Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Recovery Sleep Reverses Impaired Response Inhibition due to Sleep Restriction: Evidence from a Visual Event Related Potentials Study

  • Xiao Jin ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Xiao Jin, Enmao Ye

    Affiliations Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, PR China, Cognitive and Mental Health Research Center, Beijing, PR China

  • Enmao Ye ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Xiao Jin, Enmao Ye

    Affiliations Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, PR China, Cognitive and Mental Health Research Center, Beijing, PR China

  • Jianlin Qi,

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Psychology, Air Force General Hospital, Beijing, PR China

  • Lubin Wang,

    Affiliations Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, PR China, Cognitive and Mental Health Research Center, Beijing, PR China

  • Yu Lei,

    Affiliations Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, PR China, Cognitive and Mental Health Research Center, Beijing, PR China

  • Pinhong Chen,

    Affiliations Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, PR China, Cognitive and Mental Health Research Center, Beijing, PR China

  • Guiyun Mi,

    Affiliations Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, PR China, Cognitive and Mental Health Research Center, Beijing, PR China

  • Feng Zou,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, The 215th Clinic of 406th Hospital of the Chinese People’s Liberation Army, Dalian, PR China

  • Yongcong Shao ,

    yangzhengchina@aliyun.com (ZY); budeshao@aliyun.com (YS)

    Affiliations Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, PR China, Cognitive and Mental Health Research Center, Beijing, PR China

  • Zheng Yang

    yangzhengchina@aliyun.com (ZY); budeshao@aliyun.com (YS)

    Affiliations Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, PR China, Cognitive and Mental Health Research Center, Beijing, PR China

Abstract

Objective

To investigate response inhibition after total sleep deprivation (TSD) and the restorative effects of one night of recovery sleep (RS).

Methods

Fourteen healthy male participants performed a visual Go/NoGo task, and electroencephalogram recordings were conducted at five time points: (1) baseline, (2) after 12 h of TSD, (3) after 24 h of TSD, (4) after 36 h of TSD, and (5) following 8 h of RS. The dynamic changes in response inhibition during TSD and after 8 h of RS were investigated by examining the NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 event-related potential components.

Results

Compared with baseline, NoGo-P3 amplitudes were decreased, while the NoGo-N2 latency increased along with the awake time prolonged. NoGo anteriorization, which was minimized after 24 h of TSD, progressively decreased with increasing TSD. After 8 h of RS, recoveries of both the NoGo-P3 amplitude and NoGo-N2 latency in the prefrontal cortex were observed compared with the values after 36 h of TSD.

Conclusion

TSD induced a dose-dependent functional decline in the response inhibition of NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 on prefrontal cortex activation, and 8 h of RS resulted in recovery or maintenance of the response inhibition. However, it was not restored to baseline levels.

Limitations

Participants were chosen male college students only, thus the findings cannot be generalized to older people and women. Additionally, the sample size was small, and, thus, speculations on the meaning of the results of this study should be cautious. The EEG continuous recording should be employed to monitor the decline of alertness following TSD.

Introduction

Response inhibition plays an important role in preventing a person from engaging in a preponderant response when that reaction is not appropriate, which is very crucial for flexible performance and goal-oriented behaviors that are based on the current environment [1]. Previous studies have indicated that response inhibition involves two cognitive components: attention to incoming stimuli and prevention of automatic responses [2]. According to these studies, the neural mechanism of response inhibition can be measured by a Go/NoGo task, which is an experimental paradigm widely used in executive brain function research. In the Go/NoGo task, participants are asked to respond when frequent Go stimuli are presented and to withhold their responses to infrequent NoGo stimuli. Compared with the Go trials, two event-related potential (ERP) components are observed at prefronto-central electrodes during NoGo trials. One is the N2 component, which is a negative deflection that occurs around 140–300 ms, and the other is the P3 component, which is a positive deflection that occurs around 300–600 ms [3, 4]. Neuroimaging studies have suggested that the No-Go stimuli-elicited anterior-driven N2 and P3 components are generated from the anterior cingulate cortex, and these components reflect stimuli detection and response inhibition, respectively [5], which are higher-order cognitive processes that specifically rely on the prefrontal cortex.

Response inhibition can be adversely affected by sleep loss [68]. For example, 24 h of total sleep deprivation (TSD) results in impairments in both error detection and error remediation and a failure to avoid making the errors again [9]. Several studies have revealed that TSD impairs inhibition control and increases impulsive behaviors in response inhibition tasks [8, 10]. If the TSD is not long enough, the NoGo-N2 amplitude will not change because of the cerebral compensatory responses to the increased monitoring demands [9]. However, when the TSD is prolonged to 43 h or longer, the amplitudes of NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 are significantly decreased compared with baseline levels [11]. Furthermore, the prolonged P3 latencies and reduced P3 amplitudes during extended wakefulness suggest severe impairment of the response inhibition due to the long periods of sustained sleep restriction.

Recovery sleep (RS) is needed to improve inhibition performance after a long TSD [12]. Several studies have shown that the electrophysiological mechanisms for improving cognitive brain function during RS might involve increased delta power in the prefrontal cortex [12, 13]. Drummond and colleagues have suggested that the impaired inhibition and automatic responses that are induced by TSD can be completely remedied back to baseline level by one night of RS [14]. However, Wu and colleagues have indicated that even though RS does improve human performance, one night of sleep does not seem sufficient for full recovery of the impaired prefrontal function [15]. Whether 8 h of RS followed by a long period of wakefulness will result in full recovery of the response inhibition remains to be elucidated. Furthermore, the neural mechanisms underlying the recovery of the function of the prefrontal cortex and response inhibition are still unknown.

In the present study, a visual Go/NoGo task was employed with simultaneous electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings at 5 time points during the 36 h of TSD and after RS [0 h of TSD (Baseline), 12 h of TSD, 24 h of TSD, 36 h of TSD, and 8 h of RS]. It evaluated the following: (1) the dynamic changes of the N2 and P3 ERP components in the visual Go/No-Go task during the TSD and (2) the effects of 8 h of RS on response inhibition after 36 h of TSD. We assumed that the N2 and P3 components in the No-Go trials would attenuate with the prolonged TSD period and that the impaired response inhibition would be reversed after one night of RS.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fourteen healthy male adults (age: 25.9 ± 2.3; range, 18–28) were recruited from Beijing Normal University as paid volunteers. None of the participants had ever been involved in a psychophysiological experiment, and they all provided written informed consent prior to the start of the experiment. The general exclusion criteria were diseases of the central and peripheral nervous system, encephalic trauma, cardiovascular diseases and/or hypertension, cataracts and/or glaucoma, pulmonary problems, audiological problems, and alcohol or drug abuse. The participants did not smoke or drink alcohol and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All of the participants had normal intelligence (Raven test, IQ>100) and T scores on the General Symptom Index less than 60 (the general population has T scores over 63) [16, 17]. At the screening stage, the Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI) [18] was employed to rule out the participants with early rise or late sleep habits. The experiment was conducted in the Basic Aerospace Institute (Beijing, China) with nursing staff attending all the time. The subjects always had a partner with them during the study so they could keep each other awake through the night during continuous behavioral monitoring, and subjective sleepiness was assessed every 4 h. The experimental protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Beijing Institute of Basic Medical Science.

Experimental design

The visual Go/NoGo task was presented on a screen with a resolution of 1,280 × 768 pixels. The Go/NoGo task consisted of two arrow types (left and right, 78 × 18 pixels each, white visual stimuli with gray background) that were presented in a serial random pattern. Each trial began with the appearance of a + at the center of the screen. As shown in Fig 1, stimuli were presented for 80 ms with an inter-stimulus interval of 1,000 ms. Time window for the responses was set to 800 ms.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Go/NoGo stimuli type and temporal sequence of events in the task trials that consisted of two types of stimuli in the protocol.

In block 1, the participants were supposed to respond to the left arrow and withhold their response to the right arrow. In block 2, the response pattern was reversed. Each stimulus was presented with a duration of 80 ms and an interstimulus interval of 1,000 ms. The time window of the responses was 800 ms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.g001

The task was presented in two parallel blocks of 200 trials each (33% were No-Go trials and 67% were Go trials), which lasted for a total of 400 s. In the first block, the participants were instructed to respond to the left arrow and withhold response to the right arrow. In this case, the left arrow represented the Go stimulus, while the right arrow represented the No-Go stimulus. In the second block, the right arrow represented the Go stimulus, while the left arrow represented the NoGo stimulus. The participants were instructed to press a button when the Go stimulus appeared and to withhold their response when the NoGo stimulus appeared. In addition, the participants were told to press the key as fast as possible while maintaining a high level of accuracy.

The experiment was conducted in a quiet, magnetically shielded, and darkened room. The visual Go/NoGo task was performed with simultaneous EEG recording. The participants passively viewed the 21-in display that was 75 cm in front of them, and they were asked to maintain their gaze at the fixation circle throughout the experiment (visual angle, 3.29° × 1.76°). A training session was performed to ensure that the participants understood the task. All of the participants had hit rates that were 90% or above before the formal test.

After one week of normal sleep, the subjects visited the laboratory for the formal experiment. They had been required to maintain a regular sleep schedule of 8 h per night at least one week before the study. And all participants were asked to sign a sleep diary before the experiment to ensure the sleep quality. Central inhibition or stimulant drugs were forbidden during the entire experiment in the laboratory. The TSD started at 10:00 am the day (Day 1) after a routine sleep, and all participants needed to complete 4 times of Go/NoGo tasks within 12-h intervals. After 36 h of TSD, the participants received 8 h RS (Day 2 to Day 3), and the participants then performed the last Go/NoGo task (Fig 2). During the TSD, the Visual Analogue Scales (VAS), a self-assessment scale to evaluate subjective emotional state [19], was applied to assess the participants’ subjective sleepiness for every 4 h. The two partner participants performed the experiments at the same time. The nursing staff accompanied the participants all of the time in order to prevent the participants from falling asleep by observation and remind them throughout TSD session.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Schematic diagram of the experimental design.

TSD started after a routine nocturnal sleep period. The clock time axis represents the course of TSD and RS. TSD started at 10:00 pm at Day 1. The subjects needed to keep awaking for 36 h. Each arrow below the clock time axis represents the time points performed the Go/No-go task and EEG recording synchronously. The interval of the tasks was 12 hours during the TSD session. At the time point of 24:00 pm, Day 2, all participants accepted an 8-h recovery sleep.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.g002

EEG recordings

Continuous EEG recordings were made with a 32-channel SynAmps2 amplifier (Compumedics Neuroscan, Charlotte, NC, USA). The participants wore an Ag/AgCl electrode cap (Quik-Cap, Compumedics Neuroscan) that had electrodes at the 32 sites specified by the International 10–20 system. EEG recordings were sampled at 1,000 Hz, and the impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ for all of the channels. The bilateral mastoids (M1 and M2) were used for reference, and the forehead was used as a ground. Four additional electrodes were placed above and below the left eye in order to perform bipolar vertical electro-oculogram recordings, and horizontal electro-oculogram recordings were taken from the outer canthi of both eyes.

Behavioral and ERP statistical analyses

All of the EEG preprocessing was performed with the Compumedics NeuroScan software package (Version 4.3). For all of the participants, band-pass filters were applied between 0.05 Hz to 100 Hz with a modest 12/24-dB slope, including a 50-Hz notch filter that was applied to remove mains power noise. Ocular artifacts were estimated and subtracted with a time-domain regression analysis [20]. The continuous EEG recordings were segmented into epochs starting 100 ms before the onset of the stimulus and continuing until 800 ms after stimulus onset. The baseline was corrected to mean amplitude of 100 ms before the stimulus. The trials in which the voltage exceeded ±100 μV were rejected automatically by the system. The components were calculated with only the corrected responses, and the main ERP components were averaged. The grand-averaged ERPs were displayed graphically in order to identify the major peaks. The NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 components were defined as the maximum negative or positive peaks within the 150–300-ms and 300–600-ms latency windows, respectively, after the onset of each stimulus. According to No-Go anteriorization [21], only the components that were recorded at the F3, Fz, F4, FC3, FCz, FC4, C3, Cz, and C4 electrodes were included for further analysis (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Grand-average event-related potential waveforms at F3, FC3, C3, Fz, FCz, Cz, F4, FC4, and C4 for NoGo trials in different time points.

The color waveforms represents respectively the baseline, after 12-h of total sleep deprivation (TSD), after 24-h of TSD, after 36-h of TSD and after 8-h recovery sleep.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.g003

The data for two subjects were excluded from the study due to technical errors. Finally, the data for 12 subjects were included in the following statistical analysis. All of the analyses were conducted with PASW Statistics 18.0 for Windows, as shown in Table 1. The outcome variables for behavior included the average reaction time (RT), the hit rates on the Go trials, and the percentage of false alarms (FA). The effect of RS on behavior indexes was presented in Fig 4.

thumbnail
Fig 4. The differences of RT and Hit Rates in Go trials and FA in No-Go trials between Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS.

RTs: The Reaction Time, FA: False Alarms, Baseline: before total sleep deprivation session, 36 h TSD: after 36 hours of total sleep deprivation, 8 h RS: after 8 hours of recovery sleep. (A): The mean of RTs in Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS. (B): The mean of Hit Rate in Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS. (C): The mean of FA in Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS. The error bars represent standard deviation. *: p<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.g004

thumbnail
Table 1. The Reaction Time and Hit Rates in Go trials and False Alarms in No-Go trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.t001

The N2 and P3 amplitudes and latencies that were elicited by NoGo trials at the nine electrode sites are presented in Fig 3. The standard deviations of the N2 and P3 components during TSD are shown in Tables 2 and 3. The effect of RS on ERP components was presented in Fig 5.

thumbnail
Fig 5. ERP components differences between Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS.

Baseline: before total sleep deprivation session, 36 h TSD: after 36 hours of total sleep deprivation, 8 h RS: after 8 hours of recovery sleep. (A) The mean of NoGo-N2 latency in Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS. (B) The mean of NoGo-N2 amplitude in Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS. (C) The mean of NoGo-P3 latency in Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS. (D) NoGo-P3 amplitude in Baseline, 36 h TSD and 8 h RS. The error bars represent standard deviation. *: p<0.05 (uncorrected), **p<0.025 (corrected), corrected multiple comparison analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.g005

thumbnail
Table 2. Average NoGo-N2 Latencies and Amplitudes at F3, Fz, F4, FC3, FCz, FC4, C3, CZ, and C4 after Different Lengths of Total Sleep Deprivation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Average NoGo-P3 Latencies and Amplitudes at F3, Fz, F4, FC3, FCz, FC4, C3, CZ, and C4 after Different Lengths of Total Sleep Deprivation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.t003

In order to examine the effects of TSD on response inhibition, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was employed to analyze behavioral performance at the selected time points during TSD. The baseline, 12-h TSD, 24-h TSD, and 36-h TSD measures were the within-subject factors. For the NoGo-N2 or NoGo-P3 components during TSD, a three-way repeated ANOVA was conducted with functional cortex (prefrontal, prefronto-central, and central), site (Left, Midline, and Right), and the TSD time points (0 h, 12 h, 24 h, and 36 h) as the within-subject factors and the N2 and P3 amplitudes and latencies as the dependent variables. In further statistical analyses, we explored the effects of RS on the response inhibition in the different conditions, including baseline, 36-h TSD, and RS. In addition, we also employed a one-way ANOVA to separately analyze the condition effects (Baseline, 36-h TSD, and RS) on the behavioral index. The statistical significant value was set at 0.05 for behavioral index. We performed a three-way repeated ANOVA with functional cortex (prefrontal, prefronto-central, and central), site (Left, Midline, and Right), and the TSD time points (Baseline, 36-h TSD, and RS) on the NoGo-N2 or NoGo-P3 components during TSD. Greenhouse-Geisser corrections was applied if necessary. Corrected multiple comparison was applied to eliminate the Bonferroni effect. The p values less than 0.016 or 0.025 were considered statistically significant for TSD effect and RS effect comparison, respectively.

Results

Behavioral performance

The result of VAS showed that the participants were sleepier (t = -4.45, p = 0.001) after 36 h than the baseline level. The RTs of Go-trials, which reflected a prolonged response time window, did not show significant main effect of time during TSD [F (3, 44) = 1.824, p = 0.162]. The hit rates in the Go trials showed a significant main effect of time during TSD [F (3, 44) = 3.45, p = 0.028]. Further post-hoc analysis revealed that the hit rates in the Go trials after 36 h of TSD were significantly lower than those at baseline (mean difference = 0.079, p = 0.014). The FA rates in the No-Go trials did not differ [F (3, 44) = 0.288, p = 0.833] under the TSD session. A significant main effect of condition on the hit rates in the Go trials was found [F (2, 33) =, p = 0.008]. Further post-hoc analysis that after 36-h TSD, hit rates were decreased significantly (mean difference = -0.079, p = 0.014) compared with the baseline. However, the hit rates of the Go trials were increased significantly after RS compared with 36-h TSD (mean difference = 0.056, p = 0.02). No significant difference was found for the hit rates on Go trials between the baseline and RS (Fig 4). Furthermore, no main effects of condition were found in RTs of the Go trails and FA rates of the No-Go trials.

NoGo-N2

The means and standard deviations of the NoGo-N2 amplitudes and latencies at the F3, FZ, F4, FC3, FCZ, FC4, C3, CZ, and C4 electrodes during TSD are listed in Table 2. There was a significant main effect of time on the NoGo-N2 latency [F (3, 44) = 8.954, p = 0.000] during TSD. Adjusted multiple comparisons showed that there is no difference between the baseline and 12 h TSD for the NoGo-N2 latency. After 24 h of TSD, the NoGo-N2 latency was increased when compared to 12 h TSD (mean difference = 16.481, p = 0.010). Additionally, the NoGo-N2 latency was increased continuously after 36 h TSD and significantly longer than that of 12 h TSD (mean difference = 19.728, p = 0.007). No significant main effects were found for functional cortex or site on NoGo-N2 latency during TSD.

Significant main effects of functional cortex and site on NoGo-N2 amplitude were found [F (2, 33) = 16.130, p = 0.000; F (2, 33) = 13.957, p = 0.002, respectively] under condition of TSD. During TSD, the maximum amplitude of NoGo-N2 appeared at prefrontal cortex (Fig 3). The NoGo-N2 amplitude in the central cortex was smaller than that in the prefrontal and prefronto-central cortex (F3/Fz/F4 vs. C3/Cz/C4: mean difference = -2.290, p = 0.014; FC3/FCz/FC4 vs. C3/Cz/C4: mean difference = -2.044, p = 0.000). Moreover, the NoGo-N2 amplitudes in the middle electrodes were more negative than bilateral areas (Table 2). The left electrode site reached the statistical significance when compared with the middle and the right electrode site (Left vs. Midline: mean difference = 1.864, p = 0.002; Left vs Right: mean difference = 0.965, p = 0.008). There is no significant difference between the right site and the middle site (p < 0.016). No other interaction effects were found during the TSD (p < 0.016).

In the analysis of recovery sleep effects on the ERP components, we found a significant main effect of condition (baseline, 36 h of TSD, and RS) on NoGo-N2 latency [F (2, 33) = 7.66, p = 0.003]. The corrected multiple comparison analysis did not show any significant difference between baseline, 36 h of TSD and RS. No other significant main effect or interaction effects were found on NoGo-N2 latency among different conditions (p < 0.025). For NoGo-N2 amplitude, main effects were found for functional area [F (2, 33) = 11.023, p = 0.004] and site [F (2, 33) = 22.745, p = 0.000], but the main effect of condition was not observed. The corrected multiple comparison analysis showed that NoGo-N2 amplitudes in the prefronto-central area were larger than the amplitudes in central regions (FC3/FCz/FC4 vs. C3/Cz/C4: mean difference = -2.163, p = 0.001; Fig 3). No significant difference was found for NoGo-N2 amplitudes between the prefrontal cortex and the prefronto-central area. In addition, the NoGo-N2 amplitudes in the midline were more negative than those from the bilateral electrodes (Midline vs. Left: t = -2.234, p = 0.001; Midline vs. Right: t = -1.076, p = 0.012; Fig 3). No other interaction effects reached statistical significance (p < 0.025).

NoGo-P3

The means and standard deviations of the NoGo-P3 amplitudes and latencies at the F3, FZ, F4, FC3, FCZ, FC4, C3, CZ, and C4 electrodes are listed in Table 3. Significant main effect of time on NoGo-P3 latency was found during TSD with uncorrected Bonferroni effect [F (3, 44) = 3.005, p = 0.044]. No other significant interaction effects reached statistical significance (p < 0.016) in TSD session.

For NoGo-P3 amplitude, main effects were observed for time [F (3, 44) = 8.160, p = 0.000] during TSD. The NoGo-P3 amplitudes were reduced remarkably after 36 h TSD when compared with baseline (t = 3.828, p = 0.015) as shown in Table 3. Although the NoGo-P3 amplitudes decreased and exhibited a dose-dependent pattern along with prolonged time of TSD (Table 2), the difference did not reach statistical significance (p < 0.016). No other interaction effects reached statistical significance for NoGo-P3 amplitudes under the TSD session (p < 0.016).

In the analysis of recovery sleep effect on NoGo-P3 components, no main effect was found for the different condition (p < 0.025) on the NoGo-P3 latency. For NoGo-P3 amplitude, significant main effects of condition were observed [F (2, 33) = 8.182, p = 0.002]. The corrected multiple comparison analysis showed a reduced NoGo-P3 amplitude after 36 h of TSD when compared with the baseline (mean difference = 3.828, p = 0.007). Although NoGo-P3 amplitude showed increased tendency after 8 h RS compared with baseline, but the difference did not reach statistical significance (p < 0.025) (Fig 5). No other significant interaction effects reached statistical significance (p < 0.016) in TSD session.

The correlation between the behavioral performances change and the ERP component change

For analyzing the correlation between the behavioral performance and the ERP components change altered, we use Pearson correlation analysis to calculate the correlation coefficient of the changed behavioral performances and ERP component. The variables of behavioral performance include RTs and Hit Rates for Go trials, and FA for the NoGo trials. The ERP components include N2 latencies, N2 aptitude, P3 latencies and P3 aptitude in all electrodes sites. The Pearson correlation showed that positive correlations were found between the Hit Rates for Go trials and P3-aptitude in FC4 (r = 0.611, p = 0.035), Fz (r = 0.609, p = 0.035), FCz (r = 0.697, p = 0.012), C4 (r = 0.613, p = 0.034), Cz (r = 0.654, p = 0.021). However, other correlation between the behavioral performances change and the ERP component change had not been found

Discussion

Here we reported an investigation of the effects of TSD and RS on response inhibition with a simultaneous visual Go/NoGo task and EEG recordings. We recorded both behavioral and ERP indexes at 5 different time points in order to observe how response inhibition was altered during TSD and to what extent one night of normal sleep recovered the impaired executive brain function. In the behavioral analyses, we observed that the hit rates in the Go trials decreased markedly after TSD. However, FA, which reflected inhibition function, seemed less affected during TSD. Because response inhibition is a complex cognitive process, the behavioral task that was used in the present study may not have been sensitive enough to accurately reflect the temporal characteristics of response inhibition.

Because the circadian rhythm expresses distinct individual difference, we adopted the EEG data from the same time point in the same experimental schedule to ensure the subjects with the similar level of alertness. Circadian rhythm is related to the behavior performance along with the prolonged TSD [22]. Babkoff found that work capability decline fluctuated, and related to 24 hours of temperature curve of circadian rhythm, obviously at 2:00 am to 6:00 am. Neurobehavioral functions are affected by endogenous circadian rhythms, which including physiological alertness and cognitive performance [23]. Previous researches had been proved that cognitive function is sensitive to TSD [24, 25], so it would be best to evaluate response inhibition ability avoiding decline phase of circadian rhythm. In this study, 10:00 am and 10:00 pm was selected to performed experiment avoiding the effect of circadian rhythms on the cognitive function [26]. Due to complex circadian rhythms effect varies across individuals, the effects could not be total excluded during the TSD experiment [27, 28]. In line with our hypothesis, we observed a delay in the peak latencies tendency and a significant reduction in the amplitudes of NoGo-P3 during TSD. These results were consistent with several previous studies that reported the latency of P3 is positively related to sleepiness. Moreover, these studies suggested that extended wakefulness is related to the prolonged latency and reduced amplitude of the P3 component [21, 29, 30]. Because the P3 component is thought to reflect attention resource deployment and the P3 latency is thought to reflect the time window for stimulus categorization and evaluation, the increase in the P3 latency tendency that occurred during the prolonged TSD suggested that more time was needed for attention resource allocation after TSD. The decreased amplitudes of the NoGo-P3 potentials after 24 h of TSD, which may reflect the participants’ reduced concentration, also shows reduced discrimination to the target stimuli [22]. The prolonged latency tendency of P3 may indicate that subjects are experiencing difficulty allocating attentional resources to detect No-Go stimuli after TSD.

The N2 and P3 components of ERPs are related to the distinct sub-processes that occur in the response inhibition function [31]. In our study, the NoGo-N2 component only showed prolonged latency and did not show significant changes in amplitude. In accordance with the findings of Zhang’s study, the prolonged NoGo-N2 latency may reflect increased reaction time after a long period of sleep restriction [32]. These findings suggest that the TSD induced a difficulty in reducing the impulse to an inappropriate response. In addition, it could be the reduced speed of the response-selection processes that are required in the Go/NoGo task. The finding that N2 amplitude was not significantly altered in the NoGo trials may have been due to a cerebral compensatory response that involves an increase in the monitoring demand of the response selection from the TSD [9]. These results may indicate that a compensation mechanism exists for the impaired response inhibition, and, therefore, no significantly decreased N2 amplitudes were observed in this study. Another interpretation of these results is that the NoGo-N2 component covaries with the magnitude of the FA rates because FA rates increase after sleep deprivation [33].

For ERP component changes in NoGo trials, we had interestingly results of functional effects.

Consistent with previous studies, the NoGo-N2 amplitude showed negative tendency at the prefrontal and prefronto-central area than central functional area with TSD influence. Falkenstein and colleagues had reported that NoGo anteriorization both in NoGo-P3 and NoGo-N2 amplitude, most obviously for NoGo-N2 amplitude at the frontal area. Also they had observed that NoGo-N2 amplitude decrease steadily from electrode Fz to Oz [34]. The possible explanation for this phenomenon is that NoGo-N2 peak amplitude was located at the right fronto-central site, and the generating source of NoGo-P3 localized in left orbitofrontal cortex [35]. We did not found the effects of laterality of NoGo-P3 and NoGo-N2 amplitudes, which are maximum on central electrodes such as Fz, FCz and Cz [36, 37].

After 8 h of RS, although we observed an increase in NoGo-P3 amplitude compared with this value after 36 h of TSD, but the increase did not reach the significant statistically. Still, the recovery tendency for NoGo-P3 amplitude existed [Fig 4(D)]. Current results support the view that taking 1 to 8-h naps enhances performance and alertness during continuous operations [32, 38]. Interestingly, the increased latency of NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 that was induced by TSD was not significantly improved by one night of RS. Several studies have contended that an intense rebound occurs following sleep after a TSD [39, 40]. The rebound involved many changes, such as response inhibition function decline, increased delta power, decreased sigma power, and increased sleep time and efficiency [39, 41, 42]. Cajochen and colleagues have reported that the mechanism for improving function may be related to a dominant delta power throughout the frontal cortex that is induced by RS [43]. However, one night of RS after a long period of TSD may not be enough for full recovery of prefrontal brain function [15]. Our results of non-recovery of the NoGo-P3 amplitude provide evidence that the function for withholding inappropriate responses did not fully recover, as we speculated. Execution is a function that largely depends on activity in the prefrontal cortex [44]. In the present results, we observed obviously large N2 amplitude that was induced by No-Go stimuli in the prefrontal and prefronto-central area, which indicated the activation of prefrontal function due to the TSD. However, No-Go anteriorization still appeared in the NoGo-N2 component during the TSD, which indicated that the early stage of cognitive processing was less affected. Because the NoGo-N2 component is related to the Go stimuli [44, 45], these findings suggest that simple cognitive responses are less affected by TSD and can recover easily following RS, while higher cognitive brain function that is related to the NoGo-P3 is difficult to reverse. From the results for the NoGo-P3 component, we speculate that although the participants applied more attentional resources to the current task, their reaction speed to recognize which stimuli required the withholding of a response did not seem to improve. One possible explanation is that the improvements in executive function by RS were mainly reflected in the restoration of alertness, which allowed the distribution of more attentional resources to the current task of stimulation recognition and classification.

Limitations

Our study had several limitations. First, we only examined male college students in this study. Therefore, these results are not generalizable to older or female subjects. Second, the sample size in our study was small, so speculations on the meaning of the results of this study should be cautious. Except for the correlation between the positive correlations were found between the changes Hit Rates for Go trials and changes of P3 amplitude in FC4, Fz, FCz, C4 and Cz, we did not found other correlation between the behavioral performances change and the ERP component change, which may due to relative small sample size. The sample size should be increased and female subjects should be included in the future study. Third, we only assessed one time point when we examined the effects of RS after 36 h of TSD. We are considering assessing an additional time point in order to fix this weakness of the current study. Forth, due to the absence of EEG monitoring during the recovery sleep, the results should be interpreted conservatively. Micro-sleep had not been assessed a monitored in the stage of TSD, so the effects of frequency and total duration of these micro-sleep on the results could not be estimated. Additionally, the PVT may be sensitive to TSD influence, and will be used to test the decline of alertness following TSD administration in the future study.

Conclusion

Our results suggested that the response inhibition impairments that were induced by 36 h of TSD were improved by 8 h of RS. TSD induced a dose-dependent decrease in response inhibition that was revealed by NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 of prefrontal cortex activation, and 8 h of RS may help subjects to achieve a better recovery or maintain better response inhibition function. However, 8 h of RS did not result in recovery to baseline levels.

Supporting Information

S1 File. The supporting data include N2 and P3 components in NoGo trials under 5 time points in TSD and RS condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142361.s001

(XLSX)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: ZY YS. Performed the experiments: XJ EY JQ YS PC LW GM FZ. Analyzed the data: EY XJ YS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ZY YS. Wrote the paper: XJ YS EY YL.

References

  1. 1. Logan D, Cowan B. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological review, 1984; 91(3), 295.
  2. 2. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004.
  3. 3. Bruin KJ, Wijers A. Inhibition, response mode, and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2002; 113(7): 1172–1182. pmid:12088714
  4. 4. Smith JL, Johnstone SJ, Barry RJ. Response priming in the Go/NoGo task: the N2 reflects neither inhibition nor conflict. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2007; 118(2): 343–355. pmid:17140848
  5. 5. Fallgatter AJ, Bartsch AJ, Herrmann M. J. Electrophysiological measurements of anterior cingulate function. Journal of neural transmission. 2002; 109(5–6): 977–988. pmid:12111483
  6. 6. Harrison Y, Horne J. Sleep loss impairs short and novel language tasks having a prefrontal focus. Journal of sleep research. 1998; 7(2): 95–100. pmid:9682180
  7. 7. Jennings JR, Monk TH, Van der Molen MW. Sleep deprivation influences some but not all processes of supervisory attention. Psychological Science. 2002; 14(5): 473–486.
  8. 8. Chuah YL, Venkatraman V, Dinges DF, Chee MW. The neural basis of interindividual variability in inhibitory efficiency after sleep deprivation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006; 26(27): 7156–7162. pmid:16822972
  9. 9. Drummond SP, Brown GG, Salamat JS, Gillin JC. Increasing task difficulty facilitates the cerebral compensatory response to total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2004; 27(3): 445–451. pmid:15164897
  10. 10. Drummond S, Paulus MP, Tapert S F. Effects of two nights sleep deprivation and two nights recovery sleep on response inhibition. Journal of sleep research. 2006; 15(3): 261–265. pmid:16911028
  11. 11. Qi JL, Shao YC, Miao DM, Fan M, Bi GH, Yang Z. The effect of 43 hours of sleep deprivation on executive control functions: Event-related potentials in a visual Go/No go task. Social behavior and personality. 2010; 38(1): 29–42.
  12. 12. Cajochen C, Foy R, Dijk DJ. Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res Online, 1999; 2(3): 65–69. pmid:11382884
  13. 13. Gosselin A, De Koninck J, Campbell KB. Total sleep deprivation and novelty processing: implications for frontal lobe functioning. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2005; 116(1): 211–222. pmid:15589199
  14. 14. Drummond SP, Bischoff-Grethe A, Dinges DF, Ayalon L, Mednick SC, Meloy MJ. The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep. 2005; 28(9): 1059–1068. pmid:16268374
  15. 15. Wu JC, Gillin JC, Buchsbaum MS, Chen P, Keator DB, Wu NK, et al. Frontal lobe metabolic decreases with sleep deprivation not totally reversed by recovery sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006; 31(12): 2783–2792. pmid:16880772
  16. 16. Derogatis LR. SCL-90. Administration, scoring and procedures manual-Ⅰfor the R(evised) version. In: Derogatis LR, ed. Baltimore, John Hopkins University School of Medicine, Clinical Psychometrics Research Unit. 1977.
  17. 17. Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Manual for Raven's advanced progresssive matrices and vocabulary scales. Sec 4, The advanced progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists. 1988.
  18. 18. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. 1989; 28(2): 193–213 pmid:2748771
  19. 19. Monk TH. A visual analogue scale technique to measure global vigor and affect. Psychiatry Res. 1989; 27:89–99. pmid:2922449
  20. 20. Kenemans JL, Molenaar P, Verbaten MN, Slangen JL. Removal of the ocular artifact from the EEG: a comparison of time and frequency domain methods with simulated and real data. Psychophysiology. 1991, 28(1): 114–121. pmid:1886960
  21. 21. Panjwani U, Ray K, Chatterjee A, Bhaumik S, Kumar S. Electrophysiological correlates of cognition improve with nap during sleep deprivation. European journal of applied physiology. 2010; 108(3): 549–556. pmid:19865829
  22. 22. Koslowsky M, Babkoff H. Meta-analysis of the relationship between total sleep deprivation and performance. Chronobiol Int 1992; 9: 132–136. pmid:1533178
  23. 23. Goel M, Basner M, Rao H, Dinges DF. Circadian rhythms, sleep deprivation, and human performance. 2013; 119: 155–190. pmid:23899598
  24. 24. Haslam DR. Sleep loss, recovery sleep, and military performance. Ergonomics 1982; 25: 163–178. pmid:7084209
  25. 25. Opstad PK. The plasma vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP) response to exercise is increased after prolonged strain, sleep and energy deficiency and extinguished by glucose infusion. Peptides 1987; 8: 175–178. pmid:3575149
  26. 26. Lavie P. Sleep-wake as a biological rhythm. Annu Rev Psychol 2001; 52: 277–303. pmid:11148307
  27. 27. Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Sekihara K, Miyashita Y. No-go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Eur J Neurosci 1998; 10: 1209–1213. pmid:9753190
  28. 28. Garavan H, Ross TJ, Stein EA. Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1999; 96: 8301–8306. pmid:10393989
  29. 29. Morris AM, So Y, Lee KA, Lash AA, Becker CE. The P300 Event-Related Potential: The Effects of Sleep Deprivation. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine. 1992; 34(12): 1143–1152.
  30. 30. Jones K, Harrison Y. (2001). Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep medicine reviews. 2001; 5(6): 463–475. pmid:12531154
  31. 31. Kreusch F, Quertemont E, Vilenne A, Hansenne M. Alcohol abuse and ERP components in Go/No-go tasks using alcohol-related stimuli: Impact of alcohol avoidance. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2014; 94(1): 92–99. pmid:25110836
  32. 32. Zhang Q, Liao Y, Qi J, Zhao Y, Zhu T, Liu Z, et al. A Visual ERP Study of Impulse Inhibition following a Zaleplon-Induced Nap after Sleep Deprivation. PloS one; 2014: 9(5), e95653. pmid:24806263
  33. 33. Kopp B, Rist F, Mattler UWE. N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology. 1996; 33(3): 282–294. pmid:8936397
  34. 34. Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Hohnsbein J. ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychol. 1999; 101(2–3): 267–91.
  35. 35. Bokura H, Yamaguchi S, Kobayashi S. Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2001; 112: 2224–2232. pmid:11738192
  36. 36. Hirokazu B, Shuhei Y, Shotai . Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2001; 112: 2224–2232 pmid:11738192
  37. 37. Lisa M, Jonkman . The development of preparation, conflict monitoring and inhibition from early childhood to young adulthood: a Go/Nogo ERP study. Brain Research. 2006; 1097: 181–193 pmid:16729977
  38. 38. Bonnet MH. The effect of varying prophylactic naps on performance, alertness and mood throughout a 52-hour continuous operation. Sleep. 1991; 14(4): 307–315. pmid:1947593
  39. 39. Kleitman N. Sleep and Wakefulness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1963.
  40. 40. Carskadon MA, Dement W. Normal Human Sleep: An Overview. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement W. editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 2. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company; 1994. pp. 16–25.
  41. 41. Kales A, Tan TL, Kollar EJ, Naitoh P, Preston TA, Terry A, et al. Sleep patterns following 205 hours of sleep deprivation. Psychosomatic medicine. 1970; 32(2): 189–200. pmid:4314610
  42. 42. Borbély AA, Baumann F, Brandeis D, Strauch I, Lehmann D. Sleep deprivation: effect on sleep stages and EEG power density in man. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. 1981; 51(5): 483–493. pmid:6165548
  43. 43. Cajochen C, Foy R, Dijk DJ. Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res Online. 1999; 2(3): 65–69. pmid:11382884
  44. 44. Nilsson JP, Söderström M, Karlsson AU, Lekander M, Åkerstedt T, Lindroth NE, et al. Less effective executive functioning after one night's sleep deprivation. Journal of sleep research. 2005; 14(1): 1–6. pmid:15743327
  45. 45. Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Cohen JD. Stimulus modality, perceptual overlap, and the go/no-go N2. Psychophysiology. 2004; 41(1): 157–160. pmid:14693011