
  17. 17. Cornelissen PL, Toveé MJ, Bateson M (2009) Patterns of subcutaneous fat deposition and the relationship between body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio: Implications for models of physical attractiveness. Journal of Theoretical Biology 256: 343–350.
  18. 18. UK Department of Health (2008) Health Survey for England for 2008 (Economic and Social Data service). Available: http://www.ic.nhs.uk/statistics-and-data-collections. Accessed 19 Oct 2011.
  19. 19. Platek SM, Singh D (2010) Optimal Waist-to-Hip Ratios in Women Activate Neural Reward Centers in Men. PLoS ONE 5: e9042.
  20. 20. Rilling J, Kaufman T, Smith E, Patel R, Worthman C (2009) Abdominal depth and waist circumference as influential determinants of human female attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior 30: 21–31.
  21. 21. Tovee M, Maisey D, Emery J, Cornelissen P (1999) Visual cues to female physical attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 266: 211–218.
  22. 22. Bennett CM, Wolford GL, Miller MB (2009) The principled control of false positives in neuroimaging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4: 417 -422:
  23. 23. Tovee M, Cornelissen P (1999) The mystery of female beauty. Nature 399: 215–216.
  24. 24. World Medical Association (2008) WMA Declaration of Helsinki - Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects.. Available: http://www.wma.net/en/30publications/10policies/b3/. Accessed 16 Jun 2011.
  25. 25. Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, Evans A, et al. (2007) Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum. Brain Mapp 28: 1194–1205.
  26. 26. Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging. Thieme.
  27. 27. Lancaster J, Woldorff M, Parsons L, Liotti M, Freitas E, et al. (2000) Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human Brain Mapping 10: 120–131.
  28. 28. Astafiev SV, Stanley CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2004) Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions. Nature Neuroscience 7: 542–548.
  29. 29. Downing PE, Jiang Y, Shuman M, Kanwisher N (2001) A Cortical Area Selective for Visual Processing of the Human Body. Science 293: 2470–2473.
  30. 30. Koyama M, Hasegawa I, Osada T, Adachi Y, Nakahara K, et al. (2004) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of macaque monkeys performing visually guided saccade tasks: Comparison of cortical eye fields with humans. Neuron 41: 795–807.
  31. 31. Breiter H, Gollub R, Weisskoff R, Kennedy D, Makris N, et al. (1997) Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion. Neuron 19: 591–611.
  32. 32. Stein E, Pankiewicz J, Harsch H, Cho J, Fuller S, et al. (1998) Nicotine-induced limbic cortical activation in the human brain: A functional MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry 155: 1009–1015.
  33. 33. Koepp M, Gunn R, Lawrence A, Cunningham V, Dagher A, et al. (1998) Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature 393: 266–268.
  34. 34. Elliott R, Sahakian B, Michael A, Paykel E, Dolan R (1998) Abnormal neural response to feedback on planning and guessing tasks in patients with unipolar depression. Psychological Medicine 28: 559–571.
  35. 35. Tovée MJ, Maisey DS, Emery JL, Cornelissen PL (1999) Visual cues to female physical attractiveness. Proceedings of The Royal Society, Lond. B 266: 211–218.
  36. 36. Tovée MJ, Cornelissen PL (2001) Female and male perceptions of female physical attractiveness in front-view and profile. British Journal of Psychology 92: 391–402.
  37. 37. Smith KL, Cornelissen PL, Tovee MJ (2007) Color 3D bodies and judgments of human female attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior 28: 48–54.
  38. 38. Toveé MJ, Edmonds L, Vuong QC (2011) Categorical Perception of Human Female Physical Attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.008
  39. 39. Crawford C, Smith M, Krebs D, editors. Buss DM (n.d.) Sex differences in human mate selection criteria: An evolutionary perspective. Sociobiology and psychology: Ideas, issues and application. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 335–351.
  40. 40. Peelen MV, Downing PE (2007) The neural basis of visual body perception. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 636–648.
  41. 41. Taylor JC, Wiggett AJ, Downing PE (2007) Functional MRI analysis of body and body part representations in the extrastriate and fusiform body areas. J Neurophysiol 98: 1626–33.
  42. 42. Puhl RM, Boland FJ (2001) Predicting female physical attractiveness: Waist-to-hip ratio versus thinness. Psychology, Evolution & Gender 3: 27–46.
  43. 43. Swami V, Caprario C, Tovée MJ, Furnham A (2006) Female physical attractiveness in Britain and Japan: A cross-cultural study. European Journal of Personality 2006 20: 69–81.
  44. 44. Tovée MJ, Reinhardt S, Emery JL, Cornelissen PL (1998) Optimum body-mass index and maximum sexual attractiveness. Lancet 352: 548.
  45. 45. George HR, Swami V, Cornelissen PL, Tovée MJ (2008) Preferences for body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio do not vary with observer age. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology 6: 207–218.
  46. 46. Fan J, Dai W, Qian X, Chau KP, Liu Q (2007) Effects of shape parameters on the attractiveness of a female body. Perceptual and Motor Skills 105: 117–132.
  47. 47. Swami V, Tovée MJ (2005) Female physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia: A cross-cultural study. Body Image 2: 115–128.
  48. 48. Swami V, Knight D, MJ T, Davies P, Furnham A (2007) Preferences for female body size in Britain and the South Pacific. Britain and the South Pacific. Body Image 2007 4: 219–223.
  49. 49. Tovée MJ, Swami V, Furnham A, Mangalparsad R (2006) Changing perceptions of attractiveness as observers are exposed to a different culture. Evolution & Human Behavior 27: 443–456.
  50. 50. Manson JE, Willett WC, Stampfer MJ, Colditz GA, Hunter DJ, et al. (1995) Body weight and mortality among women. New England Journal of Medicine 333: 677–685.
  51. 51. Willet WC, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Colditz GA, Rosner B, et al. (1995) Weight, weight change and coronary heart disease in women: Risk within the normal weight range. Journal of the American Medical Association 273: 461–465.
  52. 52. Frisch RE (1988) Fatness and fertility. Scientific American 258: 88–95.
  53. 53. Lake JK, Power C, Cole TJ (1997) Women's reproductive health: The role of body mass index in early and adult life. International Journal of Obesity 21: 432–438.
  54. 54. Reid RL, Van_Vugt DA (1987) Weight related changes in reproductive function. Fertility & Sterility 48: 905–913.
  55. 55. Wang JX, Davies M, Norman RJ (2000) Body mass and probability of pregnancy during assisted reproduction to treatment: retrospective study. The Lancet 321: 1320–1321.
  56. 56. Penton-Voak IS, Little AC, Jones BC, Burt DM, Tiddeman BP, et al. (2003) Female condition influences preferences for sexual dimorphism in faces of male humans (Homo sapiens. Journal of Comparative Psychology 117: 264–271.
  57. 57. Pawlowski B, Jasienska G (2008) Women's body morphology and preferences for sexual partners' characteristics. Evolution and Human Behaviour 29: 19–25.
  58. 58. Smith FG, Jones BC, Welling LLW, Little AC, Vukovic J, et al. (2009) Waist-hip ratio predicts women's preferences for masculine male faces, but not perceptions of men's trustworthiness. Personality and Individual Differences 47: 476–480.
  59. 59. Delgado M, Nystrom L, Fissell C, Noll D, Fiez J (2000) Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology 84: 3072–3077.
  60. 60. Vartanian O, Goel V (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for paintings. Neuroreport 15: 893–897.
  61. 61. Baxter L, Phelps M, Mazziota J, Schwartz J, Gerner R, et al. (1985) Cerebral Metabolic Rates for Glucose in Mood Disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 42: 441–447.
  62. 62. Drevets W, Videen T, Price J, Preskorn S, Carmichael S, et al. (1992) A Functional Anatomical Study of Unipolar Depression. Journal of Neuroscience 12: 3628–3641.
  63. 63. American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
  64. 64. Polivy J, Herman CP (2002) Causes of eating disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol 53: 187–213.
  65. 65. Treasure J, Claudino AM, Zucker N (2010) Eating Disorders. Lancet 375: 583–593.