Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Native and Non-Native Plants Provide Similar Refuge to Invertebrate Prey, but Less than Artificial Plants

  • Bart M. C. Grutters ,

    b.grutters@nioo.knaw.nl

    Affiliation Department of Aquatic Ecology, Netherlands Institute of Ecology (NIOO-KNAW), Wageningen, the Netherlands

  • Bart J. A. Pollux,

    Affiliation Experimental Zoology Group, Department of Animal Sciences, Wageningen University, Wageningen, the Netherlands

  • Wilco C. E. P. Verberk,

    Affiliation Department of Animal Ecology and Ecophysiology, Institute for Water and Wetland Research, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, the Netherlands

  • Elisabeth S. Bakker

    Affiliation Department of Aquatic Ecology, Netherlands Institute of Ecology (NIOO-KNAW), Wageningen, the Netherlands

Abstract

Non-native species introductions are widespread and can affect ecosystem functioning by altering the structure of food webs. Invading plants often modify habitat structure, which may affect the suitability of vegetation as refuge and could thus impact predator-prey dynamics. Yet little is known about how the replacement of native by non-native vegetation affects predator-prey dynamics. We hypothesize that plant refuge provisioning depends on (1) the plant’s native status, (2) plant structural complexity and morphology, (3) predator identity, and (4) prey identity, as well as that (5) structurally similar living and artificial plants provide similar refuge. We used aquatic communities as a model system and compared the refuge provided by plants to macroinvertebrates (Daphnia pulex, Gammarus pulex and damselfly larvae) in three short-term laboratory predation experiments. Plant refuge provisioning differed between plant species, but was generally similar for native (Myriophyllum spicatum, Ceratophyllum demersum, Potamogeton perfoliatus) and non-native plants (Vallisneria spiralis, Myriophyllum heterophyllum, Cabomba caroliniana). However, plant refuge provisioning to macroinvertebrate prey depended primarily on predator (mirror carp: Cyprinus carpio carpio and dragonfly larvae: Anax imperator) and prey identity, while the effects of plant structural complexity were only minor. Contrary to living plants, artificial plant analogues did improve prey survival, particularly with increasing structural complexity and shoot density. As such, plant rigidity, which was high for artificial plants and one of the living plant species evaluated in this study (Ceratophyllum demersum), may interact with structural complexity to play a key role in refuge provisioning to specific prey (Gammarus pulex). Our results demonstrate that replacement of native by structurally similar non-native vegetation is unlikely to greatly affect predator-prey dynamics. We propose that modification of predator-prey interactions through plant invasions only occurs when invading plants radically differ in growth form, density and rigidity compared to native plants.

Introduction

Non-native species are becoming widespread due to globalization and can have a profound effect on ecosystem functioning by altering the structure of food webs [1, 2]. Commonly underlying these food web effects are changes in nutrient cycling and habitat structure [3, 4]. As predator-prey dynamics are typically mediated by the structural complexity provided by plants [59], ongoing plant invasions may distort predator-prey dynamics and ultimately ecosystem functioning through food web effects. Non-native plants may differentially shape the refuge provided to prey [1012] or change prey behaviour, i.e. through differences in the structural complexity or density of native and non-native plants. Indeed, behavioural changes in prey have been induced by non-native plants in both terrestrial [13, 14] and aquatic habitats [15].

Plants can change predator and prey behaviour [16, 17] and alter predator-prey dynamics by providing physical predator-free refuge [5, 7] or by reducing encounter rates and prey visibility [8]. The extent to which such effects are manifested is mainly dictated by the habitat structure provided by plants, which is determined by the shoot density and architectural complexity. For example, densities > 350 artificial stems m-2 have been shown to impair prey (Daphnia pulex) detection and the swimming speed and predation rate of the planktivorous fish Pseudorasbora parva [18]. Similarly, increasing artificial plant density reduced predation rates of largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides) feeding on bluegills (Lepomis macrochirus) [19] and real submerged plants reduced largemouth bass feeding on rainwater killifish (Lucania parva) [12]. Furthermore, a high plant structural complexity benefitted prey survival (Anopheles sp. larvae) under fish (Nannoperca australis) predation [6]. In general, finely dissected leaves, dense whorls and spiny leaf axils contribute to plant complexity and can reduce prey visibility and provide physical refuge [5, 20].

The provisioning and effectiveness of refuge also depends on predator and prey identity. For example, chironomid larvae were safer in complex plants under bream (Abramis brama) and roach (Rutilus rutilus) predation than under perch (Perca fluviatilis) predation [21]. Predator hunting mode (e.g. pursuing or ambushing prey [22]) can affect the role of habitat complexity in providing refuge to prey organisms [16]. For instance, some prey evade structured habitats if the risk of ambush predators is high, but they might enter structured habitats upon seeing predators in the open water [23, 24]. Additionally, the role of plants in predator-prey dynamics is further determined by prey characteristics such as size, activity, swimming speed, camouflage or susceptibility to allelochemicals [25, 26]. For example, predator size determines whether plant interstitial space hinders predation, while prey size determines whether prey can fit into the available interstitial space to survive predation attempts [2729]. Another example is prey susceptibility to plant allelochemicals, as prey face the dilemma of having to endure these chemicals in the relative safety near the plant, or escape these chemicals by venturing out in the open but risk being preyed upon [30, 31].

Altogether, plant refuge provisioning is determined by multiple parameters and can be highly plant-, predator- and/or prey-specific [9]. Therefore, alterations in habitat structure through the replacement of native by non-native plant species can greatly alter food webs and ecosystem functioning [4, 3234]. Yet, our understanding of the plant-mediated effects on predation dynamics by submerged native and non-native aquatic plants is still insufficient to predict invasion impacts [35].

In this study, we compare the refuge provided by submerged aquatic native and non-native plants to three macroinvertebrate prey species predated upon by actively hunting fish (mirror carp: Cyprinus carpio L. 1758) or ambushing dragonfly larvae (Anax imperator) in laboratory trials. We hypothesize that (1) non-native submerged macrophytes provide less refuge than native aquatic plants to macroinvertebrate prey as the unfamiliarity of native prey species with non-native plants may limit their optimal utilization of these novel sources of refuge, leaving them potentially more vulnerable to predation. In addition, we expect that (2) plant structural complexity and shoot density increase the effectiveness of refuge provisioning, and that plant refuge provisioning depends on (3) predator and (4) prey identity [9]. In addition to experiments with living macrophytes, we performed predation trials with artificial plants to exclude allelopathy and purely assess the role of plant complexity. We hypothesize (5) that artificial and living structures similar in density and complexity provide similar refuge.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

The authors declare that mirror carp feeding trials comply with the animal research laws of the Netherlands and permission for these was provided by the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences animal sciences committee under application NIOO 13.09.

The macroinvertebrates in this study were collected from non-protected privately owned streams or artificial ponds with permission from the landowner and the regional water board. All odonate species used are listed on the IUCN Red List as ‘of least concern’. We took care that our sampling of individuals for use in laboratory predation trials would not threaten local populations of macroinvertebrate species.

Experimental design

We assessed the degree of refuge provided to macroinvertebrate prey species by native and non-native submerged plants by means of predation trials. Each predation trial consisted of a predator foraging on multiple individuals of a single prey species in an aquarium with one plant species being present, or without any plants (see description below). The six selected native and non-native plants are common in European waters [36] and vary in structural complexity, as expressed by their fractal dimensions [37] (Table 1). The plants were tested using two contrasting shoot densities (‘low’ vs ‘high’), except in experiments with water flea (Daphnia pulex). Water fleas clustered in the corners of the experimental area, i.e. away from the plants, already at low shoot density, so we did not further assess them at high shoot density. Three different experiments were performed to test plant refuge provision.

thumbnail
Table 1. Information on the real and artificial aquatic plants used.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.t001

In the first experiment we tested the refuge provided by three native (Potamogeton perfoliatus, Myriophyllum spicatum and Ceratophyllum demersum) and three non-native plant species (Vallisneria spiralis, Myriophyllum heterophyllum, Cabomba caroliniana) to three widespread macroinvertebrate species varying in size and activity [38, 39] predated on by actively hunting juvenile mirror carp (C. carpio). As prey, we used a motile benthic amphipod (Gammarus pulex L. 1758), a small pelagic zooplankter (Daphnia pulex Leydig 1860) and sedentary phytophilic damselfly larvae (approximately 70% Ischnura elegans Vander Linden 1820 and 30% Coenagrion puella L. 1758 or C. pulchellum Vander Linden 1825).

To compare the refuge provision to prey predated by predators differing in their hunting mode [40], we performed a second experiment where we measured the refuge provided by the same three native and three non-native plant species to G. pulex under predation by ambushing dragonfly larvae (Anax imperator Leach 1815). Dragonfly larvae are one of the primary invertebrate top predators in waters without fish, and frequently involved in predation studies [5, 25]. G. pulex are a natural food source for dragonfly larvae [41] and served as prey. Damselfly larvae were unavailable after the carp predation trials, while D. pulex is not a major food source for A. imperator [42], therefore these prey were not tested.

Artificial plant analogues are frequently used to unravel the mechanisms involved in plant refuge provision [6, 18]. In the third experiment we tested whether refuge provision by four artificial plants of varying complexity (Table 1) to G. pulex under carp predation is equal to that provided by living plants of similar structural complexity.

Aquatic plants

Plants were collected from monocultures maintained in tanks located at the Netherlands Institute of Ecology (51.9879 N, 5.6724 E). We selected three common native and three non-native Northwestern European submerged plant species with similar structural complexity (visualised in Fig 1). Common, dominant species were picked as these are expected to provide most of the ecosystem functions [43]. Effectively, dominant non-natives are expected to replace dominant natives, and its effects on refuge provision are being tested. In addition to these living plant species, four artificial plant analogues resembling Ceratophyllum, Myriophyllum, Vallisneria and Elodea were purchased for use in the third experiment (Hardeman Aquarium, Ede, Netherlands; visualised in Fig 2). After harvesting, the shoots were rinsed, cut to 25 cm and had their base wrapped in foam before being attached to a metal grid in low (~ 300 shoots m-2) or high (~ 800 shoots m-2) density using binding wire (S1 Fig). These densities were based on actual shoot densities in natural plant beds [19, 44] and prior experimental work [18]. The leaves of adjacent shoots touched each other at high density. The qualitative rigidity of all artificial plant analogues, C. demersum, and M. spicatum are described using a photograph (S2 Fig). Five fresh shoots per species were scanned (Epson Perfection 4990 Photo) and analysed for their area fractal dimension at whole-shoot and cross-sectional scale using ImageJ following [37]. The fractal dimension was calculated at these two scales as each provides different information [37]. Specifically, the shoot scale encompasses whole-plant complexity of leaf width and internode length, whereas the cross-sectional fractal dimension assesses leaf-scale complexity such as the degree of leaf dissection. After completing all the predation trials, plant wet and dry mass (60°C to constant dry weight) were determined. Shoots that had turned brown were replaced with fresh specimens during the experiment.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Refuge provisioning by native and non-native aquatic plants.

Mean ± SEM survival (%) of (A) Daphnia pulex, (B) damselfly larvae and (C) Gammarus pulex under mirror carp predation (Cyprinus carpio; n = 8) and of (D) Gammarus pulex under Anax imperator predation (n = 9) in low (white bars; 300 shoots m-2) and high density (grey bars; 800 shoots m-2) plant monocultures grouped into native (left side) and non-native species (right side). Horizontal bars represent the groups that were compared. Comparisons between two groups are shown as non-significant (ns) or one to three asterisks (GLMM Wald χ2 tests: * P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001), whereas lowercase letters indicate significance among three or more groups (GLMM simultaneous inference post hoc; P < 0.05). ‘NA’ indicates not available.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Refuge provisioning by artificial aquatic plants.

Mean ± SEM survival (%; n = 8) of the benthic crustacean (Gammarus pulex) predated upon by mirror carp (Cyprinus carpio carpio) in the presence of artificial plant analogues of varying complexity and in low (white bars; 300 shoots m-2) and high density (grey bars; 800 shoots m-2). Horizontal bars indicate the groups that were compared, where comparisons between two groups are shown as either non-significant (ns) or their significance using asterisks (GLMM Wald χ2 tests: * P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001) and lowercase letters for significance among three or more groups (GLMM simultaneous inference post hoc; P < 0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.g002

Macroinvertebrate prey

Damselfly larvae (14 ± 2.0 mm mean length ± SD; n = 280) were collected from two experimental ponds located at the Netherlands Institute of Ecology (51.9879 N, 5.6724 E), while G. pulex individuals (12.4 ± 2.4 mm mean length ± SD; n = 240) were collected from the Heelsumse beek (51.976 N, 5.754 E) and D. pulex individuals (1–3 mm) were commercially acquired every 4 days. All macroinvertebrate prey species were housed in aerated water of 21°C and used in predation trials after 24–72 h. G. pulex fed on dead plant material from the Heelsumse Beek while D. pulex fed on yeast and microalgae (Scenedesmus sp.). We photographed G. pulex and damselfly larvae to quantify their body size using ImageJ [45] and compare damselfly species survival. Visual inspection of pre- and post-trial photographs did not show differential survival for the two damselfly genera used. Damselfly larvae survivors were re-used in equal numbers across treatments (200 out of 640 individuals used) as not enough individuals could be collected from the field. The survival probability of fresh versus re-used damselfly larvae was similar (respectively 44 ± 5% versus 48 ± 5% survival; mean percent survival ± SD; calculated across all feeding trials involving damselfly larvae).

Carp trials

In the first and third experiment we used sixteen juvenile mirror carp (5 cm) acquired from the Aquatic Research Facilities of Wageningen University and Research Centre (ARF-WUR). These were paired based on their wet mass to reduce stress and variance in predation rate, yielding 8 homogenous pairs (mean ± SD weight of 20.6 ± 0.1 g), which we randomly allocated to one of 8 aquaria on the 30th of August 2013. We fed fish with sinking pellets (Trouvit, Trouw & Co., Putten, the Netherlands; 1% wet mass day-1) for a week and then started to supplement their diet with living Gammarus pulex and later zooplankton and damselfly larvae in the three weeks prior to the trials.

The carp trials were performed at a temperature of 21°C in eight glass aquaria (180 x 40 x 40 cm in length x width x height; water depth of 38 cm; 274 L). Pairs of aquaria were connected to four biological filters. Water was added weekly to compensate for evaporation, while 50% water replenishments took place in between trials. Each aquarium was divided into three compartments using separators consisting of a wooden frame covered with 0.5 mm PE mesh (S1 Fig). In this way we constructed a fish living area (60 cm of total length), an experimental area (40 cm) and a plant storage area (80 cm) in each aquarium. Fish could swim from their living area to the experimental area through a hatch, so there was no need of transferring them with a net. This is important because the manual transfer of fish by means of hand nets would cause severe stress, affecting fish prey capture behavior for prolonged periods of time. Therefore, during the two weeks prior to the trials, we accustomed the fish to swimming through the hatch into the experimental area. Both the living and experimental areas were covered in white paper on the outside to exclude effects of external stimuli during predation trials. Artificial plants, distinctly different from all other plants, were added to the living compartment as cage enrichment so that carp could hide if desired, whereas a 4 cm layer of coarse sand was added to the experimental area. Before each predation trial, plant grids of the appropriate species were buried in the sediment in such a way that a 10 cm perimeter of open water remained around the plants. Illumination was provided by dimmable fluorescent lamps (Philips MASTER TL5 HE 28W/840) and provided 14 ± 2.1 lux (mean ± SD) measured 5 cm above the sediment in the experimental area.

All carp trials were performed within five weeks, first using real plants then with artificial plant analogues. Two (living plants) to four trials (artificial plants) a day were performed over a consecutive period of at most 8 days for each predator-prey combination according to a randomized block scheme (n = 8). As training effects may have occurred over time, we included time as a random effect (see Data analysis). In the trials, prey were acclimated to the experimental area for 10 minutes before we let the fish enter the experimental area through the hatch. As G. pulex tended to ‘escape’ into the fish living area upon opening the hatch, they were added after the fish. The carp were allowed to forage on fifty D. pulex for 10 minutes and for 30 minutes on ten G. pulex or five damselfly larvae before re-entering their living area. These prey densities reflect the natural abundance of macroinvertebrate taxa [9]. Foraging times were based on pilot experiments to estimate the time required by predators to finish most, but not all, of the prey in vegetated patches. At the end of each trial all remaining prey were counted to obtain an estimate of survival probability. We measured the standard length, gape width and wet mass for each individual fish after completing all carp feeding trials (S2 Table).

Dragonfly larvae trials

For the second experiment, we collected 15 Anax imperator larvae (body length of 39.3 ± 2.1 mm) from experimental ponds located at the Netherlands Institute of Ecology (51.9879 N, 5.6724 E). In the week prior to the trials, these ambush predators were housed individually in 2 L plastic containers containing Elodea nuttalli and fed a single gammarid daily (approximately 10 mm). Before each trial, the A. imperator larvae were individually transferred to separate plastic containers (41 x 30 x 24 cm) filled with 25 litres of water and 4 cm of coarse sand as sediment, according to an incomplete randomized block design (n = 9) for two or three trials a day. After 30 minutes, ten G. pulex individuals (9.1 ± 2.1 mm mean ± SD; n = 320) were added to each container. Then, we allowed the A. imperator to forage for 60 minutes before returning them to their housing. At the end of each trial all remaining G. pulex were counted to obtain survival numbers. The foraging time was adjusted to match the lower feeding rate of dragonfly larvae compared to carp. Six plant species monocultures (in low and high shoot density plus a no-plant control) were used, similarly as in the carp trials (see Carp trials; Table 1).

Data analysis

The survival data were separately analysed for each predator-prey combination with generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) of the binomial family and fitted using Laplace approximation [46]. In all GLMMs we added random intercepts for the following random effects: ‘individuals’ (fish pairs or dragonfly individuals), day and time of day. Gammarus pulex survival under carp predation was corrected for escapes through the fish hatch (95 out of the 1840 individuals). Six outliers (> 2.2 interquartile range) in the Daphnia pulex x carp dataset were removed. These outliers were present no more than once per pair of fish (n = 8) and most of them (3 out of 6) occurred on day 1 of the D. pulex x carp trials. In these cases, daphnid survival was higher than average, likely because fish were less actively searching for prey or could not find daphnids in the corners.

For every predator-prey combination, we first fitted a GLMM to test for a difference between the plant treatments and the no-plant controls. If prey survival differed between plants and controls, the controls were excluded from the dataset to analyse the crossed fixed effects of plant species and density in a GLMM. Subsequently, to compare native and non-native plants, a separate GLMM with plant origin as the fixed effect and an additional random intercept for plant species was fitted on the dataset without controls. Hypotheses were tested by analysing fixed effects with Likelihood Ratio Tests (LRT). Post hoc comparisons of significant fixed effects were performed by simultaneous inference using Tukey contrasts. There was no overdispersion in the GLMMs as the sum of squared residuals divided by the residual degrees of freedom was approximately equal to 1, except for the model fitted on Daphnia pulex survival. Therefore, we resorted to the glmmPQL function in R to compare plants and controls using the aforementioned random effects structure.

Fractal dimensions of shoot and cross-section were compared among real and artificial plants, as well as between native and non-native species using 1-way ANOVAs. Residuals were normally distributed, yet some data violated the assumption of homogeneity of variances for which we used Welch’s ANOVA and a Games Howell post hoc test. Additionally, Pearson correlation and linear regression were used to correlate the shoot (D between 1.63–1.88) to the cross-sectional fractal dimension (D between 1.15–1.89) and to analyse the relation between the mean prey survival (%) and fractal dimensions respectively.

Statistics were performed using R version 3.0.3 [47] and the packages ‘lme4’ [48], ‘multcomp’ [49], ‘MASS’ [50] and ‘car’ [51].

Results

Plant refuge for prey under carp predation

The survival of the three macroinvertebrate prey under carp predation did not differ significantly between native and non-native plants (Fig 1; Table 2). The presence of certain plant species increased the survival of Gammarus pulex2df = 1 = 14.1; p < 0.001) and damselfly larvae (χ2df = 1 = 83.6; p < 0.001), but not of Daphnia pulex (tdf = 1 = -1.63; p = 0.11), compared to no-plant controls (Table 2; Fig 1). Observations indicated that D. pulex individuals were clustered in the corners of the experimental area, i.e. away from the plants. The avoidance of plants by D. pulex was confirmed in separate tests using artificial plant analogues. Surprisingly, only Ceratophyllum demersum improved gammarid survival (44% survival on average) compared to other plant species (maximum 4% survival on average) and this effect was found regardless of plant shoot density (Fig 1C; Table 2). Observations revealed that G. pulex individuals settled on plants, but were then often detected and hunted down by the carp. Survival of damselfly larvae was similar among plant species (χ2df = 5 = 5.57; p = 0.35), though damselfly larvae survival was reduced at high compared to low plant density (respectively 34% and 55% average survival; Fig 1B; χ2df = 1 = 5.75; p < 0.01).

thumbnail
Table 2. Output of generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) on prey survival data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.t002

Structural complexity

Even though prey survival was largely similar across plant species, there were clear differences in structural complexity among plant species (Table 1), whereas complexity was similar for native (mean ± SD of respectively shoot and cross-sectional D: 1.76 ± 0.07 and 1.66 ± 0.16) and non-native plant species (mean ± SD of shoot D: 1.75 ± 0.05 and cross-sectional D: 1.49 ± 0.19; One-way ANOVAs for shoot complexity: F1,4 = 0.045, p = 0.84 and cross-sectional complexity: F1,4 = 1.34, p = 0.31). Both the shoot and cross-sectional fractal dimension varied among plant species (One-way Welch’s ANOVAs of respectively F5, 11.04 = 6.54, p = 0.005 and F5, 38.4 = 117.28, p < 0.001; Table 1). A high shoot complexity was not necessarily coupled to a high cross-sectional complexity (Pearson correlation, r = -0.16, n = 10, p = 0.66). Linear regression indicated a significant positive relationship between the damselfly larvae survival under carp predation and shoot fractal dimension in the treatment with high shoot density (R2 = 0.81, n = 6; p = 0.009) while all other tested relationships between living plant structural complexity and prey survival were non-significant (S3 Fig).

Predation by ambushing dragonfly larvae

Under Anax imperator predation, G. pulex survival was higher in the presence of plants (mean ± SD survival of 53 ± 19%) compared to the no-plant control (mean ± SD survival of 34 ± 11%; χ2df = 1 = 13.6; p < 0.001; Fig 1D). Native and non-native plants provided equal refuge to G. pulex (respective mean ± SD survival of 59 ± 18% versus 48 ± 22%) and there was no effect of plant density (χ2df = 1 = 2.20; p = 0.14; Table 2). Yet there was a significant interaction of species and density (χ2df = 5 = 21.8; p < 0.001) as the survival of G. pulex in low density monocultures of Cabomba caroliniana had a decreased survival compared to the other plant species (Fig 1D).

Refuge provision by artificial plants

Artificial plant analogues differed in their shoot and cross-sectional fractal dimension (1-way Welch’s ANOVAs of respectively F3, 7.78 = 578.3, p < 0.001 and F3, 4.17 = 36.6, p = 0.002; Table 1). The survival of G. pulex under carp predation was affected by artificial plant type (χ2df = 3 = 115.6; p < 0.001). Specifically, average G. pulex survival was highest in Ceratophyllum (32%) and Myriophyllum artificial analogues (49%), somewhat lower in Vallisneria (17%) and lowest in Elodea analogues (4%; Fig 2). Yet, there was no relationship between G. pulex survival and the artificial plant fractal dimensions (S3 Fig). Lastly, at high shoot density G. pulex survival increased about three-fold compared to a low shoot density (average survival of respectively 39% versus 12%; χ2df = 1 = 49.23; p < 0.001). Observations indicated that at high shoot density, fish penetrated vegetation less often, or in the case of Ceratophyllum and Myriophyllum analogues, they rarely did so. Like in living Ceratophyllum demersum plants, surviving gammarids in artificial plant analogues were well hidden in interstitial spaces and it took considerable effort to remove them.

Discussion

We found that native and non-native aquatic plants generally provided equal refuge to macroinvertebrate prey. However, refuge provisioning depended strongly on predator and prey identity. Contrary to our expectations, plant structural complexity was not consistently a major driver in shaping refuge provisioning. In some cases, the level of protection depended on species-specific interactions between plant species, prey species, and plant density. Interestingly, our results on gammarid survival under carp predation were clearly different for artificial and living plants.

Native versus non-native plants

Invading plants typically lower native fish, plant, and macroinvertebrate species abundance [32, 35] and modify the habitat structure of ecosystems [4]. Consequentially, the change in habitat structure caused by replacement of native by non-native plants could change species assemblages by altering predator-prey dynamics [1012, 15, 39]. Contrary to our expectations, native and non-native plants functioned similarly in terms of refuge provision to macroinvertebrate prey in our experiments. Native and non-native plants in our study spanned a similar range in fractal dimensions, which may explain why they did not differ in their provision of refuge to invertebrate prey. There were specific exceptions with regard to Gammarus pulex prey however, as the native plant Ceratophyllum demersum provided refuge under carp predation, while the non-native species Cabomba caroliniana in low density made G. pulex more susceptible to dragonfly larvae predation.

Species-specificity is also reported by other authors. Some threatened macroinvertebrates and fish species depend on plant species for refuge, such as the C. demersum or Stratiotes aloides [5]. Their replacement by non-native plant structures could thus threaten conservation efforts. For example, ponds invaded by floating invasive plant species had reduced macroinvertebrate abundance and lacked sensitive benthic species like mayflies, compared to uninvaded ponds dominated by submerged native plants [32]. These species-specific effects show the utility of testing multiple plant species in experiments, both to uncover species-specific effects, and in order to generalize conclusions [52]. In future experiments, it would be of interest to assess the potential benefit of rare plant species in refuge provisioning on top of that already provided by dominant plants.

Prey identity

Prey identity can affect predator-prey dynamics [9, 53, 54]. We show that the use of plant structures for shelter by macroinvertebrate prey under carp predation differs and conclude that plant refuge is prey-specific. Surprisingly, the presence of plants did not affect Daphnia pulex survival under carp predation, even though such an effect has been reported in literature [30, 55]. In the presence of predatory fish, daphnids have been observed to seek refuge in plants [56]. Yet in our trials D. pulex individuals evaded plants, a behavioural observation that has also been reported previously in lab experiments [57, 58]. It seems that D. pulex failed to detect, or respond to, the fish as it may have done in prior studies [53, 56]. The absence of anti-predator behaviour can indicate that the daphnids used had not been adapted to fish predation [59].

Contrary to the pelagic D. pulex, phytophilic clasping damselfly larvae attach themselves to a leaf or stem and rely on crypsis to survive [60]. Upon release in the experimental arena for pre-trial acclimation, damselfly larvae quickly settled and rarely moved thereafter. All plant species, including those of low complexity, provided concealment for this prey. The first larval prey consumed by carp had often not settled on plants, but was attached to the glass. This indicates that edge effects are present in aquarium experiments, and that in situ, efficient crypsis relies on adequate prey behaviour in seeking suitable structures to attach to. Furthermore, edge effects can inflate experimental predation rates compared to natural predation rates, because in aquarium experiments, prey cannot escape predation by moving to other areas. At high plant density, larval survival was lower, possibly because damselfly larvae positioned themselves at the outer bounds of dense plant patches where they were more exposed to carp (pers. observation). Presumably damselfly larvae did so because they perceived the outer bounds as safer habitat than the inside of vegetation. The perception of predation risk by prey has previously been shown to strongly affect prey behaviour [24]. Specifically, juvenile roach (Rutilus rutilus) respond adaptively to olfactory and visual cues of open-water versus ambush predators and thereby increase its chance of survival. In our study, although there was neither ambush predator nor an olfactory cue, damselfly larvae chose not to enter dense vegetation. This behaviour occurred despite the visible presence of carp in the open water.

We observed a plant species effect for the highly active benthic amphipod Gammarus pulex as only Ceratophyllum demersum provided refuge. Important for the provision of predator-free space is the body size of predator and prey in relation to interstitial plant space [29, 61]. This plant’s dense and rigid leaf whorls offered refuge to the small and agile G. pulex whenever a carp tried to hunt it down, so that C. demersum effectively provided predator-free space.

We also hypothesized that prey survival would be positively related to plant complexity expressed as the fractal dimension [37]. Surprisingly, while the fractal dimension differed among plant species, it was only positively related to damselfly larvae survival in plant patches of high density. This could indicate an effect threshold for complexity similar to threshold effects reported for density [18, 62]. However to test this idea, further experiments are required. We suggest that predator and prey identity overruled the impact of plant structural complexity and shoot density in refuge provision, at least in the range of variation in complexity and stem density used in our experiments.

Artificial plant refuge

In contrast to our results with real plants, multiple studies using artificial plants showed that increased plant structural complexity generally improves prey survival under fish predation [18, 21], whereas there was not always an effect of stem density [6]. Such enhanced survival has been related to either concealment and reduced encounters [8, 18] or to provisioning of predator-free space [5, 17]. Similarly, we found that when using artificial plants, gammarid survival under carp predation increased with increasing qualitative structural complexity, not fractal dimension, and shoot density. Predator-free space (and consequently prey survival) is larger in artificial plants of high complexity. Plants with dense leaf whorls effectively limit fish movement, whereas plants with sparse leaf whorls like the Elodea analogue, or those with singular leaves like the Vallisneria analogue provided some predator-free habitat, but required high shoot densities to do so. However, gammarid survival was not related to the fractal dimension of the artificial plants, suggesting that the fractal dimension may not adequately capture the refuge provisioning of these plants. This may be related to the scale at which fractal dimension is estimated, i.e. at whole shoot or leaf, and it may be difficult to integrate both measures into a single index. This and other limitations of the fractal dimension, such as failing to capture diversity of complexity and size elements, have been previously discussed in a recent review [63].

Plant rigidity

Real and artificial plants were similar in size, shape, and structural complexity, suggesting that they could have offered the same degree of concealment to prey, yet they did not. Therefore, it is interesting to note that our plants varied in rigidity, which is common for aquatic plants [64] due to trade-offs in energy expenditure, light capture, and water velocity [65]. Interestingly, a recent study measured artificial plant stiffness and showed that increased shoot stiffness slightly decreased newt foraging rates on damselfly larvae [66]. Although we did not measure plant rigidity (e.g. using Young’s modulus or stiffness), it seemed that articifial plants and C. demersum were more rigid than all other real plants. If held outside water, the shoots and branches of our artificial plants and living C. demersum retained their shape better than real plants (S2 Fig). Rigidity is therefore the most likely factor explaining the differences in refuge provisioning, which matches with our observations of fish not entering dense patches of artificial plants. Therefore, the role of structural complexity might be context-dependent [67]: plant density and complexity only become functional if plant rigidity is sufficiently high. This is reflected by robust leaf axils of Stratiotes aloides, root mats of floating plants, and emergent reeds providing effective physical refuge to prey [5, 68, 69]. We suggest that plant rigidity could be an important plant trait influencing predatory-prey dynamics.

Predator hunting mode

Predator identity matters for predator-prey dynamics [40, 70, 71]. We hypothesized that predator identity, especially their hunting mode, would be an important variable that determines whether plants offer protection to prey [9, 22]. Indeed, the role of plants as refuge to G. pulex differed between both predators. Under carp predation only C. demersum provided refuge to G. pulex, whereas in the predation trials with A. imperator larvae all plant species increased G. pulex survival. This highlights the importance of studying different predators or evaluating different predator tactics when estimating refuge effects for macroinvertebrates [9]. It again provides an example of context-dependency when elucidating the functional importance of plant traits. Dragonfly larvae excel at ambushing prey from the concealment of plants by using retractable mouthparts to catch their prey [60]. As gammarids were highly active, frequently entering plant patches, their survival likely depended on chance encounter rates and the foraging efficiency of the dragonfly larvae. A previous study also showed that vegetation provides benthic prey with refuge if under predation by phytophilic predators [9]. This increased refuge to benthic prey results from vegetation restricting predator vision and movement. By residing in vegetation, dragonfly larvae predators sacrifice foraging speed for increased safety against fish predation [72]. In habitats with both actively hunting and ambushing predators, plants can simultaneously be a site of refuge and danger [73] depending largely on fauna microhabitat use [9].

In laboratory experiments, predator behaviour can be affected by stress and learning. We attempted to limit fish stress by using swim-through hatches instead of nets to move fish to the experimental arena, and by letting this schooling fish species forage in pairs. Another important aspect of predator-prey interactions is learning. Bluegill sunfish learn that searching for prey more slowly in vegetation improves foraging efficiency [74] and without vegetation this efficiency can be upped fourfold by improving handling and search time [75]. As carp are better learners than bluegill sunfish [76], it is likely that over time, the repeated use of carp increased their foraging efficiency as would happen in natural environments. Due to the randomized testing of plant species across fish pairs over time, the prey survival reported is the averaged efficiency of fish at various stages of experience. Carp learning and their confinement to the experimental arena resulted in a high predation pressure. Whereas in natural systems fish might evade non-profitable habitat such as vegetated areas and find easier prey in open water [75, 77], here carp had to forage in vegetated habitats. This they did successfully, as only prey well hidden in interstitial space inaccessible to carp survived. Compared to the efficient carp, dragonfly larvae foraging was slower and less reliant on searching. Therefore, refuge requirements for prey to benefit under ambushing dragonfly predation appear less demanding than those under searching carp predation.

Synthesis

In conclusion, native and non-native submerged aquatic plants functioned similarly in terms of refuge provision. Instead of plant origin, refuge provision was largely determined by predator and prey identity and only weakly by plant complexity or shoot density. Interestingly, our study indicated that plant rigidity, which is higher for artificial plants than for their corresponding living counterparts, might be a major trait in refuge provisioning. The role of plant complexity in refuge provision is noticeable only when plants are sufficiently rigid. Therefore, results on predator-prey dynamics obtained using artificial plant analogues should be interpreted with care when extrapolating to the effects of living plants. Altogether, these results imply that modification of predator-prey interactions through plant invasions, if present, may occur only when non-native plant species are of strongly contrasting growth form, morphology, rigidity, and density compared to the native plant species.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. Close-up of an experimental arena.

Photograph of an experimental area planted with Myriophyllum spicatum in low density, while on the left a wooden wall separates the fish living area and the experimental area in view. The lower part of this separator acted as a hatch which allowed fish to swim into the experimental area on their own, thereby reducing stress.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Qualitative impression of the rigidity of artificial and real aquatic plants.

Four artificial plant analogues and two real plants were attached horizontally to a vertical metal bar and photographed. The rigidity of whole shoots (longitudinal axis) and branches (lateral axis) is classified as rigid (check mark), less rigid (cross), or not available (NA).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Fractal dimension versus macroinvertebrate prey survival correlation plots.

Mean Daphnia pulex (circles), Gammarus pulex (squares with refuge of living plants and downward triangles with refuge of artificial plants) and damselfly larvae (upward triangles) survival under mirror carp predation in plant refuge of low (closed symbols) or high plant density (open symbols) plotted against the cross-sectional (A) and shoot fractal dimension (B) of plants. Only significant regression lines were plotted for graphical clarity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.s003

(TIF)

S1 Table. Water quality data of carp predation trials.

Mean ± SEM value of multiple parameters over time (n = 24 for water characteristics, n = 5 for nutrient data).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.s004

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Mirror carp pre- and post-experiment information for each individual.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124455.s005

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Nico Helmsing for help with nutrient analysis and Fran Cortes for assistance with the fractal analysis. Furthermore, we thank two anonymous reviewers for helpful feedback that improved the manuscript. This is publication 5819 of the Netherlands Institute of Ecology (NIOO-KNAW).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: BMCG BJAP ESB. Performed the experiments: BMCG. Analyzed the data: BMCG. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: BJAP. Wrote the paper: BMCG BJAP WCEPV ESB.

References

  1. 1. Bridgewater P, Higgs ES, Hobbs RJ, Jackson ST. Engaging with novel ecosystems. Front Ecol Environ. 2011;9(8):423.
  2. 2. Vilà M, Espinar JL, Hejda M, Hulme PE, Jarošík V, Maron JL, et al. Ecological impacts of invasive alien plants: a meta–analysis of their effects on species, communities and ecosystems. Ecol Lett. 2011;14(7):702–708. pmid:21592274
  3. 3. Crooks JA. Characterizing ecosystem–level consequences of biological invasions: the role of ecosystem engineers. Oikos. 2002;97(2):153–166.
  4. 4. Simberloff D. How common are invasion-induced ecosystem impacts? Biol Invasions. 2011;13(5):1255–1268.
  5. 5. Rantala MJ, Ilmonen J, Koskimäki J, Suhonen J, Tynkkynen K. The macrophyte, Stratiotes aloides, protects larvae of dragonfly Aeshna viridis against fish predation. Aquat Ecol. 2004;38(1):77–82.
  6. 6. Warfe DM, Barmuta LA. Habitat structural complexity mediates the foraging success of multiple predator species. Oecologia. 2004;141(1):171–178. pmid:15300485
  7. 7. Finke DL, Denno RF. Spatial refuge from intraguild predation: implications for prey suppression and trophic cascades. Oecologia. 2006;149(2):265–275. pmid:16708227
  8. 8. Hughes AR, Grabowski JH. Habitat context influences predator interference interactions and the strength of resource partitioning. Oecologia. 2006;149(2):256–264. pmid:16705438
  9. 9. Klecka J, Boukal DS. The effect of habitat structure on prey mortality depends on predator and prey microhabitat use. Oecologia. 2014;176(1):183–191. pmid:25085443
  10. 10. Valinoti CE, Ho C- K, Armitage AR. Native and exotic submerged aquatic vegetation provide different nutritional and refuge values for macroinvertebrates. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol. 2011;409(1):42–47.
  11. 11. Chaplin GI, Valentine JF. Macroinvertebrate production in the submerged aquatic vegetation of the Mobile–Tensaw Delta: effects of an exotic species at the base of an estuarine food web. Estuaries Coasts. 2009;32(2):319–332.
  12. 12. Martin CW, Valentine JF. Impacts of a habitat-forming exotic species on estuarine structure and function: an experimental assessment of Eurasian milfoil. Estuaries Coasts. 2011;34(2):364–372.
  13. 13. Mattos KJ, Orrock JL. Behavioral consequences of plant invasion: an invasive plant alters rodent antipredator behavior. Behav Ecol. 2010;21(3):556–561.
  14. 14. Dutra HP, Barnett K, Reinhardt JR, Marquis RJ, Orrock JL. Invasive plant species alters consumer behavior by providing refuge from predation. Oecologia. 2011;166(3):649–657. pmid:21221650
  15. 15. Valley RD, Bremigan MT. Effects of macrophyte bed architecture on largemouth bass foraging: Implications of exotic macrophyte invasions. Trans Am Fish Soc. 2002;131(2):234–244.
  16. 16. Michel MJ, Adams MM. Differential effects of structural complexity on predator foraging behavior. Behav Ecol. 2009;20(2):313–317.
  17. 17. Orrock JL, Dill LM, Sih A, Grabowski JH, Peacor SD, Peckarsky BL, et al. Predator Effects in Predator-Free Space: the Remote Effects of Predators on Prey. Open Ecol J. 2010.
  18. 18. Manatunge J, Asaeda T, Priyadarshana T. The Influence of Structural Complexity on Fish–zooplankton Interactions: A Study Using Artificial Submerged Macrophytes. Environ Biol Fishes. 2000;58(4):425–438.
  19. 19. Savino JF, Stein RA. Predator-prey interaction between largemouth bass and bluegills as influenced by simulated, submersed vegetation. Trans Am Fish Soc. 1982;111(3):255–266.
  20. 20. Warfe DM, Barmuta LA. Habitat structural complexity mediates food web dynamics in a freshwater macrophyte community. Oecologia. 2006;150(1):141–154. pmid:16932971
  21. 21. Diehl S. Foraging Efficiency of Three Freshwater Fishes: Effects of Structural Complexity and Light. Oikos. 1988;53(2):207–214.
  22. 22. Wellborn GA, Skelly DK, Werner EE. Mechanisms creating community structure across a freshwater habitat gradient. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1996;27(1):337–363.
  23. 23. Horinouchi M. Seagrass habitat complexity does not always decrease foraging efficiencies of piscivorous fishes. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2009;377:43–49.
  24. 24. Martin CW, Fodrie FJ, Heck KL Jr, Mattila J. Differential habitat use and antipredator response of juvenile roach (Rutilus rutilus) to olfactory and visual cues from multiple predators. Oecologia. 2010;162(4):893–902. pmid:20127367
  25. 25. Flynn KE, Moon DC. Effects of habitat complexity, prey type, and abundance on intraguild predation between larval odonates. Hydrobiologia. 2011;675(1):97–104.
  26. 26. Figueiredo BRS, Mormul RP, Benedito E. Non-additive effects of macrophyte cover and turbidity on predator–prey interactions involving an invertivorous fish and different prey types. Hydrobiologia. 2013;716(1):21–28.
  27. 27. Ryer CH. Pipefish foraging: effects of fish size, prey size and altered habitat complexity. Marine ecology progress series Oldendorf. 1988;48(1):37–45.
  28. 28. Stoner AW. The influence of benthic macrophytes on the foraging behavior of pinfish, Lagodon rhomboides (Linnaeus). J Exp Mar Biol Ecol. 1982;58(2):271–284.
  29. 29. Bartholomew A, Diaz RJ, Cicchetti G. New dimensionless indices of structural habitat complexity: predicted and actual effects on a predator's foraging success. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2000;206:45–58.
  30. 30. Burks RL, Jeppesen E, Lodge DM. Littoral zone structures as Daphnia refugia against fish predators. Limnol Oceanogr. 2001;46(2):230–237.
  31. 31. Burks RL, Lodge DM. Cued in: advances and opportunities in freshwater chemical ecology. J Chem Ecol. 2002;28(10):1901–1917. pmid:12474890
  32. 32. Stiers I, Crohain N, Josens G, Triest L. Impact of three aquatic invasive species on native plants and macroinvertebrates in temperate ponds. Biol Invasions. 2011;13(12):2715–2726.
  33. 33. Keast A. The introduced aquatic macrophyte, Myriophyllum spicatum, as habitat for fish and their invertebrate prey. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1984;62(7):1289–303.
  34. 34. Strayer DL, Lutz C, Malcom HM, Munger K, Shaw WH. Invertebrate communities associated with a native (Vallisneria americana) and an alien (Trapa natans) macrophyte in a large river. Freshwat Biol. 2003;48(11):1938–1949.
  35. 35. Schultz R, Dibble E. Effects of invasive macrophytes on freshwater fish and macroinvertebrate communities: the role of invasive plant traits. Hydrobiologia. 2012;684(1):1–14.
  36. 36. Hussner A. Alien aquatic plant species in European countries. Weed Res. 2012;52(4):297–306.
  37. 37. McAbendroth L, Ramsay PM, Foggo A, Rundle SD, Bilton DT. Does macrophyte fractal complexity drive invertebrate diversity, biomass and body size distributions? Oikos. 2005;111(2):279–90.
  38. 38. Macneil C, Dick JT, Elwood RW. The dynamics of predation on Gammarus spp.(Crustacea: Amphipoda). Biol Rev. 1999;74(4):375–395.
  39. 39. Van de Meutter F, De Meester L, Stoks R. Water turbidity affects predator–prey interactions in a fish–damselfly system. Oecologia. 2005;144(2):327–336. pmid:15800739
  40. 40. Calcagno V, Sun C, Schmitz OJ, Loreau M. Keystone Predation and Plant Species Coexistence: The Role of Carnivore Hunting Mode. Am Nat. 2011;177(1):E1–E13. pmid:21091211
  41. 41. Folsom TC, Collins NC. The Diet and Foraging Behavior of the Larval Dragonfly Anax Junius (Aeshnidae), with an Assessment of the Role of Refuges and Prey Activity. Oikos. 1984;42(1):105–113.
  42. 42. Klecka J, Boukal DS. Who eats whom in a pool? A comparative study of prey selectivity by predatory aquatic insects. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(6):1–13.
  43. 43. Grime JP. Biodiversity and ecosystem function: the debate deepens. SCIENCE-NEW YORK THEN WASHINGTON-. 1997:1260–1264.
  44. 44. Sheldon RB, Boylen CW. Maximum depth inhabited by aquatic vascular plants. Am Midl Nat. 1977:248–254.
  45. 45. Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat Methods. 2012;9(7):671–675. pmid:22930834
  46. 46. Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, et al. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol. 2009;24(3):127–135. pmid:19185386
  47. 47. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013.
  48. 48. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version. 2013;1(4).
  49. 49. Hothorn T, Bretz F, Westfall P. Simultaneous Inference in General Parametric Models. Biom J. 2008;50(3):346–363. pmid:18481363
  50. 50. Venables WN, Ripley BD. Modern applied statistics with S: Springer; 2002.
  51. 51. Fox J, Weisberg S. An R Companion to Applied Regression. Second Edition ed: Thousand Oaks CA: Sage; 2011.
  52. 52. van Kleunen M, Dawson W, Bossdorf O, Fischer M. The more the merrier: Multi-species experiments in ecology. Basic Appl Ecol. 2014;15(1):1–9.
  53. 53. Van de Meutter F, Stoks R, De Meester L. Behavioral linkage of pelagic prey and littoral predators: microhabitat selection by Daphnia induced by damselfly larvae. Oikos. 2004;107(2):265–272.
  54. 54. Scheinin M, Scyphers SB, Kauppi L, Heck KL, Mattila J. The relationship between vegetation density and its protective value depends on the densities and traits of prey and predators. Oikos. 2012;121(7):1093–1102.
  55. 55. Wright D, Shapiro J. Refuge availability: a key to understanding the summer disappearance of Daphnia. Freshwat Biol. 1990;24(1):43–62.
  56. 56. Lauridsen TL, Lodge DM. Avoidance by Daphnia magna of fish and macrophytes: Chemical cues and predator-mediated use of macrophyte habitat. Limnol Oceanogr. 1996;41(4):794–798.
  57. 57. Pennak RW. Some evidence for aquatic macrophytes as repellents for a limnetic species of Daphnia. Int Rev Gesamten Hydrobiol Hydrogr. 1973;58(4):569–576.
  58. 58. Nihan Tavşanoğlu Ü, Idil Çakiroğlu A, Erdoğan Ş, Meerhoff M, Jeppesen E, Beklioglu M. Sediments, not plants, offer the preferred refuge for Daphnia against fish predation in Mediterranean shallow lakes: an experimental demonstration. Freshwat Biol. 2012;57(4):795–802.
  59. 59. Boersma M, Spaak P, De Meester L. Predator–Mediated Plasticity in Morphology, Life History, and Behavior of Daphnia: The Uncoupling of Responses. The American Naturalist. 1998;152(2):237–248. pmid:18811388
  60. 60. Corbet PS. Dragonflies: behaviour and ecology of Odonata: Harley Books; 1999.
  61. 61. Bartholomew A. Faunal colonization of artificial seagrass plots: the importance of surface area versus space size relative to body size. Estuaries. 2002;25(5):1045–1052.
  62. 62. Gotceitas V, Colgan P. Predator foraging success and habitat complexity: quantitative test of the threshold hypothesis. Oecologia. 1989;80(2):158–166.
  63. 63. Tokeshi M, Arakaki S. Habitat complexity in aquatic systems: fractals and beyond. Hydrobiologia. 2012;685(1):27–47.
  64. 64. Kouwen N, Unny TE. Flexible roughness in open channels. Journal of the Hydraulics Division. 1973;99(5):713–728.
  65. 65. Bal KD, Bouma TJ, Buis K, Struyf E, Jonas S, Backx H, et al. Trade–off between drag reduction and light interception of macrophytes: comparing five aquatic plants with contrasting morphology. Funct Ecol. 2011;25(6):1197–1205.
  66. 66. Katayama M. Stem stiffness plays a role in determining the foraging success of predators. Hydrobiologia. 2014;732(1):173–181.
  67. 67. Verberk W, Van Noordwijk C, Hildrew A. Delivering on a promise: integrating species traits to transform descriptive community ecology into a predictive science. Freshwater Science. 2013;32(2):531–547.
  68. 68. Cazzanelli M, Warming TP, Christoffersen KS. Emergent and floating-leaved macrophytes as refuge for zooplankton in a eutrophic temperate lake without submerged vegetation. Hydrobiologia. 2008;605(1):113–122.
  69. 69. Padial AA, Thomaz SM, Agostinho AA. Effects of structural heterogeneity provided by the floating macrophyte Eichhornia azurea on the predation efficiency and habitat use of the small Neotropical fish Moenkhausia sanctaefilomenae. Hydrobiologia. 2009;624(1):161–170.
  70. 70. Dörner H, Wagner A. Size–dependent predator–prey relationships between perch and their fish prey. J Fish Biol. 2003;62(5):1021–1032.
  71. 71. Wimp GM, Murphy SM, Lewis D, Douglas MR, Ambikapathi R, Van-Tull LA, et al. Predator hunting mode influences patterns of prey use from grazing and epigeic food webs. Oecologia. 2013;171(2):505–515. pmid:22926724
  72. 72. Orrock JL, Preisser EL, Grabowski JH, Trussell GC. The cost of safety: Refuges increase the impact of predation risk in aquatic systems. Ecology. 2013;94(3):573–579. pmid:23687883
  73. 73. Sagrario G, De Los Ángeles M, Balseiro E, Ituarte R, Spivak E. Macrophytes as refuge or risky area for zooplankton: a balance set by littoral predacious macroinvertebrates. Freshwat Biol. 2009;54(5):1042–1053. pmid:19686389
  74. 74. Ehlinger TJ. Learning and individual variation in bluegill foraging: habitat-specific techniques. Anim Behav. 1989;38(4):643–658.
  75. 75. Werner EE, Mittelbach GG, Hall DJ. The role of foraging profitability and experience in habitat use by the bluegill sunfish. Ecology. 1981:116–125.
  76. 76. Coble DW, Farabee GB, Anderson RO. Comparative learning ability of selected fishes. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 1985;42(4):791–796.
  77. 77. Warburton K. Learning of foraging skills by fish. Fish Fish. 2003;4(3):203–215.