Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Neurofunctional Correlates of Environmental Cognition: An fMRI Study with Images from Episodic Memory

  • Aline Vedder ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Aline Vedder, Lukasz Smigielski

    Affiliations Human Science Center, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität (LMU), Munich, Germany, Institute of Sociology, (LMU), Munich, Germany, Institute of Medical Psychology, (LMU), Munich, Germany, Parmenides Center for Art and Science, Pullach, Germany

  • Lukasz Smigielski ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Aline Vedder, Lukasz Smigielski

    Affiliation Human Science Center, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität (LMU), Munich, Germany

  • Evgeny Gutyrchik ,

    evgeny.gutyrchik@med.uni-muenchen.de

    Affiliations Human Science Center, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität (LMU), Munich, Germany, Institute of Medical Psychology, (LMU), Munich, Germany, Parmenides Center for Art and Science, Pullach, Germany

  • Yan Bao,

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, Peking University, Beijing, People’s Republic of China, Key Laboratory of Machine Perception, Peking University, Beijing, People’s Republic of China, Human Science Center, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität (LMU), Munich, Germany, Parmenides Center for Art and Science, Pullach, Germany

  • Janusch Blautzik,

    Affiliation Institute of Clinical Radiology, (LMU), Munich, Germany

  • Ernst Pöppel,

    Affiliations Human Science Center, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität (LMU), Munich, Germany, Institute of Medical Psychology, (LMU), Munich, Germany, Parmenides Center for Art and Science, Pullach, Germany, Department of Psychology, Peking University, Beijing, People’s Republic of China

  • Yuliya Zaytseva,

    Affiliations Human Science Center, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität (LMU), Munich, Germany, Institute of Medical Psychology, (LMU), Munich, Germany, Parmenides Center for Art and Science, Pullach, Germany, Moscow Research Institute of Psychiatry, Moscow, Russia

  • Edmund Russell

    Affiliations Human Science Center, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität (LMU), Munich, Germany, Department of History, University of Kansas, Lawrence, United States of America

Abstract

This study capitalizes on individual episodic memories to investigate the question, how dif-ferent environments affect us on a neural level. Instead of using predefined environmental stimuli, this study relied on individual representations of beauty and pleasure. Drawing upon episodic memories we conducted two experiments. Healthy subjects imagined pleasant and non-pleasant environments, as well as beautiful and non-beautiful environments while neural activity was measured by using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Although subjects found the different conditions equally simple to visualize, our results revealed more distribut-ed brain activations for non-pleasant and non-beautiful environments than for pleasant and beautiful environments. The additional regions activated in non-pleasant (left lateral prefrontal cortex) and non-beautiful environments (supplementary motor area, anterior cortical midline structures) are involved in self-regulation and top-down cognitive control. Taken together, the results show that perceptual experiences and emotional evaluations of environments within a positive and a negative frame of reference are based on distinct patterns of neural activity. We interpret the data in terms of a different cognitive and processing load placed by exposure to different environments. The results hint at the efficiency of subject-generated representations as stimulus material.

Introduction

How do different environments affect us on a neural level? This study capitalizes on individually unique mental imagery to address the issue of environment in a novel way. Instead of using predefined stimuli, this study focuses on the individual and his/her conceptualization of beautiful and pleasant environments. This new approach considers the complexity and inter-individual variability of the relationship between human beings and their environment. In addition, by collecting neural data we scrutinize the indirect effects of different environmental conditions.

Previous empirical research on the relationship between humans and their environment has mostly focused on direct effects and physical risks (e.g., pathogens and toxic substances), as well as on explicit judgments of environmental criteria. But a growing body of evidence has shown that environments have significant indirect effects too [1], [2], [3]. For instance, interacting with nature brings faster recuperation following stress reactions than interacting with urban environments, as shown in self-reports, as well as by tracking appropriate physiological reactions, such as systolic and diastolic blood pressure (SBP, DBP), pulse transit time (PTT), spontaneous skin conductance response (SCR), frontalis muscle tension (EMG), and heart-period data over time [4], [5], [6].

The aesthetic appeal of an environment and its effect upon individuals has been studied and incorporated into various experimental projects in healthcare facilities [7]. It has been documented that figurative art has an impact on measurable health indicators. Patients recovering from open-heart surgery who lived in rooms with images depicting nature, in comparison to those who lived in rooms with blank walls or decorated with abstract paintings, experienced less anxiety and consumed less pain medication [8]. Even differences between artistic and non-artistic images [9] or distinct artistic styles [10] can be linked to different processing mechanisms.

Natural environments may provide high levels of inherently fascinating stimuli which engage involuntary attention, thus giving the directed attention fatigue an opportunity to rest and recuperate [11]. In line with this, environmental psychologists have suggested that an innate love of nature, biophilia, makes nature essential for human well-being [12]. The biophilia hypothesis treats an environmental feature (nature) as an independent variable to which people respond in a universal way.

Therefore, to investigate the impact of environmental conditions on affect and cognition, it is necessary to focus on implicit psychological processes in response to sensory stimulation. Probably the most salient response to an environmental condition is stress. Non-effective stress-combatting strategies may affect our health through chronic arousal, suppression of the immune system, or other forms of allostatic load [13], [14]. In line with this, the concept of effortless processing (e.g. [15]) addresses the cognitive and neural demands that environments place on individuals. It can be assumed that health-promoting environments require less cognitive and neural effort to process than environments that are detrimental to health. Accordingly, the brain of an individual in a health-promoting environment might show less physiological activity than the brain of the same individual in a health-detrimental environment. This degree of physiological activity can be measured indirectly with functional magnetic resonance images of blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals. Using fMRI, Lederbogen et al. (2011) [16] showed that urban upbringing, as well as living in a city, has dissociable impacts on social evaluative stress processing in humans. The authors associate current city living with increased amygdala activity [16]. According to their study, urban upbringing affects the perigenual anterior cingulate cortex, a key region for the regulation of amygdala activity, negative affect, and stress [16]. The study reveals a strong connection between environmental settings and neural mechanisms.

Accordingly, previous research provides a coherent picture of the strong interconnectedness between environmental conditions and well-being. Nevertheless, experimental evidence is required to explain the neural mechanisms that could be responsible for the distinct effects of different environments [17]. Cognitive neuroscience can also provide insight into implicit perceptual experiences of aesthetics, its neural foundation, and its relation to other cognitive processes [18], [19].

Previous experimental studies typically asked subjects to respond to environmental stimuli preselected by experimenters. These studies often tested for universal differences, while masking variation among individuals. Albeit external stimuli, such as pictures, are highly important in the evaluation of environmental cognition processes and anthropological universals, these stimuli do not touch upon the aspects that might be characteristic and unique to individual subjective representations of positive and negative environments. There exist cultural and individual differences in regard to attractive and natural elements of environmental surroundings [20]. Even personality traits may modulate aesthetic experiences [21].

Consequently, this study aims to replace universal conceptualizations of beauty and pleasure with individual, subject-generated definitions of beautiful/non-beautiful and pleasant/non-pleasant environments to examine the neurocognitive processing of positive and negative environments. We asked subjects to tap into their rich reservoirs of episodic memory through mental imagery, an ability to generate mental representations of a stimulus in the absence of the stimulus itself, but preserving its perceptible properties. A rich remembered experience is a defining feature of episodic memory recall [22]. Episodic memory capitalizes on the interlinking of implicit and explicit knowledge frames [19]. Accumulated findings document that imagery and perception draw on most of the same neural machinery [23], including both cognitive level [24], [25], [26] and neural topography [27], [28], [29]. Belardinelli and her colleagues (2009) [30] demonstrated that the ability to generate vivid mental images influences both the format and the neural activation levels of image formation. Highly-vivid subjects, in comparison to lowly-vivid subjects create even more analogic images, characterized by higher levels of neuronal equivalence with perception.

Capitalizing on the methodological power of functional magnetic resonance imaging, controlled introspection in the form of episodic memories can be used to access the individual environmental experiences of the study participants. Accordingly, in our study, we were interested in the impact of different environments on cognitive processes. Which physiologic mechanisms can be identified for the positive or negative effects of environments? With the decision to investigate beautiful/non-beautiful as well as pleasant/non-pleasant environments, the study aim was to explore whether certain neural patterns are typical for positive and negative frames of reference. Both pleasure and beauty refer to the appeal of an environment on the perceiving person. One condition was chosen that describes the external sensation linked to the experience of an environment (beautiful/non-beautiful) while the other condition describes the internal sensation (pleasant/non-pleasant) linked to the experience of an environment. Considering previous research, we hypothesized that positive and potentially health-promoting environments require different mental effort to process than negative environments.

Methods

Participants

From a pool of 150 volunteers, we selected 16 right-handed (as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [31]) subjects (nine females, mean age 23.1), fluent in German language, who had scored high on the ability to generate vivid mental representations as assessed in the Questionnaire Upon Mental Imagery ([32], mean score 54.4). The questionnaire assesses the vividness of inner representations in seven sensory modalities (visual, auditory, cutaneous, kinaesthetic, gustatory, olfactory and organic). The short version we used for the current study, has 35 items [32], five for each of seven modalities. The responses are rated on a seven point scale where (1) = perfectly clear and as vivid as the actual experience, and (7) = no imagery present. The study participants were informed about the standard fMRI exclusion criteria (implanted metal objects in the bodies, history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, pregnancy, claustrophobia), and a written consent was obtained from all participants. The Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty at the University of Munich approved the protocol. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Stimuli

Prior to the fMRI experiment, all 16 participants took part in a one-hour preparatory training session in which they were instructed first to imagine with their eyes closed and then to describe in writing self-chosen beautiful (German term: schön) and non-beautiful as well as pleasant (German term: wohltuend) and non-pleasant environments. Subjects were free to choose scenes from their own individual experience. Written recordings were encouraged to help them refresh and store images and sensations in memory for further use in the main experiment. Inside the scanner, participants viewed the simplified instructions via a mirror attached to the head-coil on a LCD screen.

Procedure

We performed fMRI scans while the subjects imagined four sceneries in four experimental conditions (beautiful and non-beautiful; pleasant and non-pleasant). The first experiment required the participants to imagine beautiful and non-beautiful environments while the second experiment required the imagination of pleasant and non-pleasant environments. Half of the participants had to imagine beautiful and non-beautiful environments first while the other half had to imagine pleasant and non-pleasant environments first. A block design was used with 8 blocks per each condition, each block comprising instructions for the subjects.

Tasks and procedures were discussed in detail in the training sessions before the experiment. The order of blocks was pseudo-randomized (Presentation, Neurobehavioral Systems, USA). The instructions were presented for 2 seconds, followed by a black screen displayed for 18 seconds while subjects imagined the different environmental conditions. Since the subjects were asked to keep their eyes closed during each imagination period, an acoustic signal marked the end of the visualization session. Next, the participants had 2 seconds to respond to a control question asking whether the imagined scenery was easy or difficult to imagine by pressing either “yes” or “no” on a button (LUMItouch, Photon Control Inc, Canada). With this control question we could assesses the intensity of the task, obtain an immediate and intuitive answer, and encourage the concentration and attention of the study participants in the scanner. A fixation asterisk appeared on the screen for 6 seconds after each block.

Scanning and data analysis

The experiment was conducted with a 3T system (Philips ACHIEVA, Best, The Netherlands) at the University Hospital LMU Munich. Foam cushions securely, but comfortably, fastened the subject’s head to minimize movements. As anatomical reference and to detect potential morphological anomalies, a T1-weighted, magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence was performed: repetition time (TR) = 2400 ms, echo time (TE) = 3.06 ms, flip angle (FA) = 9°, number of slices = 160, matrix = 224 x 256, spatial resolution 1 x 1 mm. Structural images were acquired in sagittal orientation. For BOLD imaging, a T2*-weighted EPI sequence was used (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 35 ms, FA = 90°, 28 axial slices covering whole cerebrum, slice thickness = 4 mm, inter-slice gap = 0.4 mm, ascending interleaved acquisition, FOV = 230 x 230 mm, matrix = 128 x 128, in-plane resolution = 1.8 x 1.8 mm). In total, 248 functional volumes were acquired. Functional images were acquired in axial orientation (parallel to the anterior commissure—posterior commissure [AC-PC] line).

To account for T1 saturation effects, the first five volumes in each run were excluded from further analysis. The functional images were realigned, co-registered, and spatially normalized into standard stereotaxic space (EPI template; Montreal Neurologic Institute, MNI), re-sliced to 2 x 2 x 2 mm voxels, and smoothed with an 8 mm full-width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel using SPM8 software (Statistical Parametric Mapping; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). T-contrast images were created versus baseline for each subject at the first level. After estimation of the random-effects second level (full-factorial design), statistical parametric maps were thresholded at p <.001 (corrected for multiple comparisons at cluster-level using the family-wise error correction at p (FWE) <.05).

The clusters were anatomically described using the AAL atlas (Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations [33]; AAL Toolbox for SPM8, http://www.gin.cnrs.fr/spip.php?article217).

Results

Experiment 1: Beautiful and non-beautiful environments

Subjects reported that both environmental conditions were easy to imagine (M = 84.9, SD = 13.7% for the beautiful and M = 79.5, SD = 14.7% for the non-beautiful setting). The difference was not statistically significant (t(7) = 0.77, p >.05).

Compared to baseline, conjunction analysis revealed activations in the supplementary motor area as well as in the superior frontal gyrus for both beautiful and non-beautiful conditions.

For the non-beautiful condition as compared to the beautiful condition, activations were registered in the anterior cortical midline structures (anterior cingulate, medial prefrontal cortex, supplementary motor area), right superior frontal gyrus, as well as in the left superior, middle, inferior frontal gyri, insular cortex, orbitofrontal cortex and temporal pole. The opposite comparison, beautiful vs. non-beautiful condition, revealed no significant activations.

Fig 1 and Table 1 contain detailed quantitative information about the observed relationship.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Neurofunctional processing of beautiful and non-beautiful environments.

(A) Conjunction of beautiful and non-beautiful environments versus baseline and (B) higher neural activation for non-beautiful environments. SM = supplementary motor area, SFG = superior frontal gyrus, LPFC = lateral prefrontal cortex, STG = superior temporal gyrus, MTG = middle temporal gyrus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122470.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. Neurofunctional processing of beautiful and non-beautiful environments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122470.t001

Experiment 2: Pleasant and non-pleasant environments

Subjects reported that both environmental conditions were easy to imagine (M = 95.3, SD = 6.2% for the pleasant and M = 93.0, SD = 12.9% for the non-pleasant setting). The difference was not statistically significant (t(7) = 0.72, p >.05).

Compared to baseline, conjunction analysis revealed activations in the visual and sensorimotor cortex over baseline for both pleasant and non-pleasant conditions.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging showed significantly more cortical activations when subjects imagined non-pleasant environments than when they imagined pleasant environments. The higher blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal appeared primarily in the left prefrontal cortex (the ventrolateral part including Broca’s speech area). The opposite comparison, pleasant vs. non-pleasant condition, revealed no significant activations.

Fig 2 and Table 2 contain detailed quantitative information about the observed relationship.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Neurofunctional processing of pleasant and non-pleasant environments.

(A) Conjunction of pleasant and non-pleasant environments versus baseline and (B) higher neural activation for non-pleasant environments. VC = visual cortex, SM = supplementary motor area, SFG = superior frontal gyrus, LPFC = lateral prefrontal cortex.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122470.g002

thumbnail
Table 2. Neurofunctional processing of pleasant and non-pleasant environments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122470.t002

Discussion

The results of this study show that a positive and a negative frame of reference elicit distinct neural patterns of environmental cognition. We assume that non-beautiful and non-pleasant environments demand more mental processing than beautiful and pleasant environments. Although behavioral data illustrates that subjects found the different environmental conditions equally easy to visualize, imagining non-beautiful and non-pleasant environments activated more brain areas, revealing a greater diversification of brain reactivity than beautiful and pleasant environments. The results correlate with previous propositions to explain the experience of negative environments as characterized by the demand on more mental resources than the experience of positive environments. Taken together, cognitive experiences within different environmental frames are based on different patterns of neural activity.

The perception of non-pleasant environments is characterized by a higher neural response in the left prefrontal cortex. Previous research has identified the left prefrontal cortex as a key structure in the executive-control system [34]. The hypothesis that cognitive costs correlate with activation of brain areas responsible for executive control, particularly the left prefrontal cortex, has found solid support in functional MRI experiments [35]. Thus, the weaker BOLD signal for pleasant mental scenarios in the left lateral prefrontal cortex suggests a lower cognitive load, which indicates that positive and potentially health-promoting environments might require less effort in neural processing.

A similar pattern of increased neural activity can be observed for the processing of non-beautiful environments. The non-beautiful condition elicits activity in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) that can be linked to the online monitoring of behavior based on emotional cues and somatic signals [36]. Other areas activated in the non-beautiful condition indicate top-down cognitive control (right superior frontal gyrus and left inferior frontal gyrus) [37], [38] and response to an increase in executive demand in the working memory system (left superior frontal gyrus) [39]. In addition, a stronger activation of the supplementary motor area (SMA) can be observed. The SMA has been suggested to be a motor-limbic interface contributing to the transformation of visually-triggered emotional experiences into motor actions [40]. The non-beautiful condition also causes increased activity in the anterior cortical midline structures, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). The role of the ACC-mPFC network (having vast projections to the amygdala) in the appraisal of negatively charged emotional stimuli has been suggested previously [41]. Many studies highlight that the same circuitry is also involved in emotion regulation [42] [43]. Activity in the ACC has also been linked to a conscious mental effort related to cognitive processing [44]. Thus, an enhanced signal in the ACC and ACC-mPFC network may imply that the processing of non-beautiful settings demands increases in top-down cognitive control and regulatory mechanisms. In line with this, the stronger BOLD signal in the left insular cortex for the processing of non-beautiful environments can be attributed to higher emotional arousal. There is evidence that the insula plays an important role in generating anticipatory signals, which are critical for learning about aversive outcomes [45]. Activity in the left insular cortex may point to the mobilization of greater neural resources toward discomfort-inducing, unappealing, and potentially aversive events.

Considering the data, we propose that interacting with a negative environment requires an additional investment in emotion processing, cognitive control, and motor function. The neural results locate causal mechanisms for the fundamental effects of different environments and support the effortless processing concept of cognition [15]. This argumentation finds additional support from the results of the study conducted by Martinez-Soto and colleagues [46] on the neural correlates of restorative environments. The exposure to low restorative pictures revealed a more distributed brain pattern (same as in our study) and activated the cortical areas related to direct, endogenous and top-down attention, in comparison with the high restorative pictures activating more involuntary, exogenous and bottom-up attentional resources.

The implications of our study can be considered with respect to the discussion on the impact of the physical environment on cognitive processing, evidence-driven studies of human-environment interaction, and even health-related parameters.

The task to imagine beautiful/non-beautiful and pleasant/non-pleasant environments can be considered to be a complex cognitive process. Thus, it is plausible that no single cortical region can wholly account for the different experimental conditions. To our knowledge, this is the first study to measure the influence of mental representations of different environments on a neural level using functional MRI.

Some limits of the study point to areas for future research. Our focus was an inner experience based on a conscious, quasi-perceptual phenomenon of mental imagery fed by episodic memory. Extending the data to real perceptual experiences should be performed with caution in spite of the fact that similar neural structures are involved in both instances. The results also raise questions about temporal frames and the long-term neural effects of different environmental conditions [47]. Furthermore, intercultural comparisons would help to clarify the extent to which certain effects of environmental cognition can be generalized. A formulation of evidence-based postulates on how to mold and organize physical space for the benefit of individuals should be the aim of future research. Accordingly, the most urgent challenge for future research will be to elucidate the neural mechanisms through which environments affect our well-being and health.

The results of this study highlight the difference in neural activity linked to the experience of positive and negative environments. The outcome is remarkable, considering that subjects were simply imagining different environments rather than responding to real environments. Taken together, the observed patterns of brain responsiveness have led us to conclude that the neurofunctional processing of positive environments in comparison to negative environments might involve less processing costs, less cognitive monitoring, and emotion regulation.

Supporting Information

S1 Graphic. Schematic representation of the experimental design for one fMRI session.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122470.s001

(JPG)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: LS EG ER EP. Performed the experiments: LS EG ER JB. Analyzed the data: AV YB YZ LS EG ER. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: EG ER EP. Wrote the paper: AV LS EG ER.

References

  1. 1. Schweitzer M, Gilpin L, Frampton S. Healing Spaces: Elements of Environmental Design That Make an Impact on Health. J Altern Complem Med. 2004; 10: 71–83.
  2. 2. Ulrich R. View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science. 1984; 224: 420–21. pmid:6143402
  3. 3. Wells NM, Evans GW, Yang Y. Environments and Health: Planning Decisions As Public-Health Decisions. J Archit Plan Res. 2010; 27 (2): 124–143.
  4. 4. Ulrich RS, Simons RF, Losito BD, Fiorito E, Miles MA, Zelson M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. 1991; 11(3): 201–230.
  5. 5. Laumann K, Gärling T, Stormark KM. Rating scale measures of restorative components of environments. J Environ Psychol. 2001; 21: 31–44.
  6. 6. Hartig T, Evans GW, Jamner LD, Davis DS, Garling T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J Environ Psychol. 2003; 23: 109–123.
  7. 7. Staricoff R, Loppert S. Integrating the arts into healthcare: can we effect clinical outcomes? In: Kirklin D, Richardson R, editors. The Healing Environment: Without and Within. London: RCP. 2003. pp. 63–79.
  8. 8. Ulrich RS, Lunden O, Eltinge JL. Effects of exposure to nature and abstract pictures on patients recovering from heart surgery. Psychophysiology. 1993; 30 (Suppl. 1): 7.
  9. 9. Lutz A, Nassehi A, Bao Y, Pöppel E, Sztrókay A, Reiser M, et al. Neurocognitive processing of body representations in artistic and photographic images. Neuroimage. 2013; 66: 288–292. pmid:23123681
  10. 10. Silveira S, Graupmann V, Frey D, Blautzik J, Meindl T, Reiser M, et al. Matching Reality in the Arts: Self-Referential Processing of Naturalistic Compared to Surrealistic Images. Perception. 2012; 41: 569–576. pmid:23025160
  11. 11. Kaplan S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J Environ Psychol. 1995; 15: 169–182.
  12. 12. Kellert SR, Wilson EO. The Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington D.C.: Island Press; 2013.
  13. 13. Sapolsky RM. Why zebras don’t get ulcers. The acclaimed guide to stress, stress-related diseases, and coping. New York: Henry Holt and Company; 2004.
  14. 14. McEwen B. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New Engl J Med. 1998; 338: 171–179. pmid:9428819
  15. 15. Bao Y, Pöppel E. Anthropological universals and cultural specifics: Conceptual and methodological challenges in cultural neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav R. 2012; 36: 2143–2146. pmid:22813993
  16. 16. Lederbogen F, Kirsch P, Haddad L, Streit F, Tost H, Schuch P, et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature. 2011; 474: 498–501. pmid:21697947
  17. 17. Bratman GN, Hamilton JP, Daily GC. The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2012; 1249: 118–136. pmid:22320203
  18. 18. Avram M, Gutyrchik G, Bao Y, Pöppel E, Reiser M, Blautzik J. Neurofunctional correlates of aesthetic and moral judgments: Equal but not the same. Neurosci Lett. 2013; 534: 128–132. pmid:23262080
  19. 19. Pöppel E, Bao Y. Three modes of knowledge as basis for intercultural cognition and communication: A theoretical perspective In: Han S, Pöppel E, editors. Culture and Neural Frames of Cognition and Communication. Heidelberg: Springer Verlag; 2011. pp. 215–231.
  20. 20. Bujis AE, Elands BHM, Langers F. No wilderness for immigrants: cultural differences in images of nature and landscape preferences. Landscape Urban Plan. 2009; 91: 113–123.
  21. 21. Park M, Hennig-Fast K, Bao Y, Carl P, Pöppel E, Welker L, et al. Personality traits modulate neural responses to emotions expressed in music: An fMRI study. Brain Res. 2013; 1523: 68–76. pmid:23732338
  22. 22. Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002; 53: 1–25. pmid:11752477
  23. 23. Ganis G, Thompson WL, Kosslyn SM. Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Res. 2004; 20: 226–241. pmid:15183394
  24. 24. Finke RA. Theories relating mental imagery to perception. Psycho Bull; 1985. 98: 236–259. pmid:3901061
  25. 25. Ishai A, Sagi D. Common mechanisms of visual imagery and perception. Science; 1995. 268: 1772–1774. pmid:7792605
  26. 26. Kosslyn SM, Sukel KE, Bly BM. Squinting with the mind’s eye: Effects of stimulus resolution on imaginal and perceptual comparisons. Mem Cognition. 1999; 27: 276–287. pmid:10226438
  27. 27. Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Kim IJ, Alpert NM. Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex. Nature. 1995; 378: 496–498. pmid:7477406
  28. 28. Borts G, Kosslyn SM. Visual mental imagery and visual perception: Structural equivalence revealed by scanning processes. Mem Cognition; 2008. 36(4): 849–862. pmid:18604966
  29. 29. Cichy R, Heinzle J, Haynes JD. Imagery and Perception Share Cortical Representations of Content and Location. Cereb Cortex. 2012; 22: 372–380. pmid:21666128
  30. 30. Belardinelli MO, Palmiero M, Sestieri C, Nardo D, Di Matteo R, Londei A, et al. An fMRI investigation on image generation in different sensory modalities: The influence of vividness. Acta Psychol. 2009; 132(2): 190–200. pmid:19695558
  31. 31. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971; 9(1): 97–113. pmid:5146491
  32. 32. Sheehan P. A shortened form of Bett’s Questionnaire Upon Mental Imagery. J Clin Psychol. 1967; 23(3): 386–389. pmid:6082130
  33. 33. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002; 15: 273–289. pmid:11771995
  34. 34. Wagner AD, Maril A, Bjork RA, Schacter DL. Prefrontal contributions to executive control: fMRI evidence for functional distinctions within lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2001; 14: 1337–1347. pmid:11707089
  35. 35. McGuire JT, Botvinick MM. Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010; 107: 7922–7926. pmid:20385798
  36. 36. Beer JS, John OP, Scabini D, Knight RT. Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2006; 18: 871–879. pmid:16839295
  37. 37. Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Mangun GR. The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat Neurosci. 2000; 3: 284–291. pmid:10700262
  38. 38. Swick D, Ashley V, Turken AU. Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci. 2008; 9: 102. pmid:18939997
  39. 39. Weidner R, Pollmann S, Müller HJ, von Cramon DY. Top-down controlled visual dimension weighting: an event-related fMRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2002; 12: 318–328. pmid:11839605
  40. 40. Oliveri M, Babiloni C, Filippi MM, Caltagirone C, Babiloni F, Cicinelli P, et al. Influence of the supplementary motor area on primary motor cortex excitability during movements triggered by neutral or emotionally unpleasant visual cues. Exp Brain Res; 2003. 149: 214–221. pmid:12610690
  41. 41. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010; 15(2): 85–93. pmid:21167765
  42. 42. Beauregard M, Levesque J, Bourgouin P. Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. J Neurosci. 2001; 21: 1–6. pmid:11312316
  43. 43. Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, Robertson ER, Chopra S, Gabrieli JDE, et al. For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage. 2004; 23: 483–499. pmid:15488398
  44. 44. Mulert C, Menzinger E, Leicht G, Pogarell O, Hegerl U. Evidence for a close relationship between conscious effort and anterior cingulate cortex activity. Int J Psychophysiol. 2005; 56: 65–80. pmid:15725491
  45. 45. Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Gati JS, Clare S, Menon RS, Matthews PM, Rawlins JN. Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science. 1999; 284: 1979–1981. pmid:10373114
  46. 46. Martínez-Soto J, Gonzales-Santos L, Pasaye E, Barrios FA. Exploration of neural correlates of restorative environment exposure through functional magnetic resonance. Intelligent Buildings International. 2013; 5 (S1): 10–28.
  47. 47. Pöppel E, Bao Y. Temporal windows as bridge from objective time to subjective time. In: Lloyd D, Arstila V, editors. Subjective Time: the philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of temporality. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2014. pp. 241–261.