Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Incubation Patterns in a Central-Place Forager Affect Lifetime Reproductive Success: Scaling of Patterns from a Foraging Bout to a Lifetime

  • Akiko Shoji ,

    akikosho@gmail.com

    Affiliations Department of Biology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada, National Wildlife Research Centre, Environment Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada

  • Kyle H. Elliott,

    Affiliation Department of Zoology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada

  • Stéphane Aris-Brosou,

    Affiliation Department of Biology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada

  • Doug Crump,

    Affiliation National Wildlife Research Centre, Environment Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada

  • Anthony J. Gaston

    Affiliations Department of Biology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada, National Wildlife Research Centre, Environment Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada

Abstract

Background

Long-lived seabirds face a conflict between current and lifelong reproductive success. During incubation shifts, egg neglect is sometimes necessary to avoid starvation, but may compromise the current reproductive attempt. However, factors underlying this decision process are poorly understood. We focus on the ancient murrelet, Synthliboramphus antiquus, an alcid with exceptionally long incubation shift lengths, and test the impact of environmental factors on incubation shift length in relation to reproductive success.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Using an information theoretic approach, we show that incubation shift length was a strong predictor of reproductive success for ancient murrelets at Reef Island, Haida Gwaii, British Columbia, Canada during the 2007 and 2008 breeding seasons. The most important factors explaining an individual's shift length were egg size, wind speed and the length of the mate's previous shift. Wind speed and tide height were the two most important factors for determining foraging behavior, as measured by dive frequency and depth.

Conclusions/Significance

Our study demonstrates that (i) species-specific reproductive strategies interact with environmental conditions such as wind speed to form multiple incubation patterns and (ii) maintaining regular incubation shifts is an essential component of reproductive success.

Introduction

Life-history theory predicts that fitness is optimized by balancing investment in current reproduction with costs of reducing an individual's ability to invest in future reproduction [1]. One set of life-history decisions that birds face during the breeding season is the timing, frequency, and duration of their visits to the nest [2]. These decisions are reflected by the activities of individual birds, who try to maintain body condition and minimize predation risk [3], with the ultimate goal of maximizing reproductive fitness.

Most seabirds have a biparental incubation strategy and therefore alternate fasting bouts with foraging trips to replenish their body reserves [4], [5], [6]. Seabirds undergo large variations in body mass during their breeding period [7], [8], [9] and the rate of breeding failure can be high during the incubation period [10]. Maintaining incubation shift length in seabirds is critical and this is especially true for ancient murrelets Synthliboramphus antiquus which do not feed their chicks at the nest. Therefore, unlike nearly all other seabirds, incubation in this species is expected to be the most demanding phase of breeding. Nevertheless, most studies have focused on behaviour during the nestling period [11], [12], [13], [14], [15], [16], while incubation shifts have received less attention.

Interruptions to incubation occur routinely in many species of birds [17]. Such interruptions, when normal incubation behavior is resumed, are known as “egg neglect”. Temporary egg neglect may be observed during successful breeding (e.g. northern fulmar Fulmarus glacialis [18], blue petrels Halobaena caerulea [19], Cassin's auklet Ptychoramphus aleuticus [16]), but egg neglect is also known to increase the probability of breeding failure [19]. The ultimate manifestation of egg neglect, desertion (incubation is terminated permanently), is itself presumed to be the consequence of life-history costs and benefits [20]. One of the determinants of nest desertion is the length of foraging trips [15], as there may be a physiological limit beyond which an incubating bird will abandon the nest when its partner has been out foraging for “too long” [19], [21], [22], [23]. Therefore, it is important for a particular bird to decide on how long it can remain out at sea foraging so that its partner does not leave the nest. This reasoning suggests that incubation shift length should be synchronized between pair members to match to their partners' ability to fast [24], [25].

It has been suggested that both colony attendance and diving behavior are influenced by unusually poor foraging conditions such as high wind speed [3] and small tidal currents [26], and that egg neglect may be an indirect consequence of these factors. In seabirds, trip duration may be prolonged due to unpredictable weather conditions and patchy prey distribution [27], [28] (but see [12]). Indeed, longer trips may allow for longer search times and for more encounters with rarer but energetically more valuable prey items or patches [29], [30], [31], [32], which allow a particular foraging bird to have a longer shift. As a result, the incubating partner has to fast for an extended period of time, which depletes its body reserves [19], [33], and can lead to egg neglect. On the other hand, when feeding conditions are good, birds are expected to spend less time away from their nest. We will refer to the idea that incubation shift duration is determined primarily by foraging conditions as the “foraging hypothesis”.

However, incubation behavior may also be influenced by predation risk [16], [34], [35]. While many seabirds are diurnal, the majority of petrels (e.g. Procellariidae, Hydrobatidae) and some auks (Alcidae) that are vulnerable to predation on land, are nocturnal on their breeding grounds. Previous studies have suggested that nocturnal colony attendance among these seabirds is an adaptation to minimize predation risk from diurnal avian predators [36], [37], [38]. Nocturnal seabirds reduce nighttime activity and alter the timing of their visit to the colony on moonlit nights, supporting the idea that nocturnal visitation reduces predation risk [3], [39]. Despite nocturnal colony attendance, ancient murrelets are frequently killed by predators on their breeding grounds, presumably while returning to or departing from their nests [40]. Reducing the frequency of colony visits by extending incubation shift lengths in nocturnal, burrow-nesting marine birds is therefore expected to be an adaptive strategy further reducing their mortality from predators. Under this hypothesis parents may adjust the number of visits during the nesting season in order to maximize their life-time fitness [40]. We will refer to this hypothesis explaining incubation shift duration as the “predation hypothesis”.

In this study, we test these two non-mutually exclusive hypotheses, foraging and predation, as determinants of incubation behavior, with a particular emphasis on the duration of shifts, in the ancient murrelet. During incubation, adults nest in underground burrows and visit the breeding colony only at night. Members of this genus frequently neglect their eggs for a few days, and their mean incubation shifts are long (mean  = 3 days, range  = 1–6 days, [40]) compared to other members of the auk family. Yet, the reason for this difference in incubation shift length is unclear [40], [41], [42], [43]. Here, we first assess the extent to which environmental factors explain variation in shift lengths observed in murrelets, by testing whether longer foraging bouts increase the probability of egg neglect by their partner and whether this result affects reproductive success. Based on available information from previous studies, we predict that incubation shift length, therefore also egg neglect, should increase with: (i) increasing wind speed [44], and (ii) increasing nighttime light intensity [45]. We also predict that shift length should be affected by synchronization of incubation shift length between partners [46], which in turn should affect reproductive success. We show that our predictions hold true in ancient murrelets at our particular study site, which provides us with a novel understanding of the proximate factors influencing the incubation shift length in a seabird and of the importance of environmental conditions on reproductive success.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

All field procedures were approved by the Animal Care Committee of the National Wildlife Research Centre and the Ontario Region of Environment Canada operating under the guidelines of the Canadian Committee for Animal Care (Permit Number 0700AG02, 0800AG02, 0900AG02).

Field methods

The study was conducted on Reef Island (52°52′N, 131°31′W), Haida Gwaii, British Columbia, Canada. The experimental part of the study was conducted during April 5th to June 29th 2007, and April 4th to June 12th 2008. Ten days of additional fieldwork were carried out in May 2009 to obtain further data on diving behavior. The Reef Island colony supports approximately 5,000 pairs of ancient murrelets [47]. General monitoring procedures for nest attendance patterns in murrelets were followed [17]. The majority of burrows are in soft soil susceptible to damage by investigators. Consequently, artificial nest boxes were used to monitor incubation behavior (n = 72; see [48] for details). These boxes were installed ten years prior to the study, in 1997. The total number of occupied boxes (at least one egg laid) was 58 in 2007 and 61 in 2008. Normal clutch size in this species is two and eggs were marked to identify laying order. Their length and width was measured to calculate the egg volume index (Length × Width2) as a proxy for egg volume.

We set up knock-down tags, made of small twigs placed at the entrance of burrow nests. This technique is widely used to monitor seabirds' activity in and out of their burrow. Nest boxes were inspected only when tags were displaced [49]. In spite of their wide use in studies of burrow nesting seabirds [40], knock-down tags do not allow us to distinguish individuals, or to tell which sex is coming in or out of the borrow. However, knock-down tags provide a reliable index of nest attendance patterns, with an accuracy estimated to be 79% (range: 61–96%) [43], [49]. Once the first egg was laid, either a temperature sensor (Onset TMC1HD) or a YSI temperature probe (Yellow Springs Instruments; 400 series) was inserted into the nest chamber so that progress of incubation could be monitored (i.e. presence or absence of incubating birds). The temperature sensors were connected to an electronic recorder (Onset H08-006-04) and downloaded daily. The YSI temperature probe was connected to a telethermometer and read once daily, as ancient murrelets only enter or exit their burrows at night [40]. We secured the tips of the temperature probes in the nest cup with thumbtacks fixed onto the floor of the box during the daytime, prior to the laying of the second egg, when birds were not present. In 2008, we attached 17 transmitters randomly to one partner of each of 17 pairs. Nest attendance patterns were checked once daily during the daytime. At each check, shift changes were scored as either “change” or “no change”. The knock-down method indicates “change” when tags at the entrance have been displaced. The radio telemetry method indicates “change” when radio signals have appeared or disappeared (because only one member of each pair was fitted with a transmitter). Otherwise the shift status was scored as “no change”.

In 2007, vocalization activities were recorded by inserting a microphone attached to an mp3 recorder into occupied nest boxes (n = 10). Recorders were operated from 10:00 pm to 05:00 am, the period when changeovers may occur (time was recorded as PDT - Pacific Daylight Time). An immediate burst of chatter-calling by both members of the pair signaled the arrival of the foraging or ‘off-duty’ bird; we used this signal to determine when the off-duty bird returned to the nest.

After 30 days of incubation, at least one bird per nest was weighed and 1 ml of blood was sampled and stored on protein-saver paper for genetic sex determination using a chelex DNA extraction technique [50] and the P2/P8 Polymerase Chain Reaction method [51]. In cases where only one pair member was sexed, the other pair member was assumed to be of the opposite sex. Handling times were kept <3 min.

To measure the number, duration and depths of dives during foraging trips, incubating birds were captured during daytime in their nest box and equipped with cylindrical time depth recorders (TDRs). Between April 27th and May 20th 2008, two birds were fitted with TDRs (Lotek 1100 LTD; sampling interval  = 3 s; memory  = 128 Kb [55 hours]; weight  = 5 g; diameter  = 1 cm; length  = 3.3 cm; accuracy  =  ±2 m; [52]). Between April 28th and May 12th 2009, four birds were equipped with the same Lotek 1100 LTD TDRs and eight with the lighter and more accurate Lotek 1500 LAT (sampling interval  = 4 s with 1 s sampling when below 2 m for murrelets; memory  = 512 Kb; weight  = 3.2 g; diameter  = 0.5 cm; length  = 3.3 cm; precision  =  ±0.25 m). We used adhesive tape to attach each device to the tarsus, without any additional metal band. Due to device measurement uncertainty, only depths >2 m were considered to be actual dives. To minimize any bias associated with the daily light cycle and because there were virtually no dives during nighttime, all dives between 22:00 to 04:00 were excluded, as murrelets are diurnal feeders [53], [54].

Weather conditions and ambient light

Hourly wind data were obtained from the closest weather station (Sandspit, 40 km to the north, www.weatheroffice.gc.ca). A previous study showed that high wind speeds decrease the ability of ancient murrelets at Reef Island to forage at sea at various time scales [3]. Thus, we used mean wind speeds over two different time scales (6 h and 72 h prior to a particular night) to assess the effect of foraging conditions on incubation shift length. The 6 h period preceding arrival at the colony corresponds to the bird's return journey from the foraging area, as birds begin to arrive at the colony several hours before dusk [40]; 72 h durations correspond to the length of typical foraging trips (modal incubation shift length at present study site 2–3 days, Fig. 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Distribution of incubation shift lengths of ancient murrelets at Reef Island measured by radio-telemetry at the artificial nest boxes in the study plot in 2008 (male: n = 61, female: n = 58).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.g001

Hourly wave height data were obtained from the North Hecate Strait buoy station (www.dfo-mpo.gc.ca), which is the closest station recording such data, about 91 km away NE from our study site (www.movable-type.co.uk/scripts/latlong.html). Tide height was obtained from the Queen Charlotte City station (www.waterlevels.gc.ca). We further obtained lunar phase (or moon age; www.timeanddate.com) and daily cloud opacity (measured in tenths of the outdoor light; www.weatheroffice.gc.ca).

Effects of partner's behavior

We used the knock-down method described above to monitor incubation shift length of each partner: we considered a changeover to have occurred when tags were knocked down [49]. Comparisons of incubation shift lengths within pair members within a given year were made using the Spearman correlation coefficient as incubation shift length departed from normality.

Reproductive success

After 30 days of incubation, the reproductive success of each pair was checked [55]. Forty-one of 58 and 35 of 61 pairs produced at least one chick to departure in 2007 and 2008, respectively. Eggs depredated by deer mice (the only mammalian predator present on the island) were easily distinguished from their hatched counterparts by tooth marks on shell remains [56], [57].

Data analysis

We performed all analysis with R 2.6.1 [58]. In this study, we used either generalized linear models (GLZ) to model adult nest arrival and egg neglect, or generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) to model incubation shift length, reproductive success, dive depth and dive frequency. Model fit was checked with diagnostic scatter plots, using standard residuals, and transformations were applied where necessary.

The model-building procedure employed a multimodel inference approach [59], [60] to address hypotheses about temporal and weather-related patterns in shift length. [60], [61]. We considered all of the models that were biologically reasonable, which involved various combinations of the different independent variables. In all cases detailed below, models were ranked with AIC and ΔAIC was used to infer support for models in the candidate set [60], [61].

We modeled the proportion of parents returning to their nests and the proportion of eggs neglected (i.e. left unincubated for a period of at least 24 h) as a function of weather conditions. As these are proportion data, we used a Binomial distribution with a logit link function.

For incubation shift length, we assumed that egg size (Egg Volume Index) had an overarching effect on shift length because egg size is often related to female age and quality [42], [62]. Besides, if energetic limitation is important, we expect that the length of the previous shift would influence shift length. We therefore included this second variable in the models. We then tested whether timing of hatch influenced shift lengths by adding Julian date of hatch to the model. We controlled for site effects by considering nest identifiers (ID) as random effects. To model the effect that weather might play in day-to-day adjustment of incubation shift length, we added measurements of wind speed at two different times (6 and 72 h before nest arrival) and of visibility (moon or cloud). Finally, we tested the hypothesis that variation in hatch date and egg size modifies the relationship between previous shift length and shift length by including the corresponding interaction terms in our models. In total, we considered 10 candidate models for incubation shift length.

The models for reproductive success were specified to test the following hypotheses. First, we assumed that egg size influences reproductive success (see previous paragraph). Mean shift length was added to the models to test the effect of schedule coordination between pair members and year effect was added to test the effect of intra seasonal variation in reproductive success. Nest-specific correlation coefficients of incubation shift length between pair members (synchrony) and cumulative days of egg neglect were added to test those effects on reproductive success. Location effect was controlled by modeling nest ID as a random effect. Finally, we tested the hypothesis that variation in egg size and year modifies the relationship between mean shift length and nest specific correlation coefficients, and reproductive success by including the corresponding interaction terms in our models. In total, we considered 11 candidate models for reproductive success.

To test if dive depth frequency was related to weather conditions, we used similar GLZ modeling with Poisson distribution and a log link function. We considered wind speed and direction (circularly transformed), wave height, tide change (current tide height minus tide height one hour prior), tidal amplitude (difference between high and low tide) and time of day (circularly transformed) as independent variables. Individual identity was controlled by modeling this factor as a random effect. As weather might affect hour-to-hour adjustment of diving behavior at sea, we considered hourly weather data. Unless otherwise indicated, the results are expressed as means ± 1 SD and all reported P-values are two-tailed.

Results

Timing of nest arrivals

The exact timing of returns to the colony, as documented by recordings of vocalizations, was only available for 2007. In this year, the average time of nest arrivals was earlier during the incubation period (23:29, n = 136, mean incubation period: 10 May; range: 20 April-31 May) than during the pre-incubation period (00:17, n = 47; mean pre-incubation period: 19 April; 12 April-1 May, t = 6.89, P<0.0001). During the incubation period, the first nest arrivals on each night occurred about two hours after nautical twilight, and as the time of sunset changed throughout the season, the timing of first arrival was slightly but significantly correlated with calendar date (r2 = 0.15, P<0.0001). In contrast, timing of arrival was not significantly correlated with the calendar date during the pre-incubation period (r2 = 0.07, P = 0.08). This result suggests that the time of nest arrival during the incubation period was regulated by light conditions, but that this was less so during the pre-incubation period. Based on sound recording data, there was no significant difference in nest arrival time among individual nests (Kruskal-Wallis test: KW = 13.4, P = 0.20).

Adult nest arrival

The best model predicting the proportion of adults returning to their nests on a given night included wind speed and visibility (Table 1). Arrival rate was inversely related to wind speed during the 72 h prior to nest arrival, which suggests that foraging conditions at sea have a strong effect on the timing of return to the nest. Likewise, wind speed during the 6 h prior to nest arrival decreased adult nest arrival, which may interfere with navigation while returning to the colony. A similar effect was found for increasing lunar phase, while cloud opacity had no effect on arrival rate. These results suggest that more birds arrived at their nests when the ambient light intensity was low, which is consistent with the predation hypothesis.

thumbnail
Table 1. Results of AIC model comparison to explain adult nest arrivals and egg neglect of ancient murrelets.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.t001

Egg neglect

The model receiving greatest support suggested that the proportion of eggs neglected on a given night was related to the full model with wind speed, moon phase and cloud opacity (Table 1). Egg neglect increased with wind speed during the previous 6 h and 72 h prior to neglect. Cloud opacity decreased the proportion of egg neglect while ambient light conditions were positively correlated with the proportion of egg neglect. These results suggest that egg neglect is likely to be affected by both foraging conditions and risk of adult predation.

Incubation shift length for known sex pairs

Of the 13 pairs for which one partner was equipped with a radio transmitter in 2008, incubation shift length did not differ significantly between males and females (female: 2.60±0.88 days, n = 58; male: 2.38±0.82 days, n = 61; t = −1.45, P = 0.15, Fig. 1). The mean duration of 119 shifts measured by radio telemetry was 2.49 days, and 56% lasted three days or more.

Mate effects on shift length

As predicted, incubation shift length was positively correlated with the duration of the previous shift by the partner (r = 0.29, P<0.05, n = 1385). In addition, the mean incubation shift length throughout incubation was positively correlated with that of the partner (r = 0.64, P<0.0001, n = 75, Fig. 2). These results were robust to the experimental technique used (knock-down tags), as telemetry also suggested a correlation of incubation shift length between alternating partners at a given nest (r = 0.33, P<0.05, n = 13). Mean incubation shift length differed among nests (KW = 259, P<0.0001).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Incubation shift length was correlated between pair members in ancient murrelets (n = 75).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.g002

Factors affecting incubation shift lengths

The multimodel inference approach used here suggested that incubation shift length was influenced by the egg volume index, partner's previous shift, timing of breeding, and wind speed. We also found support for interactions among variables in these best main-effects models (Table 2). Removing short-term wind speed (6 h) increased AIC by >13 units, while removal of long-term wind speed (72 h) increased AIC by 5 units. Removal of the partner's previous shift and timing of breeding led to an increase of >26 units (see Table 2), suggesting that those variables strongly affect incubation shift length.

thumbnail
Table 2. Results of AIC model comparison to explain incubation shift length and reproductive success of murrelets.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.t002

Effects of incubation shift lengths on reproductive success

The number of chicks (0, 1 or 2) that departed per pair (n = 41 in 2007, n = 35 in 2008) was best predicted by egg volume index, nest specific correlation coefficients (pair synchrony) and days of egg neglected. This model included nest ID as a random effect to account for individual heterogeneity in reproductive success. Reproductive success tended to increase with pair synchrony and egg size (1.68±0.65 chicks/pair in 2007; 1.23±0.94 chicks/pair in 2008). We also found support for interactions of differed between years and egg size, and mean incubation shift length. Egg size of non-deserting pairs was larger than that of deserting pairs (n = 133, 27 respectively, t = 2.18, P = 0.03), as previously found [42]. We also found support for interactions among variables of shift length and year in these best main-effect models.

Foraging behavior

Little variation was found for hourly dive depth (mean  = 8.5 m, SE = 0.24, n = 327) and for dive frequency (mean  = 25.8, SE = 1.29, n = 327). The best model describing hourly maximum depth included wind speed, tide change and time of day (Table 3). Maximum dive depth per hour decreased with wind speed (Table 3). Larger tidal amplitude increased dive depth and dive frequency (Table 3, Fig. 3 and 4). Any model of maximum dive depth disregarding the effects of tide and time of day increased ΔAIC by an order of magnitude (from 7.4 to >70; Table 3), suggesting that both tide and time were important factors in this model. The best model of hourly dive frequency included wind speed, wave height, wind directions and tidal amplitude (ΔAIC  = 0.0, deviance  = 4623.3). Dive frequency increased with wind speed (Fig. 5), wave height and tidal amplitude (Table 3). Both dive depth and frequency were approximately constant outside of the periods of twilight or darkness (2100-0600), but declined rapidly to zero during the night, driving the strong relationship between those variables and time of day (Fig. 6) Dive frequency was slightly higher when wind was from the north.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Relationship between natural logarithm (ln) hourly mean dive frequency and ln hourly maximum dive depth (n = 337).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.g003

thumbnail
Figure 4. Diving behavior and tidal amplitude for ancient murrelets at Reef Island (n = 337).

(A) Hourly maximum dive depth; (B) dive frequency increased with tidal amplitude. Data are presented as mean ± SE.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.g004

thumbnail
Figure 5. Hourly dive frequency increased with hourly mean wind speed (m/s) for ancient murelets at Reef Island (n = 337).

Data are presented as mean ± SE.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.g005

thumbnail
Figure 6. Distritbution of hourly maximum dive depth as function of time of day (n = 337).

Data are presented as mean ± SE.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.g006

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of AIC model comparison to explain hourly maximum dive depth and dive frequency of ancient murrelets.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017760.t003

Discussion

To better understand the exceptionally long incubation shift length observed in Synthliboramphus compared to other auks [40], [43], [63], we conducted the first intensive study of incubation shift length taken over a period of two consecutive years. Incubation shift length varied among individual pairs of ancient murrelets, and shift duration was strongly correlated between pair members. The synchronization of an array of reproductive behaviors during breeding, including shift lengths, is critical for many seabirds, as previously reported [12], [46], [64].

We found significant variation in shift length among pairs, which is consistent with previous studies [40], [43]. Pairs members had a strong tendency to remain at sea for similar periods when off-duty, so that their shift lengths were strongly correlated. Our results suggest that foraging conditions and individual variations (i.e. parental quality), rather than predator avoidance, were the most important determinants of incubation shift length in ancient murrelets (Table 3). Variation in shift length among individual pairs could result from a variety of individual traits, including body condition and age. Body condition in particular is known to be a critical factor affecting incubation shift lengths in seabirds [12], [15], [19]. In this respect, long foraging trips usually increase energy intake for central-place foragers [29], [31], [32] and that could also apply to ancient murrelets [53]. However, long foraging trips require that partners have the physiological capacity to fast for an extended period of time, as the foraging time of one bird translates directly into fasting time for its partner. A pair that begins incubation with plentiful reserves should be capable of longer shifts than those starting with smaller reserves [65].

The incubation shift of one bird needs to be synchronized with the foraging shift of its mate so that the incubating bird does not depart before its mate returns, leaving the egg attended. This poor coordination of incubation shift length can increase the rate of breeding failure in seabirds [66]. There have been several proposals for explaining how pairs synchronize their behavior. Birds may tend to mate assortatively with respect to age and therefore experience. As a result, “high quality” (i.e., experienced) birds may choose high quality mates to avoid poor synchronization and reduce the need to compensate for a partner who requires more time at sea [66]. Although periods of uniform weather could cause successive incubation shifts to be similar in length, this cannot account for correlations in shift length over the entire incubation period, because there seems no reason why such synchronization would persist if weather changes.

In addition to the duration of the partner's previous shift, high wind speed was found to increase incubation shift length of ancient murrelets. This result is consistent with previous studies, which suggested that high wind speed led to poor foraging conditions at sea, and hence to longer shift lengths [16], [67], [68]. Dive frequency increased with wave height and wind speed, suggesting that birds need more foraging effort during rough seas that result from high wind speeds. Thus, ancient murrelets are likely to have lower foraging success and higher energy demands under unfavorable weather conditions, which leads to longer foraging trips and, from the partner's point of view, longer incubation shifts.

Tide may also affect foraging behavior [26], [68]. In some cases, tidal currents are strong enough to affect avian dive behavior by increasing travel costs [69], [70], [71]. In most cases, however, tide affects avian foraging behavior by altering the behavior or the abundance of prey [72]. Because plankton is susceptible to tidal currents, tide is expected to affect particularly the foraging of planktivorous species. Hence, it is not surprising that planktivores, including ancient murrelets, are often found in areas of tidal upwelling and are thought to follow the temporal progression of tidal changes [73]. Cassin's auklets changed diet and dove deeper during spring tides [26], while ancient murrelets selected the strongest tidal currents and fed more frequently during maximum tidal flow that at slack tides [74]. Because dive depth was the highest at around noon [Fig 6], it is likely that our birds followed the diel vertical migration of plankton to deeper depths during the middle of the day, as visibility should have been high throughout the time period examined [75].

On the other hand, ambient light affected nest arrival rate, and therefore egg neglect, but not incubation shift length, with older (brighter) moon decreasing nest arrival. Presumably, birds reduced nest visits under the threat of predation [76], [77]. This behavioral response to ambient light occurs in a number of nocturnal burrow-nesting seabirds, which are vulnerable on land [38], [39], [78], [79], [80]. Despite the effect of ambient light on nest arrival and egg neglect, light intensity did not affect incubation shift length. As the moon is at its brightest for only a few days every 28 days, the period of high predation risk is relatively short. Egg neglect is expected to occur due to a non-arriving partner and it is then likely that cloud opacity directly impacts predation at the colony. The influence of predation risk on incubation shift length depends on the length of these shifts; the threshold of light conditions that leads to increased predation risk is unknown and deserves further attention.

Overall, our results show that the foraging hypothesis and the predation avoidance hypothesis are not mutually exclusive. Seabirds have evolved a flexible approach to meet the requirements of their life-history strategy. If a bird decides not to return on a particular night due to predation risk, the on-duty bird may decide to leave the nest if its body reserves are critically depleted. However, these decisions are influenced by both foraging conditions at sea and by ambient light conditions. Reproductive success was related to incubation shift length, year, and egg size in ancient murrelets. Presumably, better quality or experienced birds [46] start their incubation shifts with higher reserves and consequently, are better able to accommodate variable foraging conditions [42]. Thus, better quality birds have higher reproductive success and longer shifts, which minimize predation risk. The idea that older birds have higher success is also supported by the relationship between egg size and desertion in this study (desertion probability inversely related to egg size), as older auks are known to lay larger eggs [81], [82].

Acknowledgments

We thank G. Martin, A. Egger, L. McKnight-Yeates, C. Pansino and Laskeek Bay Conservation Society for their hospitality in Haida Gwaii, BC. J. Pattison, M. Yoneda, J. Provencher, T. Vergoz, S Le Saout, K. Woo and S. Colantonio were able and excellent assistants. C. Eberl provided thoughtful logistic help. We thank P. Smith for assistance with study design and helpful suggestions on the data analysis. Ian L. Jones at the Memorial University of Newfoundland, Alan Burger at the University of Victoria and an anonymous reviewer provided valuable feedback on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AS AJG. Performed the experiments: AS KHE AJG. Analyzed the data: AS SA-B KHE. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AS DC. Wrote the paper: AS KHE SA-B DC AJG.

References

  1. 1. Stearns SC (1992) The evolution of life histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  2. 2. Mallory ML, Gaston AJ, Forbes MR, Gilchrist HG, Cheney B, et al. (2008) Flexible incubation rhythm in northern fulmars: a comparison between oceanographic zones. Marine Biology 154: 1031–1040.
  3. 3. Jones IL, Gaston AJ, Falls JB (1990) Factors affecting colony attendance of ancient murrelets Synthliboramphus antiquus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 433–441.
  4. 4. Chastel O, Weimerskirch H, Jouventin P (1995) Influence of body condition on reproductive decision and reproductive success in the blue petrel. Auk 112: 964–972.
  5. 5. Reid JM, Ruxton GD, Monaghan P, Hilton GM (2002) Energetic consequences of clutch temperature and clutch size for a uniparental intermittent incubator: The starling. Auk 119: 54–61.
  6. 6. Cresswell W, Holt S, Reid JM, Whitfield DP, Mellanby RJ, et al. (2004) The energetic costs of egg heating constrain incubation attendance but do not determine daily energy expenditure in the pectoral sandpiper. Behavioral Ecology 15: 498–507.
  7. 7. Weimerskirch H, Shaffer SA, Mabille G, Martin J, Boutard O, et al. (2002) Heart rate and energy expenditure of incubating wandering albatrosses: basal levels, natural variation, and the effects of human disturbance. Journal of Experimental Biology 205: 475–483.
  8. 8. Drent RH, Daan S (1980) The prudent parent - energetic adjustments in avian breeding. Ardea 68: 225–252.
  9. 9. Hatch JJ, Nisbet ICT (2011) Costs of incubation in common terns Sterna hirundo: replication and extension of a clutch-enlargement experiment. Ibis 153: 185–189.
  10. 10. Prince PA, Rothery P, Croxall JP, Wood AG (1994) Population dynamics of Black-browed and grey-headed albatrosses Diomedea melanophris and D.chrysostoma at Bird Island, South Georgia. Ibis 136: 50–71.
  11. 11. Hamer KC, Hill JK (1994) The regulation of food delivery to nestling Cory's shearwaters Calonectris diomedea: the roles of parents and offspring. Journal of Avian Biology 25: 198–204.
  12. 12. Weimerskirch H (1995) Regulation of foraging trips and incubation routine in male and female wandering albatrosses. Oecologia 102: 37–43.
  13. 13. Hamer KC, Thompson DR (1997) Provisioning and growth rates of nestling fulmars Fulmarus glacialis: Stochastic variation or regulation? Ibis 139: 31–39.
  14. 14. Gray CM, Brooke MDL, Hamer KC (2005) Repeatability of chick growth and food provisioning in manx shearwaters Puffinus puffinus. Journal of Avian Biology 36: 374–379.
  15. 15. Kato A, Ropert-Coudert Y, Chiaradia A (2008) Regulation of trip duration by an inshore forager, the little penguin Eudyptula minor, during incubation. Auk 125: 588–593.
  16. 16. Ronconi RA, Hipfner JM (2009) Egg neglect under risk of predation in Cassin's auklet Ptychoramphus aleuticus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 87: 415–421.
  17. 17. Gaston AJ, Powell DW (1989) Natural incubation, egg neglect, and hatchability in the ancient murrelet. Auk 106: 433–438.
  18. 18. Falk K, Moller S (1997) Breeding ecology of the fulmar Fulmarus glacialis and the Kittiwake Rissa tridactyla in high-arctic northeastern Greenland, 1993. Ibis 139: 270–281.
  19. 19. Chaurand T, Weimerskirch H (1994) Incubation routine, body mass regulation and egg neglect in the blue petrel Halobaena caerulea. Ibis 136: 285–290.
  20. 20. Guiguebo MF, Sealy SG (2010) Clutch abandonment by parasitized yellow warblers: egg burial or nest desertion? Condor 112: 399–406.
  21. 21. Boersma PD, Wheelwright NT (1979) Egg neglect in the Procellariiformes: reproductive adaptations in the fork-tailed storm-petrel. Condor 81: 157–165.
  22. 22. Gaston AJ, Hipfner JM (2006) Body mass changes in brunnich's guillemots Uria lomvia with age and breeding stage. Journal of Avian Biology 37: 101–109.
  23. 23. Gaston AJ, Hipfner JM (2006) Adult brunnich's guillemots Uria lomvia balance body condition and investment in chick growth. Ibis 148: 106–113.
  24. 24. Warham J (1990) The petrels: their ecology and breeding systems. London: Academic Press.
  25. 25. Bety J, Gauthier G, Giroux JF (2003) Body condition, migration, and timing of reproduction in snow geese: A test of the condition-dependent model of optimal clutch size. American Naturalist 162: 110–121.
  26. 26. Burger AE, Powell DW (1990) Diving depths and diet of Cassin's auklet at Reef Island, British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 1572–1577.
  27. 27. Lack DL (1968) Ecological adaptations for breeding in birds. London: Methuen.
  28. 28. Ashmole NP (1971) Seabird ecology and the marine environment. In: Farner DS, King JS, Parkes KC, editors. Avian biology VolI. New York: Academic Press. pp. 224–286.
  29. 29. Wanless S, Harris MP, Russell AF (1993) Factors influencing food-load sizes brought in by shags Phalacrocorax aristotelis during chick rearing. Ibis 135: 19–24.
  30. 30. Litzow MA, Piatt JF, Abookire AA, Robards MD (2004) Energy density and variability in abundance of pigeon guillemot prey: support for the quality-variability trade-off hypothesis. Journal of Animal Ecology 73: 1149–1156.
  31. 31. Elliott KH, Bull RD, Gaston AJ, Davoren GK (2009) Underwater and above-water search patterns of an Arctic seabird: reduced searching at small spatiotemporal scales. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63: 1773–1785.
  32. 32. Elliott KH, Woo KJ, Gaston AJ, Benvenuti S, Dall'Antonia L, et al. (2009) Central-place foraging in an Arctic seabird provides evidence for storer-Ashmole's halo. Auk 126: 613–625.
  33. 33. Tveraa T, Lorentsen SH, Saether BE (1997) Regulation of foraging trips and costs of incubation shifts in the antarctic petrel Thalassoica antarctica. Behavioral Ecology 8: 465–469.
  34. 34. Martin TE, Ghalambor CK (1999) Males feeding females during incubation. I. Required by microclimate or constrained by nest predation. American Naturalist 153: 131–139.
  35. 35. Smith PA, Gilchrist HG, Smith JNM (2007) Effects of nest habitat, food, and parental behavior on shorebird nest success. Condor 109: 15–31.
  36. 36. Cody ML (1973) Coexistence, coevolution and convergent evolution in seabird communities. Ecology 54: 31–44.
  37. 37. Harris S (1974) Status, chronology, and ecology of nesting storm petrels in northwestern California. Condor 76: 249–261.
  38. 38. Watanuki Y (1986) Moonlight avoidance behavior in leach's storm-petrels as a defense against slaty-backed gulls. Auk 103: 14–22.
  39. 39. Riou S, Hamer KC (2008) Predation risk and reproductive effort: impacts of moonlight on food provisioning and chick growth in manx shearwaters. Animal Behaviour 76: 1743–1748.
  40. 40. Gaston AJ (1992) The ancient murrelet: a natural history in the Queen Charlotte Islands. London: T & A D Poyser.
  41. 41. Gaston AJ (2004) Seabirds: a natural history. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  42. 42. Gaston AJ, Jones IL (1998) The auks: alcidae. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  43. 43. Sealy SG (1976) Biology of nesting ancient murrelets. Condor 78: 294–306.
  44. 44. Ojowski U, Eidtmann C, Furness RW, Garthe S (2001) Diet and nest attendance of incubating and chick-rearing northern fulmars Fulmarus glacialis in Shetland. Marine Biology 139: 1193–1200.
  45. 45. Addison B, Ydenberg RC, Smith BD (2007) Tufted puffins Fratercula cirrhata respond to predation danger during colony approach flights. Auk 124: 63–70.
  46. 46. DeForest LN, Gaston AJ (1996) The effect of age on timing of breeding and reproductive success in the thick-billed murre. Ecology 77: 1501–1511.
  47. 47. Gaston AJ (1990) Population parameters of the ancient murrelet. Condor 92: 998–1011.
  48. 48. Gaston AJ (1996) A nest box for ancient murrelets. Colonial Waterbirds 19: 116–120.
  49. 49. Shoji A, Gaston AJ (2010) Comparing methods for monitoring nest attendance in ancient murrelets. Waterbirds 260–263.
  50. 50. Egloff C, Labrosse A, Hebert C, Crump D (2009) A nondestructive method for obtaining maternal DNA from avian eggshells and its application to embryonic viability determination in herring gulls Larus argentatus. Molecular Ecology Resources 9: 19–27.
  51. 51. Griffiths R, Double MC, Orr K, Dawson RJG (1998) A DNA test to sex most birds. Molecular Ecology 7: 1071–1075.
  52. 52. Elliott KH, Gaston AJ (2009) Accuracy of depth recorders. Waterbirds 32: 183–191.
  53. 53. Sealy SG (1975) Feeding ecology of the ancient and marbled murrelets near Langara Island, British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology 53: 418–433.
  54. 54. Elliott KH, Shoji A, Campbell KL, Gaston AJ (2010) Oxygen stores and foraging behavior of two sympatric, planktivorous alcids. Aquatic Biology 8: 221–235.
  55. 55. Gaston AJ, Jones IL, Noble DG (1988) Monitoring ancient murrelet breeding populations. Colonial Waterbirds 11: 58–66.
  56. 56. Gaston AJ (1992) Annual survival of breeding Cassin's auklets in the Queen Charlotte Islands, British Columbia. Condor 94: 1019–1021.
  57. 57. Blight LK, Ryder JL, Bertram DF (1999) Predation on rhinoceros auklet eggs by a native population of Peromyscus. Condor 101: 871–876.
  58. 58. R Development Core Team (2009) A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austlia: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  59. 59. Smith PA, Bart J, Lanctot RB, McCaffery BJ, Brown S (2009) Probability of detection of nests and implications for survey design. Condor 111: 414–423.
  60. 60. Burnham KP, Anderson DR (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. New York: Springer.
  61. 61. Akaike H (1973) Information theory as an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In: Petrov BN, Csaki F, editors. Second international symposium on information theory. Budapest: Akademiai Kiado. pp. 267–281.
  62. 62. Amundsen T, Lorentsen SH, Tveraa T (1996) Effects of egg size and parental quality on early nestling growth: An experiment with the antarctic petrel. Journal of Animal Ecology 65: 545–555.
  63. 63. Gaston AJ, Shoji A (1994) Ancient murrelet Synthliboramphus antiquus. In: Pool AAFG, editor. The Birds of North America, No 132. Philadelphia: Washington, D.C.: The Academy of Natural Sciences. The American Ornithologists' Union.
  64. 64. Nisbet ICT, Dann P (2009) Reproductive performance of little penguins Eudyptula minor in relation to year, age, pair-bond duration, breeding date and individual quality. Journal of Avian Biology 40: 296–308.
  65. 65. Gaston AJ, Jones IL (1989) The relative importance of stress and programmed anorexia in determining mass loss by incubating ancient murrelets. Auk 106: 653–658.
  66. 66. Davis LS (1988) Coordination of incubation routines and mate choice in adélie penguins Pygoscelis adeliae. Auk 105: 428–432.
  67. 67. Birkhead TR (1976) Effects of sea conditions on rates at which guillemots feed chicks. British Birds 69: 490–492.
  68. 68. Finney SK, Wanless S, Harris MP (1999) The effect of weather conditions on the feeding behaviour of a diving bird, the common guillemot Uria aalge. Journal of Avian Biology 30: 23–30.
  69. 69. Wilson RP, Locca R, Scolaro JA, Laurenti S, Upton J, et al. (2001) Magellanic penguins Spheniscus magellanicus commuting through San Julian Bay; do current trends induce tidal tactics? Journal of Avian Biology 32: 83–89.
  70. 70. Heath JP, Gilchrist HG, Ydenberg RC (2006) Regulation of stroke pattern and swim speed across a range of current velocities: diving by common eiders wintering in polynyas in the Canadian Arctic. Journal of Experimental Biology 209: 3974–3983.
  71. 71. Heath JP, Gilchrist HG, Ydenberg RC (2007) Can dive cycle models predict patterns of foraging behaviour? Diving by common ieders in an Arctic polynya. Animal Behaviour 73: 877–884.
  72. 72. Heath JP, Montevecchi WA, Robertson GJ (2008) Allocating foraging effort across multiple time scales: Behavioral responses to environmental conditions by harlequin ducks wintering at Cape St. Mary's, Newfoundland. Waterbirds 31: 71–80.
  73. 73. Hunt GL, Russell RW, Coyle KO, Weingartner T (1998) Comparative foraging ecology of planktivorous auklets in relation to ocean physics and prey availability. Marine Ecology-Progress Series 167: 241–259.
  74. 74. Holm HJ, Burger AE (2002) Foraging behavior and resource partitioning by diving birds during winter in areas of strong tidal currents. Waterbirds 25: 312–325.
  75. 75. Lovvorn JR (2010) Modeling profitability for the smallest marine endotherms: auklets foraging within pelagic prey patches. Aquatic Biology 8: 203–219.
  76. 76. Dale S, Gustavsen R, Slagsvold T (1996) Risk taking during parental care: A test of three hypotheses applied to the pied flycatcher. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 39: 31–42.
  77. 77. Michl G, Torok J, Garamszegi LZ, Toth L (2000) Sex-dependent risk taking in the collared flycatcher, Ficedula albicollis, when exposed to a predator at the nestling stage. Animal Behaviour 59: 623–628.
  78. 78. Imber MJ (1975) Behaviour of petrels in relation to the moon and artificial lights. Notornis 22: 302–306.
  79. 79. Montgomerie RD, Weatherhead PJ (1988) Risks and rewards of nest defense by parent birds. Quarterly Review of Biology 63: 167–187.
  80. 80. Keitt BS, Tershy BR, Croll DA (2004) Nocturnal behavior reduces predation pressure on black-vented shearwaters Puffinus opisthomelas. Marine Ornithology 32: 173–178.
  81. 81. Hipfner JM, Gaston AJ, DeForest LN (1997) The role of female age in determining egg size and laying date of thick-billed murres. Journal of Avian Biology 28: 271–278.
  82. 82. Lloyd CS (1979) Factors affecting breeding of Razorbills Alca torda on Skokholm. Ibis 121: 165–176.