Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Gender-Specific Differences in the Relationship between Autobiographical Memory and Intertemporal Choice in Older Adults

  • Maayke Seinstra ,

    Maayke.Seinstra@hhu.de

    Affiliation Department of Comparative Psychology, Institute of Experimental Psychology, Heinrich-Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany

  • Katharina Grzymek,

    Affiliation Department of Comparative Psychology, Institute of Experimental Psychology, Heinrich-Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany

  • Tobias Kalenscher

    Affiliation Department of Comparative Psychology, Institute of Experimental Psychology, Heinrich-Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany

Abstract

As the population of older adults grows, their economic choices will have increasing impact on society. Research on the effects of aging on intertemporal decisions shows inconsistent, often opposing results, indicating that yet unexplored factors might play an essential role in guiding one's choices. Recent studies suggest that episodic future thinking, which is based on the same neural network involved in episodic memory functions, leads to reductions in discounting of future rewards. As episodic memory functioning declines with normal aging, but to greatly variable degrees, individual differences in delay discounting might be due to individual differences in the vitality of this memory system in older adults. We investigated this hypothesis, using a sample of healthy older adults who completed an intertemporal choice task as well as two episodic memory tasks. We found no clear evidence for a relationship between episodic memory performance and delay discounting in older adults. However, when additionally considering gender differences, we found an interaction effect of gender and autobiographical memory on delay discounting: while men with higher memory scores showed less delay discounting, women with higher memory scores tended to discount the future more. We speculate that this gender effect might stem from the gender-specific use of different modal representation formats (i.e. temporal or visual) during assessment of intertemporal choice options.

Introduction

You are retired. Would you now finally spend your money on small pleasures right now, or rather save for that new car you always dreamed of having? Throughout our lives we make countless choices of this kind where the outcomes become available over time, for example when we decide to refrain from eating the tasty hamburger to go for the healthy salad instead. As these intertemporal decisions can have far reaching consequences, it is important to understand how our preferences develop over time and which factors influence our choice behavior.

Intertemporal decision making is usually assessed using monetary incentives available at specific points in the future. Because a delay has a negative effect on the subjective value of a reward, amounts available in the future are worth less (i.e. are discounted) compared to when they are received now. It is commonly found that the value of a monetary amount (or other goods) is discounted in a hyperbolic fashion [1, 2], with initial steep discounting of reward values with short delays to reward consumption, and flatter discounting with longer delays. The initial steep decline in the discount function is often related to the characteristic ‘present-bias’ which relates to the tendency to reverse preferences in favor of immediate gratification at the expense of meeting long-term goals. The general steepness of the discount function can be used as an index of subjective ‘(im)patience’, i.e., the general sensitivity to delays.

Our time preferences change with age [37]. Studies on the effects of age on discounting often find increased discounting in children and adolescents, with decreasing discount rates in adulthood [811]. The change in discounting across childhood and adolescence presumably reflects the maturation process of prefrontal areas important for executive control. With regard to older aged groups, evidence is inconsistent: while some studies found no difference in discount rates between older and younger adults [10, 12, 13], other studies suggest increasing discount rates with age [14, 15], yet others found decreasing discount rates [8, 9, 11, 16, 17]. Moreover, the findings seem to depend on the type of reward (primary or secondary) as well [16]. For very old adults, it can be argued that the shortened life expectancy renders the preference for long term outcomes more risky because they may not live to experience the realization of the future outcome [18]. On the other hand, a lifetime of decision making might increase the ability to delay gratification because the older adults have learned the value of patience through experience [8, 17, 19, 20].

However, a third possibility to explain the inconsistency in the results on old age and discounting is the great variability in age-related decline of decision-relevant mental and neural functionality: older age comes with neuronal changes in areas involved in reward processing and decision making [6, 2126, 27, 28]. It is therefore possible that changes in discounting in older individuals depend on the (variable) degree in cognitive decline associated with older age [29]. One mental function that declines with age is episodic thinking [30, 31]. Interestingly, several studies have linked episodic future thinking, i.e., mental time travel, or imagining possible future outcomes, to delay discounting behavior [3235]. The core finding of these studies is that episodic future thinking goes along with decreased delay discounting [3335], supporting the hypothesis that the better one is able to imagine the future outcome, the higher the subjective valuation of that future outcome will be.

These and other studies indicated that imagining future events activates the same core neural network that is involved in episodic memory functioning [36, 37], supporting the theory that the episodic memory system is used to create images of future events in the mind’s eye [3841]. It has been suggested that to assess whether a delayed reward is the most preferable option, one needs to have a representation of future states of oneself to determine how valuable that reward will be, and use this representation to maintain motivation for overcoming short-term temptations [35, 42, 43]. This assessment could depend on recalling similar rewards obtained in the past.

Thus, because episodic future simulation and episodic memory draw on similar neural systems, and because delay discounting might depend on recalling rewards from the past, it is tempting to speculate that episodic memory performance affects discounting behavior. However, behavioral studies linking delay discounting with episodic simulation focused on the episodic projection of future, not the recall of past events. In addition, memory recall processes that bias decisions do not necessarily need to be conscious or effortful [44]. It is therefore unclear whether more general memory processes or episodic memory mechanisms in particular play a significant role in determining discounting levels in situations where episodic future thinking is not explicitly prompted in the choice task.

Not only future rewarding events, but also immediate rewards could trigger memory processes that do not necessarily have to be episodic. High integrity of the memory network, including the hippocampus, would in that case not necessarily lead to a decrease in discounting. On the other hand, unconscious preferences for more profitable delayed rewards, potentially due to a general bias towards long-term thinking in our society, could render (unconscious) memory retrieval mechanisms an important factor for biasing choice towards delayed rewards. This would be in line with findings of Kwan et al. [45], who found that in persons with hippocampal amnesia, future-orientated decision making was relatively intact, whereas they were unable to imagine detailed future events. A more recent study found that, even though persons with hippocampal amnesia show similar discounting as healthy controls, when prompted to imagine spending future rewards amnesic patients did not show decreased discounting, whereas healthy controls did [46]. This is in line with the idea that episodic future thinking is one of the factors that influences intertemporal decision making, but shows that engagement of the hippocampal network is not a necessary requirement for discounting.

The current study could shed more light on whether episodic memory retrieval processes influence intertemporal decision making without explicitly triggering future simulation. Evidence for the importance of episodic memory, i.e. the storage and retrieval of past episodes, for delay discounting is elusive. In order to test this idea, it would be desirable to have a population sample with a substantial degree of variability in episodic memory performance. As mentioned, this is the case in older subjects: several studies have shown that episodic memory functioning declines with age [30, 31], but the extent of decline differs strongly between individuals [47, 48]. We therefore aimed to investigate the relationship between episodic memory functioning and discount behavior in older aged individuals. We hypothesize that age-related variability in episodic memory performance may be an important mediating variable on discounting behavior that could explain some of the discrepancies found in the literature.

Our study was designed to investigate the relationship between episodic memory performance and delay discounting in a group of older adults. We expected that episodic memory performance correlated with decreased discounting when memory performance was higher. Overall, our results do not support our hypothesis. However, we additionally explored the role of gender, as several studies have shown gender effects in episodic memory tasks [4951, 52] and found an interaction effects of gender and autobiographical memory on discounting.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-two older adults (33 female) between 60 and 89 years (M = 72.60, SD = 6.47) were recruited from an internal database of the Institute of Experimental Psychology at Heinrich-Heine-University Düsseldorf. Of this sample, all participants denied to suffer from any neurological or psychiatric disease or to have been taking psychiatric medication at any time in their life. None of the participants used drugs or exceeded the limits of low-risk alcohol use (> 20 g alcohol per day for women; > 40 g alcohol per day for men). Of the three smokers in the sample, all smoked less than 20 cigarettes a day. All participants were German native speakers and scored at least 25 points (M = 28.37, SD = 1.54) in the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, [53]). With the exception of two, all participants were retired.

Results of four participants were excluded from analyses due to incorrectly answering catch trials in the intertemporal choice task (see below). The results reported below are therefore based on a sample of 58 adults (30 female) between 60 and 89 years (M = 72.57, SD = 6.39). See Table 1 for further demographic information. Participants received a general allowance of 5 Euro immediately after participation. Additionally, participants were paid according to their choice in the intertemporal choice task between 10 Euro tomorrow and 20 Euro in 9 months. All payments were made by checks and given directly (allowance) or sent to participant’s home address either 1 day or 9 months after the date of participation. Checks could be cashed at any bank of choice.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive variables of the complete sample and the gender subgroups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.t001

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Department of Experimental Psychology at the University of Düsseldorf. Participants were informed about the course of the study, their right to quit the study at any moment for any reason as well as the payment procedure, and all provided written informed consent prior to data collection.

Materials

Intertemporal choice task.

Temporal discounting was assessed by a computer-based intertemporal choice task implemented using the MATLAB Toolbox Cogent 2000 (developed by the Cogent 2000 team at the FIL and the ICN). The task consisted of 6 randomized blocks of trials with financial offers that differed in the delays to the smaller, sooner and larger, later reward. In four blocks the delay to the sooner option was tomorrow, while the delay to the later option was 3, 6, 9 or 12 months. In the remaining two blocks the sooner option was delayed for 6 months, while the later option was delayed for 9 (block 5) or 12 months (block 6). The delay of the soonest reward was set to tomorrow instead of today to prevent potential effects of transaction costs on decisions; the expectation of receiving cash payment directly instead by check might bias choices towards immediate rewards. Within each block the later option was fixed at 20 Euro, whereas the sooner option varied between 0 and 20 Euro in steps of 2.50 Euro. Each of the immediate reward values was presented twice within each block, yielding 18 trials per block and 108 trials in total.

Trials in which the immediate reward was either 0 or 20 Euro functioned as catch trials, as the preference within these trials should logically be the delayed (e.g., €0 now versus €20 in six months), or immediate reward (e.g., €20 now versus €20 in six months), respectively. Within each trial one option was shown on the left side of the screen (53 cm x 30 cm), while the other was shown on the right side. The side-allocation of smaller-sooner or larger-later rewards was randomized across trials. Choices were indicated by pressing either the ‘x’ or ‘m’ key on a standard keyboard, corresponding to the left or right option shown. The relevant keys were color-coded. Participants received detailed oral and visual instructions before the task was started. It was emphasized that there were no right or wrong answers and their personal preference should guide their decisions. Before the start of each new block the subsequent delays were shown. Participants were told that one of the trials would be randomly picked and their choice paid out by means of checks after the corresponding delay. The final screen of the task displayed their earnings.

Episodic memory performance.

Episodic memory can be defined as the conscious recollection of past personal events linked to a particular temporal and spatial context (e.g. [54]). In the literature, episodic memory is often decomposed into several sub-functions: associative memory, autobiographical memory for personal events, and autobiographical memory for personal facts and dates. Associative memory is the ability to learn and remember the relationship between unrelated items and is known to rely strongly on the hippocampus [5557], autobiographical memory refers to the 'meaningful reconstruction of one's own past' [58]. Although autobiographical memory for facts and dates shares the personal component with episodic memory, as well as neural correlates in specific operationalizations (see [59]), it has also been related to semantic memory, and termed 'personal semantics' [59]. We assessed memory performance using independent standard tests for associative and autobiographical memory, as explained in the following paragraphs.

Associative memory task.

Associative memory performance was measured with a face-name paired-associates (FNPA) task, in which new face-name associations need to be stored in memory and subsequently remembered. This task was implemented using the online survey tool Unipark (QuestBack GmbH, Hürth, Germany). It consisted of 4 blocks, each with 10 encoding, 3 subsequent distraction and 10 retrieval trials. The encoding trials consisted of 10 face-name pairs successively presented for 4 seconds each, followed by a fixation dot for 1 second (see Fig 1). Participants were instructed to memorize the name belonging to each face. The face-name pairs were only shown once in random order and were not repeated across blocks. To prevent rehearsal, the encoding trials were followed by the distraction trials, which consisted of mathematical equations presented with a solution that was either correct or false. In each trial participants had to indicate by a mouse click whether the proposed solution was correct or false. In the following retrieval block ten face-name pairs were presented in randomized order, consisting of the same names and faces as shown during the previous encoding trials. However, only half of the trials showed the correct face-name combinations from the first round, the other half contained incorrect face-name combinations. In each trial participants had to indicate with a mouse click whether the face-name pair was correct or false. Blocks were intermitted by a short break of 10 seconds. Before starting the experiment participants received detailed instructions and performed a test trial to practice handling the mouse and to make sure that they had understood the instructions. In addition, block-specific instructions were shown at the beginning of each block.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Schematic overview of the Face-Name Paired-Associates (FNPA) task.

Participants went through four blocks, consisting of encoding, distraction and retrieval trials. At the beginning of each block participants were required to memorize ten face-name pairs. These were followed by three distraction trials, in which participants had to indicate whether the shown equation was correct or false. Each block ended with ten retrieval trials, in which participants indicated whether the shown face-name pair was correct or false. The faces shown in the figure are from two exemplary persons of the FACES database [60].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.g001

The 40 face stimuli used in this task were taken from the FACES database [60] and were unknown to the participants. The stimuli included 14 young (19–28 years), 12 middle-aged (39–54 years) and 14 older (69–80 years) faces with an equal number of male and female faces from each age group and within each block. The fictional names were taken from public lists of popular German forenames and were carefully chosen and assigned to the faces so that they were not suggestive of the person’s age.

Autobiographical memory task.

The participants’ ability to remember episodes from their past was tested using Module C of the Inventar zur Gedächtnisdiagnostik (IGD, [61]) which is a German memory inventory. While Modules A and B measure memory retention and semantic memory, Module C was specifically designed to capture autobiographical memory. Module C again is divided into two sub modules, C1 and C2. While C1 measures memory performance and memory quality for personal events, C2 captures memory performance and quality for dates and facts related to oneself and one’s personal environment.

Sub-module C1 required participants to describe several personal events that occurred before the age of 6, between the age of 7 and 16, from the age of 17 until one year ago and within the last 12 months. The described events must be concrete (i.e. restricted in time and location). For example, having been at college was not counted as an event, whereas the first party at college was. Participants were instructed only to recall events they really remembered and not just knew from photos, movies or narratives. To ensure that the recalled events fulfilled the requirements and avoid biases due to differences in writing speed, sub module C1 was implemented as interview. A time limit was set to 4 minutes, in which the participants had the chance to describe a maximum of 5 events per life episode. The interviewer was instructed not to prompt or give any hints to facilitate recall, and wrote down the recalled events in note form. Before continuing to the next episode, participants rated one of the recalled events on its vividness, specificity and emotionality, each on a 4-point scale. The to-be-rated event was specified in the test.

Submodule C2 contained 64 items about 11 different general life-related topics (i.e. work, transportation, education). For each item participants had to indicate by ticking ‘yes’ or ‘no’ whether they were able to remember a certain date or fact like, for instance, their partner’s or child’s date of birth, previous home addresses or how they used to travel to school or work. If participants answered ‘yes’ they additionally had to rate how confident they were about the specific memory. Questions about topics that did not apply to the participant (e.g. questions about children with childless subjects) were skipped. There was no time restriction for this part of the IGD.

Although Baller et al. [61] evaluate the content validity of Module C to be sufficient, it should nevertheless be admitted that the participants’ descriptions, answers and ratings could not be verified and were therefore interpreted with caution.

General level of intelligence.

To control for potential confounding effects of general intelligence, the 10-Minutes-Test [62] was used as a short measure of fluid as well as crystallized intelligence. The test includes 32 items that require mathematical and deductive reasoning or general knowledge and vocabulary. The total test score corresponds to the sum of all items that were solved correctly within 10 minutes. This relatively short implementation time allows for a fast, yet valid and reliable estimate of general intelligence. Beside its objective administration and interpretation, the test was shown to have a high loading on Spearman’s g factor with r = .57 [63]. First validation studies also found a high internal consistency of Cronbach’s α > .80 and significant correlations with several other cognitive measures [62, 64]. To date the 10-Minutes-Test has only been standardized for pupils and students. However, due to the lack of alternative short intelligence screening methods and the possibility that a long test session might overstrain the cognitive capacity of older participants, the 10-Minutes-Test was considered to be suitable for our purposes.

Dementia.

Participants were screened for dementia or severe cognitive impairment using the mini-mental state examination (MMSE). This interview assesses global cognitive abilities like orientation, attention and memory as well as numerical and language skills. Participants with scores lower than 13 out of 30, which indicate global cognitive disorders, were excluded from participation. All screened participants had a score above 13.

Post-test questionnaire.

General demographic information (education, job status, income and lifestyle habits) was obtained with a post-test questionnaire.

Procedure

The experiment took place in a laboratory at Heinrich-Heine-University Düsseldorf. Participants were tested individually in one 90 minute session. Participants were given verbal and written information on the procedure of the experiment before giving informed written consent. All participants were screened for dementia before performing the tasks. The order of tasks was determined depending on their importance and degree of difficulty, with the most demanding tasks set at the beginning. Thus, participants started with the intertemporal choice task, followed by the FNPA task, the 10-Minutes-Test, and finally the IGD task. In the end, participants filled out the post-test questionnaire and received their general show-up fee of 5 Euro. In addition, they were given a signed receipt stating that the remaining amount earned in the intertemporal choice task, to be received at the specified date, would be sent in the form of a check by post.

Data analysis

Delay discounting.

All mathematical procedures to determine the participants’ discount rates were performed using the MATLAB (The MathWorks, Inc.). First of all, we identified, for each of the six blocks, the individual indifference points (IPs; the amount for the smaller, sooner reward that renders the smaller, sooner reward equally valuable as the larger, later reward) using logistic regression. For further analysis, all IPs were converted into proportions of the late reward of 20 Euro.

We fitted two different models to the estimated IPs of blocks 1 to 4. First, we fitted the standard hyperbolic model [1, 65, 66]: (1) where SV is the subjective value of the reward, A is the monetary amount of the reward and T is the delay in months. The amount was set to A = 1 as the values were expressed as proportions of the later reward. Larger k-values indicate a greater impact of delay on value and therefore steeper discounting.

In addition, Laibson’s [67] quasi-hyperbolic β-δ model was fitted to the indifference point to obtain measures of present-bias and patience: (2)

SVt is the subjective value of a reward at time T. This equation models the often found initial rapid decline in subjective value with small delays (present-bias) separately, represented by the parameter β (with 0 ≤ β ≤ 1). The inverse of β can be interpreted as the extra weight added to immediacy, thus smaller β-values can be construed as stronger present-bias. In our analysis, T = 0 corresponds to ‘tomorrow’, as this was the soonest option available in our task. We opted to define tomorrow, and not today, as the soonest option to control for potential transaction costs, and assumed that tomorrow would be part of the extended present [68]. The discount function’s discount rate is log(1/δ). Thus, the parameter δ (with 0 ≤ δ ≤ 1) can be interpreted as a measure of patience with higher δ-values indicating higher patience.

The hyperbolic and quasi-hyperbolic models were fit to the first four indifference points, which were implemented as proportions of the delayed reward (e.g. an indifference point of 10 Euro would yield a proportion of .5 relative to an immediately available 20 Euro reward) for each participant individually, using a least-squares algorithm implemented in MATLAB R2013a (The MathWorks, Inc.). The fitting parameters k, β and δ were allowed to vary freely. Fig 2 shows the average indifference points of the whole sample as well as their hyperbolic and quasi-hyperbolic fits. Goodness of fit analyses using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), which takes into account the number of parameters, showed that the data was better described by Laibson’s quasi-hyperbolic model (M = -27.8) than the standard hyperbolic model (M = -11.5).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Illustrative hyperbolic and quasi-hyperbolic model fit to the average choice data of the whole sample.

The indifference points at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months and one day, averaged across participants, were used to fit the hyperbolic model (light gray line) and the quasi-hyperbolic model (dark gray and dashed line). Errorbars show the standard error of the mean (s.e.m.). The steepness of the hyperbolic function is reflected by parameter k. The dark grey line of the quasi-hyperbolic model represents present-bias and is reflected by parameter β, whereas further decline in value (dashed line) is reflected by ‘patience’ parameter δ.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.g002

We furthermore conducted additional analyses with several model-free parameters, which yielded similar results (see S1 Text).

Associative and autobiographical memory performance.

The four retrieval blocks of the FNPA task contained 20 correctly and 20 falsely paired face-name pairs. Correct face-name pairs that were recognized as such were counted as hits (FNPA-Hits), whereas face-name pairs that were actually false but judged as correct were counted as false alarms (FNPA-FA). Overall associative memory performance (FNPA-PF) was calculated by subtracting false alarms from hits (FNPA-PF = FNPA-Hits—FNPA-FA).

Module C of the IGD was analyzed according to the standard procedure suggested in the test manual [61]. The overall score of sub module C1 (IGD-C1) consists of the total number of events recalled proportional to the maximum of 20 recalled events, added to the sum of the quality rating scores from all four episodes proportional to the maximum rating score of 36. The score of sub module C2 (IGD-C2) is the proportion of all items answered with yes relative to the total number of answered items added to the proportion of quality rating scores from all answered areas.

Statistical analyses.

Statistical analyses reported below were performed using the software package IBM SPSS Statistics 20. The main analysis consisted of OLS regressions using the discounting parameters as dependent variables and the memory scores and mediator/moderator variables age, IQ and income as predictors. We used three models. In the first model, (3) we check for the effects of the memory scores on the discounting parameters. In the second model the mediator/moderator variables and gender were added: (4)

In the third model, three interaction terms of gender and memory performance were added: (5)

Missing data (see results) was replaced using the Expectation-Maximization procedure [69] to ensure inclusion of all participants in the regression analyses.

In addition, several correlation analyses were performed (see supplemental material). Where necessary, the significance level α was adjusted using the Holm-Bonferroni method to control the familywise error rate.

Results

We excluded participants from further analysis when more than half of the catch trials in the intertemporal choice task were answered incorrectly. We assumed that this indicated insufficient attention or understanding of the task. Four participants met this criterion and were therefore excluded from further analyses, rendering the overall sample size at n = 58. However, the main results reported in the following sections did not change when these participants were included. Furthermore, income data was missing in 6 participants.

Since we also found gender effects (see below), results are presented for the complete sample as well as the male (n = 28) and female (n = 30) subsamples. Table 1 summarizes the descriptive variables age, IQ and income for the complete, male and female group. The gender subgroups only differed in their income, with the women subgroup earning significantly less than men, t(56) = 3.942, p < .001. In addition, Table 2 shows the averages and standard deviations for the memory task scores and the discount parameter values, for the complete group as well as the gender subgroups. As predicted, women scored higher than men on the episodic memory tasks, but there was no significant gender difference in discount behavior. Correlations of the different episodic memory scores for men and women are summarized in S2 Table.

thumbnail
Table 2. Intertemporal choice and episodic memory scores of the complete sample and the gender subgroups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.t002

Since we expected our participant sample to show variable memory scores that would reflect the level of cognitive decline related to healthy aging, we checked for correlations between the memory scores FNPA-PF, IGD-C1 and IGD-C2 and age, as well as IQ and income. Results are shown in Table 3. Only the FNPA-PF scores showed a close-to-significant negative correlation with age, r = -.308, p = .018 > α = .017, r2 = .09, suggesting that older participants have lower scores on the FNPA task.

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlations of memory scores with age, IQ and income within the complete sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.t003

Regression analyses

Separate regression analyses with the discounting parameters ln(k), β and δ as dependent variables and memory scores (FNPA-PF, IGD-C1 and IGD-C2) as predictors show no significant contribution of any memory score on the model-based discounting values on group level, ln(k): F(3,54) = .153, p = .928, R2 = .008; βF(3,54) = .067, p = .977, R2 = .004; δ:F(3,54) = .272, p = .846, R2 = .015 (Table 4). Thus, our results did not support the hypothesis that a better functioning episodic memory system is associated with reduced discounting.

thumbnail
Table 4. Relationship of memory scores, moderator variables and gender x memory interactions with discounting parameters.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.t004

The second regression model showed no significant contribution of gender, age or IQ on discounting parameters ln(k), β and δ, whereas the variable income significantly predicted ln(k) (beta = -.427, p = .005) and β (beta = .326, p = .009) (Table 4). A higher income was related to lower values of k and thus less discounting. This was reflected by a similar significant effect of income on the number of impulsive choices (NImp) (see S1 Table). Similarly, a higher income was associated with higher values of β, indicating a lower present-bias.

To further explore a potential effect of gender, interaction terms were calculated by multiplying centered memory scores with gender. The addition of the three interaction terms (gender x FNPA-PF, gender x IGD-C1 and gender x IGD-C2) in the third regression model revealed several interaction effects (Table 4). The interaction between gender and IGD-C2 scores significantly predicted discount parameters ln(k) (beta = 1.129, p = .007) as well as β (beta = -3.538, p = .001) (Table 4), indicating that in males, a higher scores for autobiographical fact and date recall went along with less discounting, whereas in females higher recall scores went along with more discounting (see Fig 3 and Fig 4). The increase in R2 was significant compared to the second regression model for both ln(k), F(3,47) = 3.292, p = .029, and β, F(3,47) = 4.371, p = .009, as dependent variable. As gender is a binary variable, there was a high correlation between the interaction terms and the corresponding memory terms. Therefore, the significant effects of the memory measures in regression model 3 should be ignored.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Interaction effects of gender and IGD-C2 scores on ln(k).

(A) Scatterplot with regression line of the IGD-C2 and ln(k) scores in the male subsample. (B) Scatterplot with regression line of the IGD-C2 and ln(k) scores in the female subsample. (C) To further illustrate the gender-dependent differences in the relationship between memory performance and discounting, we performed a median split to categorize participants according to their IGD-C2 performance (high- vs. low performers). Individual bars show mean ln(k) values for subgroups with high and low IGD-C2 scores. Error bars show the standard error of the mean (s.e.m.).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Interaction effects of gender and IGD-C2 scores on parameter β.

(A) Scatterplot with regression line of the IGD-C2 and βscores in the male subsample. (B) Scatterplot with regression line of the IGD-C2 and βscores in the female subsample. (C) To further illustrate the gender-dependent differences in the relationship between memory performance and discounting, we performed a median split to categorize participants according to their IGD-C2 performance (high- vs. low performers). Individual bars show mean βvalues for subgroups with high and low IGD-C2 scores. Error bars show the standard error of the mean (s.e.m.).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.g004

Further, a similar significant interaction was found for IGD-C1 scores and the discounting parameter δ (beta = -.977, p = .038) (Table 4), indicating that better recall of autobiographical events went along with more patience in males, whereas in females better autobiographical memory predicted more impatience (see Fig 5). However, the model itself as well as the increase in R2 was not significant, F(3,47) = 1.540, p = .217.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Interaction effects of gender and IGD-C1 scores on parameter δ.

(A) Scatterplot with regression line of the IGD-C1 and δ scores in the male subsample. (B) Scatterplot with regression line of the IGD-C1 and δ scores in the female subsample. (C) To further illustrate the gender-dependent differences in the relationship between memory performance and discounting, we performed a median split to categorize participants according to their IGD-C1 performance (high- vs. low performers). Individual bars show mean δ values for subgroups with high and low IGD-C1 scores. Error bars show the standard error of the mean (s.e.m.).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.g005

Discussion

The aim of this experiment was to investigate the relationship between episodic memory and delay discounting in older adults. We hypothesized that episodic memory performance correlated with intertemporal choice behavior. Overall, we found no evidence for a relationship between episodic memory scores and time discounting. However, when considering gender differences, we found several interactions of gender and memory scores on discounting. First of all, we found that higher memory scores for autobiographical facts and dates, but not associative memory performance or memory for personal events, were related to a decreased level of discounting in men, as indicated by lower values of the hyperbolic discounting parameter k and higher values of present-bias parameter β. By contrast, in women, we found the opposite pattern; higher autobiographical memory scores for facts and dates were related to higher levels of discounting, as indicated by higher values of k and lower values of β. Furthermore, a similar interaction was found for gender and recall of autobiographical events on the discounting parameter δ, representing patience. Whereas men with better autobiographical event recall showed more patience, women with better autobiographical event recall showed less patience. These findings were not due to a general difference in discounting behavior between men and women.

Our hypothesis that episodic memory functioning and time discounting may be related was based on the finding that the core network involved in episodic memory functioning is also responsible for episodic future thinking, which has been related to lower rates of discounting [3335], e.g. through episodic tagging techniques. Here, we focused on memory retrieval of past episodes and events instead of eliciting future thinking. The question remains whether participants still use future simulation during intertemporal choice when not explicitly elicited.

If future rewards are indeed imagined when not being explicitly triggered during temporal discounting, one explanation of our findings could be that imagining future outcomes might depend on recall of past personal facts, rather than events, to form a new ‘image’ of the future. Although we do not find a general effect of autobiographical memory on discounting, we do find interaction effects of gender with autobiographical memory recall on discounting, with recall of events linked to patience, and recall of facts and dates linked to present-bias as well as the overall level of discounting.

Present-bias can be characterized using Laibson’s quasi-hyperbolic model [67] as the drop in subjective value between obtaining the monetary reward now and obtaining it in the nearest possible future. In contrast, the level of patience (reflected by parameter δ) gives an indication of how further delays affect the subjective value of the reward. Present-bias can be seen as a measure of intertemporal inconsistency, as a stronger present-bias indicates a larger deviation from constant time discounting (e.g. a linear or exponential decrease in value of a reward with time), due to a disproportionally strong focus on the present. That personal semantics might have a particular effect on present-bias makes sense with regard to the semantic content of any imagined future event, which would be similar regardless of the specific delay associated with it. As the parameter δ indicates further decline of value with increasing delays, this parameter is sensitive to the temporal context, which is an important aspect of a particular event and thus of episodic memory. It therefore makes sense that this parameter is related to recall of autobiographical events.

Previous literature has shown not only a dissociation between memory for autobiographical events and autobiographical facts and dates on behavioral level [70, 71], but also in underlying brain mechanisms [72, 73]. Just as retrieval of general facts and events, the retrieval of autobiographical facts is less dependent on the connectivity between the parahippocampal gyrus and hippocampus than the recall of autobiographical events [73]. Hence, recall mechanisms for autobiographical facts might have more in common with recall of general facts and events, which is less hippocampus-dependent, and less with recall of specific autobiographical events. For example, the recall of the means of transportation during one’s first job does not require recall of a specific time and space. Indeed, the memory of autobiographical facts and dates is also termed 'personal semantics' [59]. The specificity of the relationship between recall of autobiographical facts and dates and present-bias might therefore depend more on general memory retrieval mechanisms, and less on mechanisms specific for retrieval of highly context-dependent memories, such as autobiographical events.

That other (i.e. semantic or unconscious) long-term memory mechanisms could play a role is also supported by the absence of a relationship between associative memory performance and discounting. It is thought that with healthy aging, primarily the formation of new memories is affected, whereas older episodic memories are relatively preserved [74, 75]. The autobiographical memory task used here probably depends less on hippocampal functioning and more on neocortical integrity, as memory retrieval of items stored long ago in general seems to depend more on the latter [76, 77], whereas the associative memory task used here assessed both the formation and recall of newly formed face-name associations, which might rely most on hippocampal areas affected early with aging. In line with this view, we found a close-to-significant negative correlation between associative memory (FNPA) scores and age, but not between autobiographical memory scores and age. Why would older-aged men and women with better recall of autobiographical facts and dates/events show such opposing patterns regarding their present-bias/patience? It is possible that men differ from women regarding their “cognitive style” by which they make economic decisions. In a fMRI study by Piefke et al. [78], three brain areas were found to exhibit differential responses in men and women during autobiographical memory retrieval; whereas the parahippocampal region was more active in men, the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) as well as the right insular cortex showed increased activity in women compared to men. More recently, Young et al. [79] replicated the finding that women showed increased activity in the right dlPFC during autobiographical memory recall. Since there were no gender differences in behavioral performance, these findings likely support the “cognitive style hypothesis”, which states that men and women differ in the way they encode, rehearse and process emotional experiences, and exhibit differential response strategies during laboratory memory tasks [77, 79, 80].

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is one of the areas found to be important for episodic memory retrieval, and PFC functioning is related to recalling the temporal context of memories [8185]. An fMRI study by Suzuki et al. [85] indicated that the right dlPFC is predominantly engaged in recall of the temporal order of separate events, whereas the left dlPFC showed more engagement in recall of the temporal order within a specific event. It was therefore argued that the increased activity in the right dlPFC in women compared to men might reflect that women relied more strongly on the temporal context of autobiographical memories when recalling these events [77]. If this is indeed the case, it is possible that women are differentially sensitive to temporal information when it comes to imagining future rewards. This might explain why women with better recall for past autobiographical facts/events showed more sensitivity for the delays to future outcomes in our task, and as a result showed increased discounting.

The parahippocampal region has been shown to be involved in spatial learning, navigation and spatial context memory [8688]. In men, the increased activity found in this area during episodic memory tasks compared to women [78] could therefore point towards an increased role of spatial context, not only during recall of autobiographical events, but also when imagining future rewards. A general larger emphasis on spatial context processing in men, in combination with increased dependence of autobiographical memory recall performance on spatial context processing, could potentially explain why men, but not women, with better memory scores showed less discounting. It thus seems that men and women differ in their cognitive styles and strategies when performing episodic memory tasks [77, 79, 89], and arguably this difference might generalize to performance on other tasks requiring putatively similar cognitive strategies, such as future episodic simulation during delay discounting.

It is possible that this gender difference only occurs in older-aged individuals. One of the aims of this study was to investigate whether individual differences in episodic memory functioning and age-related decline could potentially explain the large variability in the effects of aging on time discounting. Although gender effects are not consistently found in time discounting, memory decline could give rise to differential choice preferences in males and females, which can bias the overall picture towards more or less discounting in older adults compared to younger groups. However, more research is necessary to shed light on this issue.

Recent studies [46, 90], in which patients with medial temporal lobe (MTL) lesions that showed impaired episodic future thinking completed an intertemporal choice task, indicated that processes involving the MTL are not essential for discounting, as these amnesic patients had similar discounting rates as control participants [45, 46, 90]. However, when amnesic patients were cued to imagine future events during the intertemporal choice task, they did not show decreased levels of discounting, whereas a healthy control group did show the decreased levels of discounting shown in previous studies [46]. Therefore, processes involving the MTL, such as episodic future thinking, may only play a role as moderators on intertemporal decision making [46, 90] and might therefore not necessarily be invoked by default.

Interestingly, when amnesic patients were asked to imagine personal future situations [90], instead of more general future events [46], they also show the attenuating effect of episodic future thinking on discounting, suggesting that different ‘types’ of future thinking depend more or less on MTL functioning. A study with patients with semantic dementia shows that episodic future thinking is critically dependent on semantic memory, as these patients showed relatively intact episodic memory for recent past events, but impaired episodic future thinking [91]. Kwan et al. [90] suggest this role of semantic memory could have been the cause of the differential effects of their study compared to the results found by Palombo et al. [46]; the personal cues might have triggered semantic future thinking instead of episodic future thinking [90], yielding similar reductions in discounting. This would be in line with our finding that memory for autobiographical facts and dates (i.e. personal semantics) is related to discounting.

Several additional measures were found to differ between the gender groups. In line with previous findings, we found that women scored higher on episodic memory tasks than men [30, 49, 50, 52]. Whether this is due to differences in verbal production/visuospatial processing [52] or possibly a difference in the richness of detail encoding between men and women [80] remains unclear, although our results showed that this gender effect is not limited to autobiographical events, as also associative memory performance showed a gender effect. In contrast, our results revealed no gender- difference in discounting, present-bias or patience in older subjects. Second, men and women differed in their income. This is potentially important as income is found to be related to time discounting [9, 14, 92, 93]. However, our results did not change when including income as additional variable in our partial correlation analyses, suggesting that income affected discounting independent of episodic memory.

In summary, we found no clear evidence for a general relationship between episodic memory and delay discounting in older-aged adults. However, we found a gender difference in this relationship: whereas men with better memory for autobiographical facts and dates/events showed less present-bias/more patience, women with better autobiographical memory were more present-biased/impatient. The finding that older-aged men and women with better autobiographical recall discount less, or more respectively, could be explained by assuming gender-differences in “cognitive styles” when making intertemporal decisions. As these interaction effects were not predicted, further behavioral studies should confirm these findings. Whether this interaction of gender and temporal discounting in older adults depends on neocortical integrity, in particular of the right dlPFC, hippocampus, or entirely different networks, requires verification using additional methods, such as fMRI.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Relationship of memory scores, moderator variables and gender x memory interactions on model-free discounting measures.

Each column represents one OLS regression. Dependent variables are shown in the column titles. Each row represents one predictor variable or statistics of the regression model. Cells show regression coefficients (beta) and p-values in brackets or model statistics. * p < .05. ** p < .01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Correlations of the different episodic memory tasks.

Correlation coefficients and p-values (in brackets) are reported. All p-values are two-tailed. * p < .025.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137061.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Axel Buchner for providing the participant database and Adam Schweda, Barbara Gorczyca and David Schymainski for their help during data collection.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: KG TK. Performed the experiments: KG MS. Analyzed the data: MS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MS TK. Wrote the paper: MS KG TK.

References

  1. 1. Mazur JE. Tests of an Equivalence Rule for Fixed and Variable Reinforcer Delays. J Exp Psychol Anim B. 1984;10(4):426–36. WOS:A1984TM70700002.
  2. 2. Kalenscher T, Pennartz CMA. Is a bird in the hand worth two in the future? The neuroeconomics of intertemporal decision-making. Progress in Neurobiology. 2008;84(3):284–315. ISI:000254598200005. pmid:18207301
  3. 3. Deakin J, Aitken M, Robbins T, Sahakian BJ. Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age. J Int Neuropsych Soc. 2004;10(4):590–8. WOS:000222367200012.
  4. 4. Denburg NL, Recknor EC, Bechara A, Tranel D. Psychophysiological anticipation of positive outcomes promotes advantageous decision-making in normal older persons. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;61(1):19–25. pmid:16426691.
  5. 5. Agarwal S, Gabaix X, Driscoll JC, Laibson D. The Age of Reason: Financial Decisions over the Life Cycle and Implications for Regulation. Brookings Pap Eco Ac. 2009;(2):51–117. WOS:000276644600003.
  6. 6. Samanez-Larkin GR, Kuhnen CM, Yoo DJ, Knutson B. Variability in Nucleus Accumbens Activity Mediates Age-Related Suboptimal Financial Risk Taking. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(4):1426–34. WOS:000274050000024. pmid:20107069
  7. 7. Worthy DA, Gorlick MA, Pacheco JL, Schnyer DM, Maddox WT. With Age Comes Wisdom: Decision Making in Younger and Older Adults. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(11):1375–80. WOS:000300826400003. pmid:21960248
  8. 8. Green L, Fry AF, Myerson J. Discounting of Delayed Rewards—a Life-Span Comparison. Psychol Sci. 1994;5(1):33–6. WOS:A1994MR35600007.
  9. 9. Reimers S, Maylor EA, Stewart N, Chater N. Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Pers Indiv Differ. 2009;47(8):973–8. WOS:000271484400032.
  10. 10. Whelan R, McHugh LA. Temporal Discounting of Hypothetical Monetary Rewards by Adolescents, Adults, and Older Adults. Psychol Rec. 2009;59(2):247–58. WOS:000265337300006.
  11. 11. Lockenhoff CE, O'Donoghue T, Dunning D. Age differences in temporal discounting: the role of dispositional affect and anticipated emotions. Psychology and aging. 2011;26(2):274–84. pmid:21534688.
  12. 12. Chao LW, Szrek H, Pereira NS, Pauly MV. Time preference and its relationship with age, health, and survival probability. Judgm Decis Mak. 2009;4(1):1–19. WOS:000263647400001. pmid:20376300
  13. 13. Green L, Myerson J, Lichtman D, Rosen S, Fry A. Temporal discounting in choice between delayed rewards: The role of age and income. Psychology and aging. 1996;11(1):79–84. WOS:A1996UE32300009. pmid:8726373
  14. 14. Harrison GW, Lau MI, Williams MB. Estimating individual discount rates in Denmark: A field experiment. Am Econ Rev. 2002;92(5):1606–17. WOS:000180306100018.
  15. 15. Read D, Read NL. Time discounting over the lifespan. Organ Behav Hum Dec. 2004;94(1):22–32. WOS:000221508100003.
  16. 16. Jimura K, Myerson J, Hilgard J, Keighley J, Braver TS, Green L. Domain independence and stability in young and older adults' discounting of delayed rewards. Behav Process. 2011;87(3):253–9. WOS:000293941300003.
  17. 17. Eppinger B, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. Reduced Sensitivity to Immediate Reward during Decision-Making in Older than Younger Adults. Plos One. 2012;7(5). ARTN e36953 WOS:000305338900007.
  18. 18. Sozou PD, Seymour RM. Augmented discounting: interaction between ageing and time-preference behaviour. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 2003;270(1519):1047–53. pmid:12803894; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1691344.
  19. 19. Logue AW, Rodriguez ML, Penacorreal TE, Mauro BC. Choice in a Self-Control Paradigm—Quantification of Experience-Based Differences. J Exp Anal Behav. 1984;41(1):53–67. WOS:A1984SB47700006. pmid:16812358
  20. 20. Samanez-Larkin GR, Mata R, Radu PT, Ballard IC, Carstensen LL, McClure SM. Age Differences in Striatal Delay Sensitivity during Intertemporal Choice in Healthy Adults. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2011;5:126. pmid:22110424; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3217179.
  21. 21. Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D, Dupuis JH, McQuain J, Briggs SD, et al. Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: Differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7(3):268–82. WOS:A1997WR58900006. pmid:9143446
  22. 22. Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Williamson A, et al. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(11):1676–89. WOS:000232595700003. pmid:15703252
  23. 23. Backman L, Nyberg L, Lindenberger U, Li SC, Farde L. The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: Current status and future prospects. Neurosci Biobehav R. 2006;30(6):791–807. WOS:000241208800007.
  24. 24. Mell T, Wartenburger I, Marschner A, Villringer A, Reischies FM, Heekeren HR. Altered function of ventral striatum during reward-based decision making in old age. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;3. Artn 34 WOS:000274618700004.
  25. 25. Weiler JA, Bellebaum C, Daum I. Aging affects acquisition and reversal of reward-based associative learning. Learning & memory. 2008;15(4):190–7. WOS:000254622600003.
  26. 26. Marschner A, Mell T, Wartenburger I, Villringer A, Reischies FM, Heekeren HR. Reward-based decision-making and aging. Brain Res Bull. 2005;67(5):382–90. WOS:000232862900006. pmid:16216684
  27. 27. Brown SBRE, Ridderinkhof KR. Aging and the neuroeconomics of decision making: A review. Cogn Affect Behav Ne. 2009;9(4):365–79. WOS:000272256100002.
  28. 28. Mohr PNC, Li SC, Heekeren HR. Neuroeconomics and aging: Neuromodulation of economic decision making in old age. Neurosci Biobehav R. 2010;34(5):678–88. WOS:000276722500006.
  29. 29. Boyle PA, Yu L, Segawa E, Wilson RS, Buchman AS, Laibson DI, et al. Association of cognition with temporal discounting in community based older persons. Bmc Geriatr. 2012;12. Artn 48 WOS:000313488400001.
  30. 30. Lundervold AJ, Wollschlager D, Wehling E. Age and sex related changes in episodic memory function in middle aged and older adults. Scandinavian journal of psychology. 2014;55(3):225–32. pmid:24601911.
  31. 31. Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of aging. 2009;30(4):507–14. WOS:000264579500003. pmid:19231028
  32. 32. Benoit RG, Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. A Neural Mechanism Mediating the Impact of Episodic Prospection on Farsighted Decisions. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(18):6771–9. WOS:000290212100019. pmid:21543607
  33. 33. Peters J, Buchel C. Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions. Neuron. 2010;66(1):138–48. WOS:000277016200015. pmid:20399735
  34. 34. Lin H, Epstein LH. Living in the Moment: Effects of Time Perspective and Emotional Valence of Episodic Thinking on Delay Discounting. Behav Neurosci. 2014;128(1):12–9. WOS:000331136200002. pmid:24512061
  35. 35. Lebreton M, Bertoux M, Boutet C, Lehericy S, Dubois B, Fossati P, et al. A Critical Role for the Hippocampus in the Valuation of Imagined Outcomes. Plos Biol. 2013;11(10). ARTN e1001684 WOS:000330350700016.
  36. 36. Schacter DL, Addis DR. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Philos T R Soc B. 2007;362(1481):773–86. WOS:000245599000003.
  37. 37. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC. The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci. 2007;30(3):299-+. WOS:000254847800033. pmid:17963565
  38. 38. Schacter DL, Addis DR. On the constructive episodic simulation of past and future events. Behav Brain Sci. 2007;30(3):331-+. WOS:000254847800053.
  39. 39. Schacter DL, Addis DR. The ghosts of past and future. Nature. 2007;445(7123):27-. WOS:000243225100020. pmid:17203045
  40. 40. Schacter DL, Addis DR. On the nature of medial temporal lobe contributions to the constructive simulation of future events. Philos T R Soc B. 2009;364(1521):1245–53. WOS:000264662000008.
  41. 41. Johnson A, van der Meer MA, Redish AD. Integrating hippocampus and striatum in decision-making. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2007;17(6):692–7. pmid:18313289; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3774291.
  42. 42. Boyer P. Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in cognitive sciences. 2008;12(6):219–24. pmid:18468941.
  43. 43. Rick S, Loewenstein G. Intangibility in intertemporal choice. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 2008;363(1511):3813–24. pmid:18829432; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2607360.
  44. 44. Wimmer GE, Shohamy D. Preference by Association: How Memory Mechanisms in the Hippocampus Bias Decisions. Science. 2012;338(6104):270–3. WOS:000309712300050. pmid:23066083
  45. 45. Kwan D, Craver CF, Green L, Myerson J, Rosenbaum RS. Dissociations in Future Thinking Following Hippocampal Damage: Evidence From Discounting and Time Perspective in Episodic Amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General. 2013;142(4):1355–69. WOS:000327108200028. pmid:23978187
  46. 46. Palombo DJ, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. The medial temporal lobes are critical for reward-based decision making under conditions that promote episodic future thinking. Hippocampus. 2014;25(3):345–53. pmid:25284804; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4331231.
  47. 47. Nyberg L, Lovden M, Riklund K, Lindenberger U, Backman L. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2012;16(5):292–305. pmid:22542563.
  48. 48. Ronnlund M, Nyberg L, Backman L, Nilsson LG. Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychology and aging. 2005;20(1):3–18. pmid:15769210.
  49. 49. Herlitz A, Nilsson LG, Backman L. Gender differences in episodic memory. Memory & cognition. 1997;25(6):801–11. pmid:9421566.
  50. 50. Kramer JH, Delis DC, Kaplan E, O'Donnell L, Prifitera A. Developmental sex differences in verbal learning. Neuropsychology. 1997;11(4):577–84. pmid:9345701.
  51. 51. Oberg C, Larsson M, Backman L. Differential sex effects in olfactory functioning: the role of verbal processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS. 2002;8(5):691–8. pmid:12164678.
  52. 52. Herlitz A, Rehnman J. Sex differences in episodic memory. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008;17(1):52–6. WOS:000252896200011.
  53. 53. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, Mchugh PR. Mini-Mental State—Practical Method for Grading Cognitive State of Patients for Clinician. J Psychiat Res. 1975;12(3):189–98. WOS:A1975AY27900005. pmid:1202204
  54. 54. Tulving E. Episodic memory and common sense: how far apart? Philos T Roy Soc B. 2001;356(1413):1505–15. WOS:000171436000016.
  55. 55. Davachi L, Wagner AD. Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: insights from relational and item-based learning. Journal of neurophysiology. 2002;88(2):982–90. pmid:12163547.
  56. 56. Eichenbaum H. Hippocampus: mapping or memory? Curr Biol. 2000;10(21):R785–7. pmid:11084350.
  57. 57. Suzuki WA. Making new memories: the role of the hippocampus in new associative learning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2007;1097:1–11. pmid:17413005.
  58. 58. Fink GR, Markowitsch HJ, Reinkemeier M, Bruckbauer T, Kessler J, Heiss WD. Cerebral representation of one's own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1996;16(13):4275–82. pmid:8753888.
  59. 59. Renoult L, Davidson PS, Palombo DJ, Moscovitch M, Levine B. Personal semantics: at the crossroads of semantic and episodic memory. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2012;16(11):550–8. pmid:23040159.
  60. 60. Ebner NC, Riediger M, Lindenberger U. FACES-A database of facial expressions in young, middle-aged, and older women and men: Development and validation. Behav Res Methods. 2010;42(1):351–62. WOS:000285917600036. pmid:20160315
  61. 61. Baller G, Brand M, Kalbe E, Kessler J. Inventar zur Gedächtnisdiagnostik (IGD). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. 2006.
  62. 62. Musch J, Ostapczuk M, Hilbig BE, Auer TS, Brandt M, Cüpper L, et al. 10-Minuten-Test [10-Minutes-Test]. Unpublished Test. University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany. 2009.
  63. 63. Ostapczuk M. Analytischer Test: Eine Validierungsstudie. Unpublished Master's thesis, University of Bonn, Germany. 2006.
  64. 64. Ostapczuk M, Musch J, Lieberei W. Der "Analytische Test": Validierung eines neuen eignungsdiagnostischen Instruments zur Erfassung von schlussfolgerndem Denken. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie. 2011;55(1–16).
  65. 65. Ainslie G. Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull. 1975;82(4):463–96. pmid:1099599.
  66. 66. Chung SH, Herrnstein RJ. Choice and Delay of Reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1967;10(1):67-&. WOS:A19678866400007. pmid:16811307
  67. 67. Laibson D. Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Q J Econ. 1997;112(2):443–77. WOS:A1997XC57800004.
  68. 68. Haushofer J, Cornelisse S, Seinstra M, Fehr E, Joels M, Kalenscher T. No Effects of Psychosocial Stress on Intertemporal Choice. Plos One. 2013;8(11). ARTN e78597 WOS:000327216200033.
  69. 69. Dempster AP, Laird NM, Rubin DB. Maximum Likelihood from Incomplete Data Via Em Algorithm. J Roy Stat Soc B Met. 1977;39(1):1–38. WOS:A1977DM46400001.
  70. 70. Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and aging. 2002;17(4):677–89. pmid:12507363.
  71. 71. Piolino P, Desgranges B, Benali K, Eustache F. Episodic and semantic remote autobiographical memory in ageing. Memory. 2002;10(4):239–57. pmid:12097209.
  72. 72. Maguire EA, Frith CD. Lateral asymmetry in the hippocampal response to the remoteness of autobiographical memories. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2003;23(12):5302–7. pmid:12832555.
  73. 73. Maguire EA, Mummery CJ, Buchel C. Patterns of hippocampal-cortical interaction dissociate temporal lobe memory subsystems. Hippocampus. 2000;10(4):475–82. pmid:10985287.
  74. 74. Rybash JM, Monaghan BE. Episodic and semantic contributions to older adults' autobiographical recall. The Journal of general psychology. 1999;126(1):85–96. pmid:10216971.
  75. 75. Schroots JJ, van Dijkum C, Assink MH. Autobiographical memory from a life span perspective. International journal of aging & human development. 2004;58(1):69–85. pmid:15248473.
  76. 76. Squire LR. Memory and the Hippocampus—a Synthesis from Findings with Rats, Monkeys, and Humans. Psychological review. 1992;99(2):195–231. WOS:A1992HP21900001. pmid:1594723
  77. 77. Piefke M, Fink GR. Recollections of one's own past: the effects of aging and gender on the neural mechanisms of episodic autobiographical memory. Anatomy and embryology. 2005;210(5–6):497–512. pmid:16172875.
  78. 78. Piefke M, Weiss PH, Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR. Gender differences in the functional neuroanatomy of emotional episodic autobiographical memory. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;24(4):313–24. pmid:15704151.
  79. 79. Young KD, Bellgowan PS, Bodurka J, Drevets WC. Functional neuroimaging of sex differences in autobiographical memory recall. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34(12):3320–32. pmid:22807028.
  80. 80. Seidlitz L, Diener E. Sex differences in the recall of affective experiences. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1998;74(1):262–71. pmid:9457787.
  81. 81. Kopelman MD, Stanhope N, Kingsley D. Temporal and spatial context memory in patients with focal frontal, temporal lobe, and diencephalic lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35(12):1533–45. pmid:9460723.
  82. 82. Cabeza R, Mangels J, Nyberg L, Habib R, Houle S, McIntosh AR, et al. Brain regions differentially involved in remembering what and when: a PET study. Neuron. 1997;19(4):863–70. pmid:9354332.
  83. 83. Cabeza R, Anderson ND, Houle S, Mangels JA, Nyberg L. Age-related differences in neural activity during item and temporal-order memory retrieval: a positron emission tomography study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12(1):197–206. pmid:10769316.
  84. 84. Henson RN, Shallice T, Dolan RJ. Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval: a functional MRI test of the monitoring hypothesis. Brain. 1999;122 (Pt 7):1367–81. pmid:10388802.
  85. 85. Suzuki, Fujii T, Tsukiura T, Okuda J, Umetsu A, Nagasaka T, et al. Neural basis of temporal context memory: a functional MRI study. NeuroImage. 2002;17(4):1790–6. pmid:12498752.
  86. 86. Maguire EA, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD. Learning to find your way: a role for the human hippocampal formation. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 1996;263(1377):1745–50. pmid:9025317.
  87. 87. Malkova L, Mishkin M. One-trial memory for object-place associations after separate lesions of hippocampus and posterior parahippocampal region in the monkey. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2003;23(5):1956–65. pmid:12629201.
  88. 88. Tsukiura T, Fujii T, Takahashi T, Xiao R, Sugiura M, Okuda J, et al. Medial temporal lobe activation during context-dependent relational processes in episodic retrieval: an fMRI study. Functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17(4):203–13. pmid:12395388.
  89. 89. St Jacques PL, Conway MA, Cabeza R. Gender differences in autobiographical memory for everyday events: retrieval elicited by SenseCam images versus verbal cues. Memory. 2011;19(7):723–32. pmid:20981611; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3190064.
  90. 90. Kwan D, Craver CF, Green L, Myerson J, Gao F, Black SE, et al. Cueing the personal future to reduce discounting in intertemporal choice: Is episodic prospection necessary? Hippocampus. 2015;25(4):432–43. pmid:25676022.
  91. 91. Irish M, Addis DR, Hodges JR, Piguet O. Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: evidence from semantic dementia. Brain. 2012;135(Pt 7):2178–91. pmid:22614246.
  92. 92. Anderson CL, Gugerty MK. Intertemporal Choice and Development Policy: New Evidence on Time-Varying Discount Rates from Vietnam and Russia. Dev Econ. 2009;47(2):123–46. WOS:000266696600001.
  93. 93. Eisenhauer JG, Ventura L. The prevalence of hyperbolic discounting: some European evidence. Appl Econ. 2006;38(11):1223–34. WOS:000238480600002.