Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Territory Quality and Plumage Morph Predict Offspring Sex Ratio Variation in a Raptor

  • Nayden Chakarov ,

    az.nayden@gmail.com

    Affiliation Department of Animal Behaviour, Bielefeld University, PO Box 10 01 31, 33501, Bielefeld, Germany

  • Martina Pauli,

    Affiliation Department of Animal Behaviour, Bielefeld University, PO Box 10 01 31, 33501, Bielefeld, Germany

  • Anna-Katharina Mueller,

    Affiliation Department of Animal Behaviour, Bielefeld University, PO Box 10 01 31, 33501, Bielefeld, Germany

  • Astrid Potiek,

    Affiliation Department of Animal Behaviour, Bielefeld University, PO Box 10 01 31, 33501, Bielefeld, Germany

  • Thomas Grünkorn,

    Affiliation BioConsult SH, Brinckmannstr. 31, 25813, Husum, Germany

  • Cor Dijkstra,

    Affiliation Behavioural Biology, University of Groningen, PO Box 11103, 9700 CC, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • Oliver Krüger

    Affiliation Department of Animal Behaviour, Bielefeld University, PO Box 10 01 31, 33501, Bielefeld, Germany

Abstract

Parents may adapt their offspring sex ratio in response to their own phenotype and environmental conditions. The most significant causes for adaptive sex-ratio variation might express themselves as different distributions of fitness components between sexes along a given variable. Several causes for differential sex allocation in raptors with reversed sexual size dimorphism have been suggested. We search for correlates of fledgling sex in an extensive dataset on common buzzards Buteo buteo, a long-lived bird of prey. Larger female offspring could be more resource-demanding and starvation-prone and thus the costly sex. Prominent factors such as brood size and laying date did not predict nestling sex. Nonetheless, lifetime sex ratio (LSR, potentially indicative of individual sex allocation constraints) and overall nestling sex were explained by territory quality with more females being produced in better territories. Additionally, parental plumage morphs and the interaction of morph and prey abundance tended to explain LSR and nestling sex, indicating local adaptation of sex allocation However, in a limited census of nestling mortality, not females but males tended to die more frequently in prey-rich years. Also, although females could have potentially longer reproductive careers, a subset of our data encompassing full individual life histories showed that longevity and lifetime reproductive success were similarly distributed between the sexes. Thus, a basis for adaptive sex allocation in this population remains elusive. Overall, in common buzzards most major determinants of reproductive success appeared to have no effect on sex ratio but sex allocation may be adapted to local conditions in morph-specific patterns.

Introduction

Parents should preferentially produce the sex with the highest fitness value under the prevailing environmental conditions [14]. One basic determinant of fitness is the difference in resource demands between offspring of both sexes and the resulting difference in infant mortality under harsh conditions [2, 5]. In consequence, parents may adapt their offspring sex ratio to the local conditions. The conjunction of the “costly sex” and “local adaptation” hypotheses implies that in harsh conditions, such as prey-deficient years and in poor-quality territories, parents should preferentially produce the less demanding sex [2, 6, 7]. Another basic fitness component is the length of reproductive careers (timespan in which individuals attempt to reproduce). Reproductive lifespan can differ between sexes and hence may explain sex-ratio variation [8]. In birds, being raised early in the breeding season may enable one sex to recruit earlier and have a longer reproductive career than the other sex, raised under the same conditions. In monogamous species, reproductive career length is strongly correlated with fitness, thus providing an incentive for sex allocation over the breeding season [8]. Such an allocation should lead to different distributions of reproductive career length between sexes. Under this “early sex hypothesis”, offspring of the more rewarding sex should be produced earlier in the season than the less rewarding sex. Finally, the lifetime reproductive success (LRS, here used as the number of fledglings produced over the individual lifetime) is a major component of fitness and its variation gives a good impression of fitness variance in a population, albeit it does not account for variance in pre-reproductive survival [9]. Although the overall mean LRS should be equal between the sexes, differences in the variance and distribution of LRS between sexes would present a cause for adaptive sex allocation.

In this study, we use a large dataset of reproductive performance and offspring sex of common buzzards Buteo buteo, encompassing the entire lifetime reproductive output of individuals. Common buzzards, like most birds of prey, exhibit reversed sexual size dimorphism (RSD), with female offspring fledging up to 20% heavier and therefore probably demanding greater parental investment than males [1013]. Females of medium-sized birds of prey, such as marsh harriers Circus aeruginosus and goshawks Accipiter gentilis, may accelerate the start of their reproductive careers, if fledged early in the season [8], for similar patterns in ungulates see [14]. Similarly, some female buzzards could have longer breeding careers and if so, the variance and other aspects of the distribution of breeding career length may differ between sexes. An earlier study showed that female buzzards indeed may have a slightly longer reproductive lifespan and slightly more breeding attempts than males, without differences in mean LRS [15]. With a mean brood size of two and no sign of sex-biased dispersal [16, 17] in buzzards, the “local adaptation hypothesis” predicts increased male production under harsh conditions, while the “early sex hypothesis” predicts increased female production earlier in the breeding season [14].

Furthermore, common buzzards appear in three plumage morphs (dark, intermediate and light) which are inherited in a Mendelian fashion [18, 19]. Buzzard plumage morphs differ substantially in individual fitness (intermediates having higher LRS than extreme morphs; [15, 20]), behaviour (light males are more aggressive than intermediate and dark ones; [21]), immunity (intermediates have lower cellular and higher specific humoral response; Chakarov et al. unpublished manuscript) and parasite loads [endoparasite infection intensity decreases and ectoparasite infestation increases with melanisation; [20]. Each of these traits could influence sex allocation but due to their possible complex interactions we refrain from directed predictions about the specific sex allocation of morphs.

Here, we first search for correlates of sex ratio among variables known to influence sex allocation in other systems. Among them are: Laying date (corresponding to predictions of the “early sex hypothesis”) [8], prey availability [22, 23], and territory quality (corresponding to predictions of the “local adaptation hypothesis”) [24, 25], as well as parental plumage morph [26, 27] and parental age [28], being correlates of reproductive performance [15, 19]. In buzzards, plumage morph and prey availability are the two main drivers of reproductive investment, therefore their interaction may also be expected to affect sex allocation [15, 2931]. We analyse two hierarchical levels of sex variation: (1) Nestling sex allows to test hypotheses about effects of individual offspring traits, such as laying date corresponding to the “early chick hypothesis”[32]. (2) Lifetime sex ratio provides us with the rare opportunity to search for effects of lifetime reproductive strategies on offspring sex ratios and to identify potential constraints of adaptive sex allocation such as genetically-encoded polymorphisms or maternal effects, although it is not a good parameter to study short-term adaptations of sex ratio. Finally, we test whether the three previously discussed premises for adaptive sex allocation (higheroffspring mortality corresponding to the “costly sex hypothesis”, longer reproductive lifespan, and higher lifetime reproductive success in either sex) are met in common buzzards.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

Blood samples were collected for common buzzard nestlings via venepuncture. All field studies and animal handling were performed with permission from the local authority Kreis Gütersloh, permit nr: 4.5.2-723-Bussard in accordance to German federal and state laws.

Study site

The study was carried out in a ca. 300 km2 area in eastern Westphalia, Germany (8°25′ E and 52°06′ N) between 1989 and 2012 with sex ratio data being available from 2002–2012. The habitat consists of pastures and meadows, interspersed with woodlots, varying between 0.001 and 7 km2 in size [33, 34].

Population and explanatory variables

Buzzard population studies with individual identification of breeding birds in this area have been performed since 1989 (e.g. [30, 35, 36]). Buzzards have distinctive individual plumage pigmentation patterns, allowing individual recognition without artificial marking. In more recent years, photographing individuals and genetic fingerprinting of buzzard chicks were also used to increase the resighting accuracy [37]. Buzzards, like other birds of prey, have a high breeding site fidelity throughout their lives [38]. For all breeding buzzards, the first breeding attempt in the study area was assumed to be the beginning of the reproductive career, and thus a minimum age is known. An individual was assumed to be dead if it was not recorded in the area for at least two consecutive years [9]. LRS for individuals with completely known breeding life-histories was calculated as the total number of fledglings produced during their lifetime. Partner change can happen between years, so LRS of female and male breeding partners can differ. Territory quality was defined as the proportion of years a territory was occupied since it was used for the first time [39] and is a good predictor of reproductive success [40]. Since our dataset spans more than several generations of territory holders, we consider environmental quality to be a substantial component of territory quality.

Common buzzards are highly territorial, specialised raptors, and density of voles, their main prey, strongly predicts annual fluctuations in buzzard reproductive output [23, 41]. To estimate food availability, we scored vole abundance at the beginning of the breeding season, using three categories: low, intermediate or high. We used the re-opened holes method, where the number of active holes per unit area is counted and no trapping is performed [42]. This score strongly predicts the fraction of common voles Microtus arvalis among all prey items found in the nest when chicks are sampled (r = 0.746, df = 9, P < 0.001). Since 2002, all accessible nests were climbed (85% of all successful broods) and nestlings ringed. Tarsus length was measured with a calliper to the nearest 0.1 mm, wing length with a ruler to the nearest 1 mm, and weight was taken with a Pesola spring balance to the nearest 5 g. Age of the chicks was estimated by comparison of morphometric measurements with a standard, sex-specific growth curve [43]; mean age at ringing 29 ± 6 days] and subtracted from day of the year in order to derive hatching and laying date estimates, assuming an incubation period of 34 days (Mebs 1964). After ringing the nest was visited short before the estimated day of fledging to count the number of visible fledglings. The number of fledglings of the brood is further referred to as brood size. During the breeding season, morph of the female and male territory holders (further referred to as mother and father) and of the nestlings were recorded. Individuals with dark head, heavily speckled or dark breast and underwing coverts were considered as dark. Birds with dark head, intermediately speckled breast and underwing coverts were scored as intermediate. Birds with little or no melanisation of breast and underwing coverts, in extreme cases with light head and upperwing coverts were scored as light. From each ringed nestling, a blood sample of ca. 200 μl was taken via venipuncture of the brachial vein and used for DNA-sexing following a standard protocol [44]. More than 100 samples were scored more than once to verify repeatability of molecular sexing. DNA-sexing results were compared with morphological sexing, common in size-dimorphic birds of prey [45]. No inconsistencies were detected between both methods.

Datasets and statistical analyses

Overall, 1780 nestlings from 881 broods were sexed. This included 43 chicks which died before fledging and were used to test for sex-specific mortality before fledging. To explain nestling sex ratio in the final dataset, we used binomial generalized linear mixed models with logit link. Factors used to explain nestling sex were the morphs of both parents and of the nestling, and annual vole score. The age of both parents (i.e. current length of their reproductive career), date of laying, brood size and territory quality were entered as covariates. The final dataset with data available for all variables contained 1678 nestlings. Brood and mother identities were entered as nested random factors in order to account for potential non-independence of sibling sexes. Lifetime sex ratio was computed as the proportion of males among hatchlings produced during the lifetime of a parent. Individual-specific explanatory variables were lifetime reproductive success (LRS), the individual’s and partner’s melanin morphs (in case of multiple partners the mean morph of the partners was computed with light and dark considered as extremes and intermediates as being 50% of both extremes), the length of the individual reproductive career (longevity) and territory quality, after assuring that included individuals had not switched territories.

LRS, observed longevity (corresponding to the length of the reproductive career in our data) and lifetime sex ratio were analysed for 109 mothers of 432 nestlings and 94 fathers of 384 nestlings with fully known life histories and where all offspring were sexed. These datasets include only individuals which have started breeding after 2001 and therefore have no overlap with a sample from the same population used for previous LRS analyses [15]. Lifetime sex ratio was analysed with GLMMs with Gaussian distribution with cohort of the individual included as a random term. A complete description of all datasets and models is included in Tables 15.

thumbnail
Table 1. Datasets for analysis of nestlings sex, lifetime sex ratio of females and males, lifetime reproductive success, reproductive lifespan (longevity) and mortality of nestlings.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Initial (A) and best (B) models of nestlings sex in the dataset including all sampled common buzzard nestlings (n = 1678).

ANOVA between initial and best model of nestling sex χ2 = 21.548, df = 21, P = 0.426, ΔAIC = 19.6.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Initial (A) and best (B) models of lifetime sex ratio of female common buzzards with completely known life histories (n = 109).

ANOVA between initial and best model of female lifetime sex ratio χ2 = 8.503, df = 6, P = 0.203, ΔAIC = 3.5.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Initial (A) and best (B) models of lifetime sex ratio of male common buzzards with completely known life histories (n = 94).

ANOVA between initial and best model of female lifetime sex ratio χ2 = 0.943, df = 2, P = 0.624, ΔAIC = 3.1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Generalized linear models of lifetime reproductive success, LRS (A), reproductive lifespan, longevity (B) and nestling mortality (C).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.t005

Maximal models contained all variables without missing values in the final dataset. Model selection was based on AIC model weights [46]. Statistical modelling was performed in R 3.1.1, using the packages lme4 1.1–7 and MuMIn 1.10.5 [47]. Plots of sex ratio against all independent variables were visually inspected for non-linear relationships. Values are given and plotted ± SE. All datasets, full initial and best final models are summarised in Tables 15.

Results

Nestling sex

The overall sex ratio in our most comprehensive dataset did not significantly deviate from random (50.7% male, Neuhäuser test, z = 0.563, P = 0.645, N = 1780; test described in Neuhäuser 2004 [48]. The best model explaining nestling sex in the full dataset contained territory quality (Table 2). More female nestlings hatched in territories of better quality (Fig 1). Additionally, the interaction of annual vole score and plumage morph of the mother was a significant predictor of nestling sex ratio (χ2 = 9.552, df = 4, P = 0.049). In low vole years, both light and dark mothers produced more male nestlings, while intermediate mothers produced more female offspring (Fig 1), and there was no difference in nestling sex ratio in years of intermediate and high vole abundance. This interaction of maternal morph and vole score, however, was not included in the best model explaining nestling sex (ΔAIC = 1.6 between best model and model including vole score × morph of mother).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Sex ratio of nestling buzzards hatched (a) in poor, intermediate and good territories based on their proportional occupancy and (b) in years with low, intermediate and high vole abundance to dark intermediate and light mothers.

Binning into three territory quality classes is for visual purposes only. Statistical analyses were performed with the continuous variable territory quality.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.g001

Lifetime sex ratio

We found no significant predictors of female lifetime sex ratio (LRS: χ2 = 0.652, df = 1; longevity: χ2 < 0.001, df = 1, female morph: χ2 = 2.727, df = 2, mean morph of male partners: χ2 = 2.627, df = 1, all P > 0.1, N = 109; Fig 2, Table 3). Lifetime sex ratio of fathers was predicted by their own melanin morph and territory quality, but not by their LRS or longevity (Table 4). Over their lifetimes, light fathers fledged significantly higher proportion of daughters than dark and intermediate fathers (Fig 2). Males from pairs in better territories also sired more daughters. However, none of these effects remained significant when reproductive careers consisting of single broods were excluded. With increasing LRS, lifetime sex ratio approached equity (absolute difference between lifetime sex ratio and 0.5, Pearson r = -0.451, df = 94, P < 0.001).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Lifetime sex ratio (+SE) of (a) mothers and (b) fathers of different melanin morphs with entirely known reproductive output.

Sample sizes are number of individuals of the respective class with completely known lifetime sex ratio.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.g002

LRS, longevity and sex-specific chick mortality

There were no differences in the distributions of LRS or longevity between males and females with completely known life histories and sexed offspring (Poisson GLMs with log link, difference between sexes: LRS χ2 = 0.017, df = 1, P = 0.895, N = 205; longevity χ2 = 0.080, df = 1, P = 0.777, N = 205; Fig 3, Tables 5 and 6).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Histograms of observed length of reproductive career in years and lifetime reproductive success (LRS, total number of fledglings) of adult female and male buzzards (# individuals) with completely known LRS and lifetime sex ratio.

LRS distributions of males and females were statistically indistinguishable.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.g003

thumbnail
Table 6. Properties of distributions of lifetime reproductive success (LRS, total number of fledglings produced over the individual lifetime) and length of reproductive career of adult female and male buzzards with completely sampled offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.t006

Out of 43 nestlings known to have died before fledging, 16 were female and 27 were male. Nestling mortality was not significantly sex-biased, but tended to be predicted by vole score and was significantly predicted by the interaction of vole score and nestling sex (binomial GLM with logit link: vole score deviance = 5.263, df = 2, P = 0.072; nestling sex deviance = 2.571, df = 1, P = 0.109; vole score × sex deviance = 6.666, df = 2, P = 0.036, N = 1780). Years with intermediate vole score tended to have lower mortality than both high and low vole years. In high vole years, male nestlings had significantly higher mortality than females. Broods with known mortality cases were larger before but not after mortality (broods without mortality, mean size = 2.00 ± 0.03; broods before mortality, mean size = 2.63 ± 0.14, χ2 = 20.295, N = 884, P < 0.001; broods after mortality, mean size = 2.12 ± 0.14, χ2 = 0.664, P = 0.415, N = 43). Broods with mortality cases were male-biased both before (mean male fraction before mortality = 0.662 ± 0.046, N = 43; broods without mortality mean = 0.495 ± 0.0138, N = 841; χ2 = 7.253, P = 0.007) and after mortality (mean male fraction after mortality = 0.714 ± 0.057, N = 39; χ2 = 11.203, P = 0.001). Nestling mortality was not explained by territory quality or its interaction with nestling sex.

Discussion

In common buzzards, we predicted a male-biased sex ratio under harsh conditions such as vole-poor years and in low-quality territories, corresponding to the “local adaptation hypothesis”. At the same time, we predicted more females to be produced earlier in the breeding season, corresponding to the “early sex hypothesis”. However, in common buzzards we found no systematic sex-ratio variation in response to survival-related variables such as food abundance alone or laying date. The overall scarcity of sex ratio correlations might be due to the similarity of distributions of LRS, length of reproductive career and infant mortality between sexes. The distribution of these basic fitness components was very similar between buzzard sexes. Such a pattern could reduce the scope for adaptive sex ratio adjustment.

The most consistent variable explaining buzzard nestling sex ratio was territory quality. In accordance with the “local adaptation hypothesis”, more males were produced in poor-quality territories (Fig 1). Frequently-used territories are associated with higher reproductive success and can therefore be considered to be better [40]. We are still unaware what features make a territory preferable for buzzards. However, annual food availability reflected by vole abundance showed no direct effects on offspring sex ratio. Additionally, we could not find an effect of territory quality on nestling mortality. Therefore it needs to be investigated in more detail at which developmental phase the bias in sex ratio arises.

Surprisingly, despite the absence of strong indicators for the adaptive value of biased sex ratios, we found a sex ratio pattern in relation to plumage morphs in buzzards. On the one hand, under poor food conditions, dark and light females produced more males, while intermediately-coloured females produced more females (Fig 1). The sex allocation of extremely-coloured females is in line with the “local adaptation hypothesis”. Intermediate females have higher fitness and may overall be of higher quality than extreme morphs [19]. Thus, intermediate females might be less resource-restricted and able to allocate offspring sex in a contrasting way to utilize the higher reproductive value of females in the given cohort despite harsh conditions [49]. A similar interaction between local conditions and melanin colouration has been found to affect sex allocation in barn owls [31]. Although the interaction of plumage morph and food abundance explained population-wide nestling sex, its significance should be treated cautiously since it was not part of the best explanatory models and a conservative interpretation would be that it does not contribute to explaining nestling sex.

On the other hand, morph explained male lifetime sex ratio, with light males having more female offspring over their lifetimes. Females often adapt their offspring sex ratio to features of their partners, even in monogamous species with low extra pair paternity [50]. This effect might be connected to differences between morphs and sexes in aggressiveness, parasitism and capability to cope with food limitations (see Introduction; [20, 21, 27, 51]). However, melanin morph in buzzards is inherited from both parents and it should be further investigated why offspring sex ratio would be influenced only by morph of the male partner [19, 27].

Any apparent bias of lifetime sex ratio decreased with higher offspring numbers. Too few buzzards had sufficiently high offspring numbers to identify sample-size independent extremes in lifetime sex ratio. Thus, lifetime sex ratio as a measure of allocation flexibility would be better applicable to organisms with larger litter sizes. So far, we cannot recognize individual constraints on sex allocation in buzzards.

While the “costly sex hypothesis” predicts higher female nestling mortality, especially in years of low food abundance, we found a trend for higher mortality of the smaller, supposedly less demanding males in vole-rich years. However, in such years, parents might be prone to overestimate their own provisioning capabilities and female chicks may be better able to outcompete their male siblings for prey [6, 52]. Higher mortality of males may occur also for competition-independent cryptic reasons. For example, if years with high prey abundance should coincide with years with high parasite abundance a lower immune-competence of males could lead to their higher mortality. Competition-independent mortality might explain why males died more often in male-biased and not in female-biased broods [53]. Our sample of nestling mortality was small but our result, showing overall balanced infant mortality between sexes, are supported by findings in goshawks and sparrowhawks, where also no sex-biased nestling mortality has been found [45, 54].

We did not find any patterns in LRS and longevity supporting the “early sex hypothesis” and correspondingly, we did not find any sex-ratio bias along hatching date between broods. Unfortunately, a direct test of this hypothesis by comparing the life histories of individuals of both sexes born on the same date is currently impossible for highly mobile and long-lived species such as buzzards. Nonetheless, if individuals of one sex occasionally have longer breeding careers and produce more offspring overall, the distributions of LRS and longevity should differ between the sexes. Longevity of both sexes is similar in our population but a recent study found adult survival of male buzzards to increase faster than survival of female buzzards [29]. This pattern may arise because males can profit more from increasing winter food availability under rapidly changing climate conditions. A difference in adult survival between sexes may be a significant cause for sex allocation in the future.

In summary, we did not find clear differences in LRS, length of reproductive career or mortality that may provide a basis for sex allocation in common buzzards. Only after these analyses we can comprehend why several expected life history traits and environmental conditions do not explain offspring sex ratio. However, offspring sex ratio was explained by environmental quality and its interaction with parental phenotype. This indicates potential phenotype-specific adaptation of sex allocation to local conditions and emphasizes the need for further examination of sex allocation in colour-polymorphic species.

Supporting Information

S1 Datasets. Buzzard nestling sex and lifetime sex ratio.

Datasets on buzzard nestling sex, nestling mortality, lifetime sex ratio and reproductive career of adult buzzards used by Chakarov et al. “Territory quality and plumage morph predict offspring sex ratio variation in a raptor”.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138295.s001

(TXT)

Acknowledgments

We thank Roelie Wiegman, Bonnie de Vries, Ellis Mulder and Elke Hippauf for assistance with molecular sexing and Ulrich Stefener for help with buzzard and goshawk monitoring. We are grateful for the comments of many anonymous reviewers which greatly helped to improve this manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: OK NC CD. Performed the experiments: CD NC AKM AP MP TG OK. Analyzed the data: NC OK. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: CD OK NC TG. Wrote the paper: NC OK AKM AP.

References

  1. 1. Trivers RL, Willard DE. Natural selection of parental ability to vary sex-ratio of offspring. Science. 1973;179(4068):90–2. pmid:ISI:A1973O337700022.
  2. 2. Fisher RA. The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1930.
  3. 3. West SA, Shuker DM, Sheldon BC. Sex-ratio adjustment when relatives interact: A test of constraints on adaptation. Evolution. 2005;59(6):1211–28. pmid:ISI:000230077500005.
  4. 4. West SA. Sex allocation. Levin SA, Horn HS, editors. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2009.
  5. 5. Benito MM, Gonzalez-Solis J. Sex ratio, sex-specific chick mortality and sexual size dimorphism in birds. J Evol Biol. 2007;20(4):1522–30. pmid:ISI:000248177600026.
  6. 6. Nicolaus M, Michler SPM, Ubels R, van der Velde M, Komdeur J, Both C, et al. Sex-specific effects of altered competition on nestling growth and survival: an experimental manipulation of brood size and sex ratio. J Anim Ecol. 2009;78(2):414–26. pmid:WOS:000263038800014.
  7. 7. Brommer JE, Karell P, Pihlaja T, Painter JN, Primmer CR, Pietiäinen H. Ural owl sex allocation and parental investment under poor food conditions. Oecologia. 2003;137(1):140–7. pmid:12836010
  8. 8. Daan S, Dijkstra C, Weissing FJ. An evolutionary explanation for seasonal trends in avian sex ratios. Behav Ecol. 1996;7(4):426–30. pmid:ISI:A1996VW03800006.
  9. 9. Newton I. Lifetime reproduction in birds. London: Academic Press; 1989.
  10. 10. Riedstra B, Dijkstra C, Daan S. Daily energy expenditure of male and female Marsh Harrier nestlings. Auk. 1998;115(3):635–41. pmid:WOS:000074893700008.
  11. 11. Krijgsveld KL, Dijkstra C, Visser GH, Daan S. Energy requirements for growth in relation to sexual size dimorphism in marsh harrier Circus aeruginosus nestlings. Physiol Zool. 1998;71(6):693–702. pmid:ISI:000081684800009.
  12. 12. Vedder O, Dijkstra C, Dekker AL, Waasdorp S, Visser GH. Sex-specific nestling body mass in relation to brood sex composition in the Eurasian Sparrowhawk Accipiter nisus. Ardea. 2005;93(2):179–87. pmid:WOS:000235734500004.
  13. 13. Vedder O, Dekker AL, Visser GH, Dijkstra C. Sex-specific energy requirements in nestlings of an extremely sexually size dimorphic bird, the European sparrowhawk (Accipiter nisus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2005;58(4):429–36. pmid:ISI:000230705600013.
  14. 14. Krüger O, Radford AN, Anderson C. Successful sons or superior daughters: sex-ratio variation in springbok. Proc R Soc B-Biol Sci. 2005;272(1561):375–81. pmid:ISI:000227809000004.
  15. 15. Krüger O, Lindström J. Lifetime reproductive success in common buzzard, Buteo buteo: from individual variation to population demography. Oikos. 2001;93(2):260–73. pmid:ISI:000169451000009.
  16. 16. Chakarov N, Jonker RM, Boerner M, Hoffman JI, Krüger O. Variation at phenological candidate genes correlates with timing of dispersal and plumage morph in a sedentary bird of prey. Mol Ecol. 2013;22(21):5430–40. pmid:WOS:000325990500012.
  17. 17. Walls SS, Kenward RE. Movements of radio-tagged common buzzards Buteo buteo in their first year. Ibis. 1995;137(2):177–82. pmid:WOS:A1995QW24000006.
  18. 18. Cramp S, Simmons KEL. Handbook of the birds of Europe, the Middle East and North Africa. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press; 1980.
  19. 19. Krüger O, Lindström J, Amos W. Maladaptive mate choice maintained by heterozygote advantage. Evolution. 2001;55(6):1207–14. pmid:ISI:000169903000014.
  20. 20. Chakarov N, Boerner M, Krüger O. Fitness in common buzzards at the cross-point of opposite melanin-parasite interactions. Funct Ecol. 2008;22(6):1062–9. pmid:ISI:000260670000015.
  21. 21. Boerner M, Krüger O. Aggression and fitness differences between plumage morphs in the common buzzard (Buteo buteo). Behav Ecol. 2009;20(1):180–5. pmid:ISI:000262152300024.
  22. 22. Desfor KB, Boomsma JJ, Sunde P. Tawny Owls Strix aluco with reliable food supply produce male-biased broods. Ibis. 2007;149(1):98–105. pmid:ISI:000243474600012.
  23. 23. Byholm P, Ranta E, Kaitala V, Linden H, Saurola P, Wikman M. Resource availability and goshawk offspring sex ratio variation: a large-scale ecological phenomenon. J Anim Ecol. 2002;71(6):994–1001. pmid:ISI:000178975100009.
  24. 24. Heg D, Dingemanse NJ, Lessells CM, Mateman AC. Parental correlates of offspring sex ratio in Eurasian Oystercatchers. Auk. 2000;117(4):980–6. pmid:ISI:000090070500012.
  25. 25. Rubenstein DR. Natural history miscellany—Temporal but not spatial environmental variation drives adaptive offspring sex allocation in a plural cooperative breeder. Am Nat. 2007;170(1):155–65. pmid:ISI:000247527200015.
  26. 26. Janssen K, Erikstad KE, Bensch S. Offspring sex ratio allocation in the parasitic jaeger: selection for pale females and melanic males? Behav Ecol. 2006;17(2):236–45. pmid:ISI:000235451700012.
  27. 27. Roulin A, Altwegg R, Jensen H, Steinsland I, Schaub M. Sex-dependent selection on an autosomal melanic female ornament promotes the evolution of sex ratio bias. Ecol Lett. 2010;13(5):616–26. pmid:WOS:000276790200010.
  28. 28. Weimerskirch H, Lallemand J, Martin J. Population sex ratio variation in a monogamous long-lived bird, the wandering albatross. J Anim Ecol. 2005;74(2):285–91. pmid:ISI:000227593600010.
  29. 29. Jonker RM, Chakarov N, Krüger O. Climate change and habitat heterogeneity drive a population increase in Common Buzzards Buteo buteo through effects on survival. Ibis. 2014;156(1):97–106. pmid:WOS:000328545700008.
  30. 30. Krüger O. Dissecting common buzzard lifespan and lifetime reproductive success: the relative importance of food, competition, weather, habitat and individual attributes. Oecologia. 2002;133(4):474–82. pmid:ISI:000179733800005.
  31. 31. Dreiss AN, Antoniazza S, Burri R, Fumagalli L, Sonnay C, Frey C, et al. Local adaptation and matching habitat choice in female barn owls with respect to melanic coloration. J Evol Biol. 2012;25(1):103–14. pmid:WOS:000298013700009.
  32. 32. Carranza J. Sex allocation within broods: the intrabrood sharing-out hypothesis. Behav Ecol. 2004;15(2):223–32. pmid:ISI:000220485100007.
  33. 33. Chakarov N, Krüger O. Mesopredator release by an emergent superpredator: A natural experiment of predation in a three level guild. PLoS One. 2010;5(12):e15229. pmid:ISI:000284995300023.
  34. 34. Krüger O. The importance of competition, food, habitat, weather and phenotype for the reproduction of Buzzard Buteo buteo. Bird Stud. 2004;51:125–32. pmid:ISI:000222597600005.
  35. 35. Chakarov N, Linke B, Boerner M, Goesmann A, Kruger O, Hoffman JI. Apparent vector-mediated parent-to-offspring transmission in an avian malaria-like parasite. Mol Ecol. 2015;24(6):1355–63. pmid:WOS:000351465300014.
  36. 36. Krüger O. Interactions between common buzzard Buteo buteo and goshawk Accipiter gentilis: trade-offs revealed by a field experiment. Oikos. 2002;96(3):441–52. pmid:ISI:000175112200006.
  37. 37. Boerner M, Hoffman JI, Amos W, Chakarov N, Krüger O. Testing heterozygosity-fitness-correlations and inbreeding in colour morphs of different fitness in the common buzzard J Evol Biol. 2013;26:2233–43.
  38. 38. Newton I. Population ecology of raptors. Berkhamsted: Poyser; 1979.
  39. 39. Sergio F, Newton I. Occupancy as a measure of territory quality. J Anim Ecol. 2003;72(5):857–65. pmid:ISI:000185063900016.
  40. 40. Krüger O, Chakarov N, Nielsen JT, Looft V, Grünkorn T, Struwe-Juhl B, et al. Population regulation by habitat heterogeneity or individual adjustment? J Anim Ecol. 2012;81(2):330–40. pmid:WOS:000300502600004.
  41. 41. Reif V, Jungell S, Korpimäki E, Tornberg R, Mykra S. Numerical response of common buzzards and predation rate of main and alternative prey under fluctuating food conditions. Ann Zool Fenn. 2004;41(4):599–607. pmid:ISI:000224055500007.
  42. 42. Heise S, Wieland H. Zu den Methoden der Abundanzbestimmung bei Feldmauspopulationen als Grundlage eines umweltgerechten Pflanzenschutzes. Nachrichtenblatt Deutscher Pflanzenschutzdienst. 1991;43:30–3.
  43. 43. Bijlsma RG. Handleiding veldonderzoek roofvogels. Utrecht: KNNV Uitgeverij; 1997.
  44. 44. Fridolfsson AK, Ellegren H. A simple and universal method for molecular sexing of non-ratite birds. J Avian Biol. 1999;30(1):116–21. pmid:WOS:000079569700017.
  45. 45. Newton I, Marquiss M. Sex-ratio among nestlings of the European Sparrowhawk. Am Nat. 1979;113(2):309–15. pmid:WOS:A1979GG92600014.
  46. 46. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd edn. ed. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 2002.
  47. 47. R Development Core Team. A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014.
  48. 48. Neuhäuser M. Tests for a biased sex ratio when the data are clustered. Environ Ecol Stat. 2004;11(3):295–304. pmid:WOS:000223332600004.
  49. 49. Hamilton WD. Extraordinary sex ratios. Science. 1967;156(3774):477–88. pmid:WOS:A19679243000012.
  50. 50. Aparicio JM, Cordero PJ. The effects of the minimum threshold condition for breeding on offspring sex-ratio adjustment in the lesser kestrel. Evolution. 2001;55(6):1188–97. pmid:ISI:000169903000012.
  51. 51. Jacquin L, Recapet C, Bouche P, Leboucher G, Gasparini J. Melanin-based coloration reflects alternative strategies to cope with food limitation in pigeons. Behav Ecol. 2012;23(4):907–15. pmid:WOS:000305630800028.
  52. 52. Byholm P, Kekkonen M. Food regulates reproduction differently in different habitats: Experimental evidence in the Goshawk. Ecology. 2008;89(6):1696–702. pmid:WOS:000256540300022.
  53. 53. Byholm P. Site-specific variation in partial brood loss in northern goshawks. Ann Zool Fenn. 2005;42(2):81–90. pmid:WOS:000228907500001.
  54. 54. Rutz C. Brood sex ratio varies with diet composition in a generalist raptor. Biol J Linnean Soc. 2012;105(4):937–51. pmid:WOS:000301114900016.