Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effects of Age and Cognition on a Cross-Cultural Paediatric Adaptation of the Sniffin' Sticks Identification Test

  • Laís Orrico Donnabella Bastos ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Laís Orrico Donnabella Bastos, Laura Silveira-Moriyama

    Affiliations Complex motor disorders service, Neurology Department and Physical and Occupational Therapy Department, HC-FCM, University of Campinas, UNICAMP, Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil, Neurology Department, HC-FCM, University of Campinas, UNICAMP, Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil

  • Marilisa Mantovani Guerreiro ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Neurology Department, HC-FCM, University of Campinas, UNICAMP, Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil

  • Andrew John Lees ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Reta Lila Weston Institute of Neurological Studies, UCL Institute of Neurology, London, United Kingdom

  • Thomas T. Warner ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Reta Lila Weston Institute of Neurological Studies, UCL Institute of Neurology, London, United Kingdom

  • Laura Silveira-Moriyama

    Contributed equally to this work with: Laís Orrico Donnabella Bastos, Laura Silveira-Moriyama

    laurasilveira@gmail.com

    Current address: Neurology Department, HC-FCM, University of Campinas, UNICAMP, Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil

    Affiliations Complex motor disorders service, Neurology Department and Physical and Occupational Therapy Department, HC-FCM, University of Campinas, UNICAMP, Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil, Neurology Department, HC-FCM, University of Campinas, UNICAMP, Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil, Reta Lila Weston Institute of Neurological Studies, UCL Institute of Neurology, London, United Kingdom, Medicine Department, Universidade Nove de Julho, Uninove, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil

Abstract

Objectives

To study the effects of age and cognition on the performance of children aged 3 to 18 years on a culturally adapted version of the 16 item smell identification test from Sniffin' Sticks (SS16).

Methods

A series of pilots were conducted on 29 children aged 3 to 18 years old and 23 adults to produce an adapted version of the SS16 suitable for Brazilian children (SS16-Child). A final version was applied to 51 children alongside a picture identification test (PIT-SS16-Child) to access cognitive abilities involved in the smell identification task. In addition 20 adults performed the same tasks as a comparison group.

Results

The final adapted SS16-Child was applied to 51 children with a mean age of 9.9 years (range 3-18 years, SD=4.25 years), of which 68.3% were girls. There was an independent effect of age (p<0.05) and PIT-SS16-Child (p<0.001) on the performance on the SS16-Child, and older children reached the ceiling for scoring in the cognitive and olfactory test. Pre-school children had difficulties identifying items of the test.

Discussion/Conclusions

A cross-culturally adapted version of the SS16 can be used to test olfaction in children but interpretation of the results must take age and cognitive abilities into consideration.

Introduction

Various clinical diseases which manifest in the adult life are known to present with olfactory loss, including Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s type dementia, which both present with significant smell loss in more than 70% of patients [1]. Nevertheless, until fairly recently there was scarce literature on smell testing in children, and currently little is known about the occurrence of smell loss in neuropaediatric conditions. The limited literature on smell testing in children suggests that adult tests might be adapted to children, and that there is an association between children’s age and performance in the test with older children scoring higher [218], but there is no consensus on the minimum age limit for the application of smell identification tests.

Three main modalities of smell tests are used in the clinical and research practice: odour identification, odour discrimination and odour detection threshold [1922]. The most common task of odour detection threshold is the 3-alternative forced-choice in which the subject must detect which of three pens (or tubes) contains an odorant. The odorant comes in different concentrations and the threshold is the minimal concentration at which reliable detection is observed. In the tests of odour discrimination, the subject is again presented with triplets, and needs to discriminate which one contains a smell that is different from the other two. Smell identification is usually a forced choice task in which the subject must identify an odorant among a given number of written options (generally four). Although this is the most practical modality in the clinical setting due to ease of application, it is nevertheless more affected by familiarity with the items. This renders identification tasks more sensitive to cultural differences and careful adaptation and cultural validation is needed before it can be applied cross-culturally. In theory, this modality is strongly influenced by the olfactory learning which happens in children and significant adaptations are needed before a test which was developed for adults can be applied to a paediatric population.

The two most commonly used smell identification tests worldwide are the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT) [23] and the smell identification test from Sniffin' Sticks [24], both of which have been cross-culturally adapted for use in different countries, but only in the adult population. No smell test has been adapted for the Brazilian paediatric population to date. The University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification test has had two different adaptations to the Brazilian population yielding a final one which can be reliably applied to Brazilian adults [25,26], but no world-wide adaptation of this test exists for children. The 16 item smell identification test from Sniffin' Sticks (SS16) has been successfully adapted for the use in Brazilian adults [27].

The development of olfactory tests for children occurred at a slower rate than for the adult population. In contrast, the normative data for more than 2000 adults have been available for the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test in the US [23,28] for over 20 years, but the normative data for a pediatric test from the same group was only made available in 2013 [13]. Large European studies provided normative data for adult Europeans using the smell test battery form the Sniffin' Sticks (SS16) in 2007[29], and only recently Schriever et al [15] produced an adaptation of the 16 item smell identification test from Sniffin Sticks to German children older than 6 years, showing that 14 of the 16 items were well recognized by children (a subscale they called "Sniffin Kids"). A small number of studies evaluated the applicability of smell identification tests to children, and ours is the first to include subjects outside of Europe, North-America or Australia. Cultural factors strongly influence the performance on odour identification tests [26] and therefore in order to apply the SS16 or the “Sniffin Kids” to Brazilian children, modifications are likely to be necessary.

In addition to the influence of cultural factors on the performance of smell tests, cognitive status is a known determinant of smell identification performance in adults [22,30,31]. Nevertheless, the relationship between cognition and olfactory testing has not been systematically explored in children.

Thus, the objective of this study was to create an adapted version of SS16 suitable to a Brazilian paediatric population. A secondary objective was to examine how age and cognition (as measured by a simple cognitive screening task) affected the performance of children and therefore the interpretation of test results. This new version (called by us SS16-Child) was applied in children aged 3 to 18 years old alongside a cognitive test adapted from the Picture Identification Test (PIT), which is used for cognitive screening in adult populations who undergo the UPSIT [32].

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the Research and Ethics Committee of State University of Campinas (Comitê de Ética em Pesquisa da Faculdade de Ciências Médicas da Unicamp, CEP-FCM-UNICAMP) and was registered in the national registry at the National Commission for Ethics in Research (Comissão Nacional de Ética em Pesquisa, CONEP) under the number CAAE: 06852212.4.0000.5404. Written informed consent was obtained from adult participants. For participants aged 18 or younger verbal informed assent was obtained from the participant and written informed consent was obtained from a parent or legal guardian on behalf of the participant, as approved by the CEP-FCM-UNICAMP.

Subjects

A convenience sample was recruited amongst healthy adults and children who visited Hospital de Clínicas (UNICAMP, Brazil) accompanying adult patients or members of staff. The inclusion criteria were (1) willingness to participate and (a) informed consent from the adult participants, or (b) informed consent from parents or legal guardians combined with assent from participant, (2) clinical judgement of being neurologically normal based on an interview with the subjects or with the parents (in the case of small children), (3) ability to recognize at least 70% (11/16) of items of the picture identification test, therefore clearly demonstrating ability to understand the identification task and furthermore showing familiarity with at least 70% of items in the test. For the final study none of the 51 children fulfilling criteria (2) failed on criteria (3).

Exclusion criteria were a) known or suspected neurological or psychiatric disease; b) active upper respiratory tract infection or acute rhinitis in the day of evaluation. In order to check for exclusion criteria a brief questionnaire modified from Fornazieri et al [33] was applied. The initial pilots were tested in 29 children (mean age±SD: 9.4±4.3 years) and 23 adults (40±15.5 years) for comparison, and the final test was applied to 51 children (9.9±4.2 years) and 20 adults (39.4±10.7 years).

Data Collection

The participant's assessment was performed in three sequential stages: (1) application of socioeconomic and health questionnaire (described in "demographic data"), (2) smell test and (3) cognitive test. The order was thus established because the questionnaire was needed to screen for inclusion and exclusion criteria, and then the cognitive test was performed last to avoid influencing recognition of the items in the smell test.

Demographic Data

For all subjects we collected age, gender, socioeconomic level, health conditions and schooling. The socioeconomic level was assessed as previously described [25,26] by a question about the family income (average family income in comparison with the Brazilian minimum wage). Minimum wage is defined as the lowest monthly remuneration legally stipulated that the employer must pay to the employee. This value is updated regularly by the Brazilian government to reflect changes in cost-of-living [34,35]. Based on the answer to this question, subjects were further classified in the groups A (monthly family income of less than 5 times the value of the standardized Brazilian minimum wage) or B (monthly family income higher than 5 times the value of the standardized Brazilian minimum wage).

Cognitive Evaluation

Due to the variable cognitive profile of children aged 3 through 18 years, we opted for a cognitive screening using a modification of the UPSIT Picture Identification Test, adapted to the Sniffin' Sticks smell test (PIT-SS16-Child) in its Brazilian version. The PIT is used as a means of cognitive screening in adult populations who undergo the UPSIT [32]. The PIT-SS16-Child is composed of sixteen pictures, which are identical to the pictures used to identify the correct answers in each of the 16 items of the smell test. For each figure in the test, four options are read by the examiner, and the subject must recognize the word which describes the figure.

The 23 adults who participated in the pilot studies were screened using the validated Portuguese version of Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) and all scored above the recommended cut-off of 24 points [36]. In addition, the 20 adult subjects who underwent pilot 4 also performed the PIT-SS16-Child.

Smell Testing

Smell identification was tested using the standardized application of a commercially available and extensively validated smell identification test called "16-item Smell Identification Test from Sniffin Sticks", hereafter called SS16 in this manuscript. This test is composed of 16 standardized pens produced in Germany (Burghart Messtechnik company) which must be presented sequentially to the subject, who then identifies the smell by a forced choice amongst 4 alternative (one correct label and 3 distractors). Each pen is held approximately 2 centimetres from the subject's nose for no more than 3 or 4 seconds. The interval between the presentations of each odour is of roughly 30 seconds [24, 37]. The original version of SS16 was validated to Brazilian adults by our group in 2008 [27]. This Brazilian version was then used as the base for a child-friendly adaptation produced as described in the following sections:

  1. Replacement of the words for pictures as alternatives for the forced choice. When necessary, the picture for the item was replaced by a picture of an equivalent item more familiar to children. Example: for the item “mint” a photo of mint flavoured candies was used;
  2. Replacement of labels of smells unknown to children for an equivalent or similar item. Example: the odour of anise seed (in the original labelled “anise”) was labelled “toothpaste” due to its perceived similarity with the essences often used for toothpaste in Brazil, as reported by children during informal focus groups preceding our pilot studies;
  3. Replacement of the unfamiliar distractors for other more familiar options;
  4. Replacement of distractors which were felt to be too similar to the smell (ie: to the correct choice) of the item by distractors which were more contrasting. E.g.: for the item 16, “fish”, the distractors used were “strawberry, orange, rose”, which are quite contrasting to “fish”. The selection of contrasting distractors was done based on a subjective impression of the researchers based on informal focus groups with children before and during the pilot studies, and on the percentage of subjects who choose each one of the distractors. If a larger proportion of subjects chose one specific wrong distractor, that was considered indirect evidence of subjective similarity of that distractor and the item.
  5. After each pilot study involving a smaller group of children, new changes were made for items in which difficulties were demonstrated objectively (less than 75% identifiability) or reported subjectively (children referred that the item was confusing, etc).

Results

The characteristics of the children enrolled in the four pilots of the study are shown in the first three columns of Table 1. The progressive changes made on the adaptation process can be seen in detail S1 Table) which shows how the labels for each of the items and distractors changed in the adaptation process.

The final version of the test will be henceforth called SS16-Child. The SS16-Child was applied to 51 children aged 3 to 18 years in the final study. The final items and distractors used in the test are described in Table 2, in English. The images as well as the adapted Portuguese translation actually used for the SS16-Child in Brazil are provided as images in S1 Appendix and table in S2 Table). The raw data on all 51 children is provided as data in S3, S4 and S5 Tables), as well as for the 20 adults who performed the tests and were not included in the statistical analyses.

thumbnail
Table 2. Comparing the adult adaptation of the SS16 with the SS16-Child.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.t002

All children in the study scored 12 or more in the PIT-SS16-Child, and 37 (72.5%) scored 16. All items in this final version were correctly identified by 60% or more of the children tested (including pre-school children). Among children aged 10 or older, all items were recognized by at least 83.3% of children, and of the 20 adults included in the comparison group 18 scored 16 on the SS16-Child (and 2 scored 15). Some items were poorly recognized by children younger than 6 years old, despite the modifications. In this age group only 5 items (orange, banana, lemon, apple and pineapple) were recognized by more than 75% of subjects. Details of identification of items by age group are provided in the supplementary material (data in S6 Table).

A strong correlation was found between age and SS16-Child score (r² = 0.482, p<0.001), age and PIT-SS16-Child score (r² = 0.436, p<0.001) and SS16-Child score and PIT-SS16-Child score (r² = 0.741, p<0.001). All these associations are represented in Fig 1. In addition, calculation of the partial correlation coefficients showed an independent effect of age on the SS16-Child when adjusted for PIT-SS16-Child (r2 = 0.109; p = 0.019) and of the PIT-SS16-Child on the SS16-Child when adjusted for age (r2 = 0.555, p<0.001).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Correlations between SS16-Child, PIT-SS16-Child and age.

SS16-Child: New version of Sniffin' Sticks smell test, adapted to Brazilian children; PIT-SS16-Child: Picture Identification Test, adapted for use with SS16-Child; *** highly significant association (p<0.001). A) r² = 0.482 indicating that the variation in age explains roughly 42% of the variation in the score of the test; B) r² = 0.436 indicating that 43% of the PIT-SS16-Child variation can be explained by the variation in age; C) This relationship explains roughly 74% of the variation in the SS16-Child data (r2 = 0.74) and is highly significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.g001

For the same subjects a multiple linear regression was performed to determine the association between the SS16-Child and the independent variables of gender, household income and self-reported rhinitis, when adjusted for age and performance in the PIT. There were no evidence that these former variables influenced performance in the SS16-Child (p for gender = 0.647, p for income = 0.763, and p for self-reported rhinitis = 0.232). The multiple linear regression confirmed the independent effects of age (p = 0.024) and the PIT-SS16-Child score (p<0.001) on the olfactory performance (SS16-Child as dependent variable), with a larger effect size of the later (B for age = 0.17; 95% confidence interval = 0.024–0.317, and B for PIT-SS16-Child = 1.57; 95% confidence interval = 0.92–2.22).

Discussion

This study shows that children as young as 5 could recognize most items from the SS16 when labels were translated and when necessary replaced by more culturally suitable items. It further demonstrates that in addition to age, which is known as a significant determinant of olfactory performance, cognition is an independent predictor of the results of the test and should be taken into consideration when interpreting test scores. Sniffin' Sticks is a commercially available smell test that has undergone extensive validation and that has been used internationally for more than a decade to diagnose hyposmia in adults. Therefore, any changes we made did not concern the smell themselves, which are produced by Burghart Messtechnik company (www.burghart-mt.de), but only the labels used in our adaptation, a procedure that can be reproduced in other countries in which a smell test is not available. The present study is the first to include the smell identification test in children outside of Europe, North-America or Australia and the results demonstrate that the cultural validation is an important step to adapt smell identification tests to children. Furthermore, the study shows that results in an adaptation (PIT-SS16-Child) of the PIT, a cognitive screening which was created to accompany the UPSIT test, is highly correlated with the performance in the SS16 when adapted to children, and therefore can be used to estimate the effect of cognition on the results of the olfactory task.

Although cognition is known to significantly impact smell identification performance in adults [22,30,31], no systematic study of the relationship between age, cognition and performance on smell identification has been included in most studies as can be seen from Table 3. Only Richman et al [2], Monnery-Patris et al [8], Dalton et al [9,10,13] and we have performed standardized cognitive screening. In addition, only our study and Richman et al [2] presented the statistical association between the olfactory and cognitive tests. Our study shows that a large part of the effect of age on olfactory performance is likely to be due to the effect of age on the cognitive abilities involved in the smell identification task (i.e., the same abilities which are involved in the PIT-SS16-Child) which include familiarity with the item, and also other abilities that impact identification and naming of figures, independently of familiarity with its smell and the capacity to form and identify olfactory imagery. The independent effect of the performance on the PIT-SS16-Child on the SS16 had a larger effect size than age itself, although both the multiple linear regression and partial correlation coefficient analyses showed a significant independent effect of age on olfaction, probably highlighting the role of olfactory learning that happens as children experiment various odours as they grow up [14]. This is visible in the data presented in Fig 1, which show that although most children older than 6 identified all pictures on the PIT-SS16-Child, many of these children failed to identify one or more odours in the SS16-Child. Although there is a possibility that these mistakes could be due to the odours presented not corresponding to the figures, this is unlikely given that 18 or the 20 adults included in the study correctly identified all 16 of the SS16Child items, indicating these items were highly identifiable. The item “leather”, for example, was correctly identified by 50/51 children in the PIT-SS16-Child, but it was only identified by 40/51 children in the SS16-Child, suggesting that although children knew what leather was, they probably were not familiar with its smell, or maybe were not able to form its olfactory image based on the odour provided, unlike adults which identified this item with more ease (19/20 identified correctly).

thumbnail
Table 3. Previous studies about smell identification test in children.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.t003

Unlike Dalton et al [13] who performed the picture identification test before the olfactory task, as a means to exclude subjects unable to perform, and also clarify the meaning of images to smaller children before they performed the test, we only offered the PIT-SS16-Child after children completed the SS16-Child. Therefore children’s previous cognitive experiences with the items and their pre-test capacity to associate the images with the items were more influential in our test. Dalton did not report the statistical association between performance in the picture and olfactory identification tasks.

Our findings largely reproduce other common findings in the studies included in Table 3 [210,12,13,1517], showing that for any given olfactory test, the effect of age on olfaction will eventually reach a ceiling and children will no longer perform differently from young adults. It is predictable that tests that used more highly identifiable items like ours tended to level down the results in different ages to the same common denominator, generating an earlier ceiling effect (ie: older teenagers identified almost all items, please see the data in S6 Table). We avoided direct comparisons with adults because the limited sample size using a low sensitivity test can generate non-significant results for certain age groups, without actually reflecting lack of subtle differences between the groups. A future study, comparing the performance of teenagers and adults in an adult test (therefore a more sensitive instrument) would be more suited to fulfil this specific knowledge gap. To enable other researchers to compare the data from the children and adults in our study, we have included the data from the adults in the S7, S8 and S9 Tables). The development of cognition and olfaction in children mirrors the olfactory decline and cognitive loss observed in the elderly [38], and most studies that included wide age groups show that olfactory performance peaks in young adults. Although some variation can be seen in the SS16-Child result up to 18 years, the largest variation is found amongst children aged 10 or less. From our data, and the other data existing in the literature, it looks like olfactory learning is much more significant in pre-school children. Children younger than 3 are unlikely to perform well on the smell identification test of the SS16-Child, and children older than 10 are likely to score correctly on all or almost all 16 items if they have normal olfaction. This probably makes the SS16-Child in its current form a test with a high specificity for hyposmia in teenagers (ie: if an adolescent looses a couple of points, we can conclude there is a high chance of a true smell deficit), but at the cost of sensitivity (ie: because the smell identification test is very “easy” it is possible that subjects with only mild smell deficit could still perform very well in the test). But this larger “reserve” might be useful to test olfaction in conditions which cause cognitive deficit, because children might still be able to perform the test even with some cognitive loss, making this test more likely to be suitable in these conditions. This needs to be studied further in a study involving children with confirmed smell disorders, and also children with neurological and cognitive conditions.

In ours and in most studies, pre-school children seem to form a separate group when it comes to olfactory identification. Children younger than 6 presented considerable underperformance compared to older children in ours and other studies [2,6,810,1214,16], suggesting that separate subscales, or separate norms for the same scales should be applied to them. Most authors found only up to 5 or 6 of the items tested in any test were suitable for this age group. In our study only fruit items (orange, banana, lemon, apple and pineapple) were highly identified (more than 75%) by children younger than 6. It might be that further development of tests specifically targeting this age group might provide a longer test with high identifiability. Even young children were able to perform the PIT-SS16-Child after the SS16-Child with good engagement scoring at least 12 points, showing they are amenable to longer tests and that the length of the SS16-Child was not a limitation to their performance.

With cultural adaptations of the distractors and using figures to represent distractors and items, the SS16 could be used cross-culturally to test Brazilian children. The performance of children in our study was similar to the recent study performed in Germany [15] where the SS16 was developed, with the difference that while in Germany children miss-identified the item “apple” and "turpentine" which was excluded from the so-called “Sniffin-Kids”, in Brazil children did not recognize “cinnamon”, raising the point that in further cultural adaptations for other countries, the 16 item test might be more useful than adapting the Sniffin-Kids directly.

Even though the current study included a limited number of subjects, it suggests that the SS16-Child could be used cross-culturally to test olfaction in children older than 5, and that a subscale could be used in younger children. The current version of the SS16-Child when applied to Brazilian children might render the test less sensitive to smell deficit in older children. The results suggests that the PIT-SS16-Child could be used as a screening test for cognitive abilities necessary to perform the test, and as an adjustment method for the effects of cognitive abilities on olfaction when testing smaller children or patients with potential cognitive deficits. In fact, given that the effect size of the PIT-SS16-Child was larger than the effect size of age, this adjustment might be paramount for the understanding of the results of the test. Larger studies including cognitive and olfactory testing in children from all age groups are warranted.

The creation of truly “cross-cultural” smell identification tests suitable to most major cultures worldwide would enable multicenter clinical studies of olfaction and has been target of clinical research [39]. However the current study only involved Brazilian children, precluding direct conclusions regarding suitability of the SS16 for use in children of other backgrounds. It is possible that a similar process of adaptation with replacement of labels and the application of adapted picture identification tests could be performed successfully in other cultures, but further studies are warranted. The SS16 is a clinical test of olfaction used to detect clinically meaningful hyposmia and consequently guide the clinical management of the patients. The values used as cut-off for hyposmia are usually arbitrarily defined by the 10th percentile [3,7,15,16,29] of a large sample of subjects of a given age group. Therefore, in order to provide cut-off values for this test, studies in larger cohorts are warranted. The main contributions of this work are (1) to demonstrate that the original version of the Sniffin Sticks can be applied to Brazilian children, (2) to indicate that future studies in larger cohorts aiming to determine cut-off values for the SS16 in children (either in Brazil or other countries) should include cognitive testing to aid interpretation of the results correctly and that (3) studies using other smell testing kits may benefit from the inclusion of some form of cognitive testing to aid interpretation of the test.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Changes in each pilot.

* = Correct alternatives for items are marked by a star.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s002

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Portuguese version of SS16-Child.

*Correct items.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s003

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Demographic data of 51 children participating in the final study.

Legend: f—female; m = male; A—family income less than 5 Brazilian minimum wage; B—family income over than 5 Brazilian minimum wage.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s004

(DOCX)

S4 Table. SS16-Child scores of 51 children participating in the final study.

Legend: 1—correct answer; 0—wrong answer.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s005

(DOCX)

S5 Table. PIT-SS16-Child scores of 51 children participating in the final study.

Legend: 1—correct answer; 0—wrong answer.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s006

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Percentage of correct identification by age group.

The columns show the percentage of subjects in each age group who correctly identified each item. Cells are coloured according to the following rules: ≥75% green, 60–74.9% yellow, <60% red. N = number of subjects; y = years.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s007

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Demographic data of adults.

Legend: f—female; m = male; A—family income less than 5 Brazilian minimum wage; B—family income over than 5 Brazilian minimum wage.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s008

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Adults results in SS16-Child.

Legend: 1—correct answer; 0—wrong answer.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s009

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Adults results in PIT-SS16-Child.

Legend: 1—correct answer; 0—wrong answer.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131641.s010

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Enrico Ghizoni, Fabiano Reis and Gustavo Pereira Fraga for their suggestions throughout the writing process of this work.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: LODB LSM. Performed the experiments: LODB LSM. Analyzed the data: LODB MMG AJL TW LSM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LODB MMG AJL TW LSM. Wrote the paper: LODB LSM. Revising the work critically for important intellectual content: MMG AJL TW LSM.

References

  1. 1. Doty RL. Olfactory dysfunction in Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Neurol. 2012;8: 329–339. pmid:22584158
  2. 2. Richman RA, Post EM, Sheehe PR, Wright HN. Olfactory performance during childhood. I. Development of an odorant identification test for children. J Pediatr. 1992;121: 908–911. pmid:1447653
  3. 3. Kobal G, Klimek L, Wolfensberger M, Gudziol H, Temmel A, Owen CM, et al. Multicenter investigation of 1,036 subjects using a standardized method for the assessment of olfactory function combining tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2000;257: 205–211. pmid:10867835
  4. 4. Frank RA, Dulay MF, Gesteland RC. Assessment of the Sniff Magnitude Test as a clinical test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav. 2003;78: 195–204. pmid:12576116
  5. 5. Frank R, Dulay M, Niergarth K, Gesteland R. A comparison of the sniff magnitude test and the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test in children and nonnative English speakers. Physiol Behav. 2004;81: 475–480. pmid:15135019
  6. 6. Hummel T, Bensafi M, Nikolaus J, Knecht M, Laing D, Schaal B. Olfactory function in children assessed with psychophysical and electrophysiological techniques. Behav Brain Res. 2007;180: 133–138. pmid:17418432
  7. 7. Laing D, Segovia C, Fark T, Laing O, Jinks A, Nikolaus J, et al. Tests for screening olfactory and gustatory function in school-age children. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2008;139: 74–82. pmid:18585565
  8. 8. Monnery-Patris S, Rouby C, Nicklaus S, Issanchou S. Development of olfactory ability in children: Sensitivity and identification. Dev Psychobiol. 2009;51: 268–276. pmid:19130505
  9. 9. Dalton P, Mennella JA, Cowart BJ, Maute C, Pribitkin EA, Reilly JS. Evaluating the Prevalence of Olfactory Dysfunction in a Pediatric Population. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1170: 537–542. pmid:19686190
  10. 10. Dalton P, Mennella JA, Maute C, Castor SM, Silva-Garcia A, Slotkin J, et al. Development of a test to evaluate olfactory function in a pediatric population. Laryngoscope. 2011; 121: 1843–1850. pmid:22024835
  11. 11. Dudova I, Vodicka J, Havlovicova M, Sedlacek Z, Urbanek T, Hrdlicka M. Odor detection threshold, but not odor identification, is impaired in children with autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;20: 333–340. pmid:21528391
  12. 12. Cameron EL, Doty RL. Odor identification testing in children and young adults using the smell wheel. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2013;77: 346–350. pmid:23246420
  13. 13. Dalton P, Doty RL, Murphy C, Frank R, Hoffman HJ, Maute C, et al. Olfactory assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology. 2013;80: S32–S36. pmid:23479541
  14. 14. Dżaman K, Zielnik-Jurkiewicz B, Jurkiewicz D, Molińska-Glura M. Test for screening olfactory function in children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2013;77: 418–423. pmid:23312529
  15. 15. Schriever VA, Mori E, Petters W, Boerner C, Smitka M, Hummel T. The “Sniffin’ Kids” Test—A 14-Item Odor Identification Test for Children. Louis M, editor. PLoS ONE. 2014;9: e101086. pmid:24979650
  16. 16. Sorokowska A, Schriever VA, Gudziol V, Hummel C, Hähner A, Iannilli E, et al. Changes of olfactory abilities in relation to age: odor identification in more than 1400 people aged 4 to 80 years. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2014; In press (Epub ahead of print).
  17. 17. Richman RA, Sheehe PR, Wallace K, Hyde JM, Coplan J. Olfactory performance during childhood. II. Developing a discrimination task for children. J Pediatr. 1995;127: 421–426. pmid:7658273
  18. 18. Stevenson RJ, Mahmut M, Sundqvist N. Age-related changes in odor discrimination. Dev Psychol. 2007;43: 253–260. pmid:17201523
  19. 19. Doty RL, McKeown DA, Lee WW, Shaman P. A study of the test-retest reliability of ten olfactory tests. Chem Senses. 1995;20: 645–656. pmid:8788098
  20. 20. Doty RL. Olfaction in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2007;13 Suppl 3: S225–228. pmid:18267240
  21. 21. Lotsch J, Reichmann H, Hummel T. Different Odor Tests Contribute Differently to the Evaluation of Olfactory Loss. Chem Senses. 2008;33: 17–21. pmid:17761724
  22. 22. Hedner M, Larsson M, Arnold N, Zucco GM, Hummel T. Cognitive factors in odor detection, odor discrimination, and odor identification tasks. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2010;32: 1062–1067. pmid:20437286
  23. 23. Doty RL, Shaman P, Dann M. Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav. 1984;32: 489–502. pmid:6463130
  24. 24. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G. “Sniffin” sticks’: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses. 1997;22: 39–52. pmid:9056084
  25. 25. Silveira-Moriyama L, Azevedo A, Ranvaud R, Barbosa ER, Doty RL, Lees AJ. Applying a new version of the Brazilian-Portuguese UPSIT smell test in Brazil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2010;68: 700–705. pmid:21049178
  26. 26. Fornazieri M, Doty R, Santos C, Pinna F, Bezerra T, Voegels R. A new cultural adaptation of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test. Clinics. 2013;68: 65–68. pmid:23420159
  27. 27. Silveira-Moriyama L, Carvalho M de J, Katzenschlager R, Petrie A, Ranvaud R, Barbosa ER, et al. The use of smell identification tests in the diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease in Brazil. Mov Disord. 2008;23: 2328–2334. pmid:18785265
  28. 28. Doty RL. The smell identification test administration manual. 3rd ed. Haddon Heights: Sensonics; 1995.
  29. 29. Hummel T, Kobal G, Gudziol H, Mackay-Sim A. Normative data for the “Sniffin’ Sticks” including tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds: an upgrade based on a group of more than 3,000 subjects. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2007;264: 237–243. pmid:17021776
  30. 30. Dulay MF, Gesteland RC, Shear PK, Ritchey PN, Frank RA. Assessment of the influence of cognition and cognitive processing speed on three tests of olfaction. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2008;30: 327–337. pmid:17852612
  31. 31. Larsson M, Hedner M, Olofsson J. Differential age and sex effects in semantic recognition of odors and words. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 2009;41(11): 1049–1053.
  32. 32. Vollmecke TA, Doty RL. Development of the Picture Identification Test (PIT): a research companion to the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT). Chem Senses. 1985;10: 413–414.
  33. 33. Fornazieri MA. Validação do teste de identificação do olfato da Universidade da Pensilvânia (UPSIT) para brasileiros.Doctoral dissertation, University of São Paulo. 2013. Available: http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/5/5143/tde-10032014-090524/.
  34. 34. Daniel-Kagbare TE. A dictionary of economics and commerce. Bloomington: Authorhouse. 2014.
  35. 35. Maloney WF, Mendez JN. Measuring the impact of minimum wages: evidence from Latin America. In: Heckman JJ, Pageés C, editors. Law and employment: lessons from Latin America and the Caribbean. Chicago: University of Chicago Press 2004. pp 109–130.
  36. 36. Brucki SMD, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PHF, Okamoto IH. Suggestions for utilization of the mini-mental state examination in Brazil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003;61: 777–781. pmid:14595482
  37. 37. Sniffin' sticks manual. Wedel: Burghart-Medizintechnik.
  38. 38. Doty RL, Kamath V. The influences of age on olfaction: a review. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1–20.
  39. 39. Doty RL, Marcus A, Lee WW. Development of the 12-item Cross-Cultural Smell Identification Test (CC-SIT). The Laryngoscope. 1996;106: 353–356. pmid:8614203