Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Experimental Shifts in Intraclutch Egg Color Variation Do Not Affect Egg Rejection in a Host of a Non-Egg-Mimetic Avian Brood Parasite

  • Rebecca Croston ,

    RebeccaLCroston@gmail.com

    Current address: Ecology, Evolution, and Conservation Biology Program in Biology, University of Nevada - Reno, Reno, Nevada, United States of America

    Affiliation Ecology, Evolutionary Biology, and Behavior Subprogram in Biology, The Graduate Center of the City University of New York, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Mark E. Hauber

    Affiliations Ecology, Evolutionary Biology, and Behavior Subprogram in Biology, The Graduate Center of the City University of New York, New York, New York, United States of America, Department of Psychology, Hunter College and The Graduate Center of the City University of New York, New York, New York, United States of America

Abstract

Avian brood parasites lay their eggs in the nests of other birds, and impose the costs associated with rearing parasitic young onto these hosts. Many hosts of brood parasites defend against parasitism by removing foreign eggs from the nest. In systems where parasitic eggs mimic host eggs in coloration and patterning, extensive intraclutch variation in egg appearances may impair the host’s ability to recognize and reject parasitic eggs, but experimental investigation of this effect has produced conflicting results. The cognitive mechanism by which hosts recognize parasitic eggs may vary across brood parasite hosts, and this may explain variation in experimental outcome across studies investigating egg rejection in hosts of egg-mimicking brood parasites. In contrast, for hosts of non-egg-mimetic parasites, intraclutch egg color variation is not predicted to co-vary with foreign egg rejection, irrespective of cognitive mechanism. Here we tested for effects of intraclutch egg color variation in a host of nonmimetic brood parasite by manipulating egg color in American robins (Turdus migratorius), hosts of brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater). We recorded robins’ behavioral responses to simulated cowbird parasitism in nests where color variation was artificially enhanced or reduced. We also quantified egg color variation within and between unmanipulated robin clutches as perceived by robins themselves using spectrophotometric measures and avian visual modeling. In unmanipulated nests, egg color varied more between than within robin clutches. As predicted, however, manipulation of color variation did not affect rejection rates. Overall, our results best support the scenario wherein egg rejection is the outcome of selective pressure by a nonmimetic brood parasite, because robins are efficient rejecters of foreign eggs, irrespective of the color variation within their own clutch.

Introduction

Hosts of brood parasitic birds face fitness costs associated with the rearing of genetically unrelated parasitic offspring [1]. Many hosts, across widely divergent brood parasite-host systems, have evolved defenses which either decrease the chances of being parasitized, or reduce the costs incurred as a result of parasitism [2, 3]. These defenses range from aggressive responses to adult brood parasites near the nests [4, 5] through foreign egg rejection [6, 7] to the rejection of brood parasitic chicks and fledglings (reviewed in [8]).

Recognition and removal of parasitic eggs from the nest is the most common host defense against parasitism [8,9]. Egg ejection, however, is an imperfect defense, and can itself lead to fitness losses for hosts through misrecognition and–rejection (i.e., recognition errors), or accidental damage to the hosts own eggs (i.e., rejection costs) [10,11]. Because of these costs, evolutionary theory predicts that hosts involved in an arms race with brood parasites will be under selective pressure to avoid recognition and rejection errors [12]. For some brood parasites, this results in laying of eggs mimicking host eggs in appearance (mimetic eggs [13]), while others lay eggs that do not appear to mimic those of their hosts (non-mimetic eggs [14], but see [15]). Egg mimicry is unlikely to evolve in systems where hosts do not reject parasitic eggs (i.e. no selective pressure toward mimicry), where parasites exploit a wide range of hosts with divergent egg phenotypes, or where there is evolutionary lag between parasites and hosts [9].

For hosts of mimetic-egg laying brood parasites, there are at least two strategies toward reducing the likelihood of recognition errors. A parasitized species can evolve towards 1) reduced within-clutch (intraclutch) variation in egg appearance (color and maculation), and/or 2) egg appearance unlike the parasitic eggs (e.g. [16,17]), effectively increasing between-clutch (interclutch) variability [1722]. One or both of these patterns in clutch variation has been observed in many host species of the common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus, [20,21,2325] but see [26]) and Diederik cuckoo (Chrysococcyx caprius, [22]), as well as in rejecters of intraspecific (functionally mimetic) parasitic eggs [27]. Many other observational, and a handful of experimental studies have, however, found inconsistent support for these patterns [21,2832], and thus the literature as a whole is equivocal as to what extent brood parasitism and egg recognition fuel or limit the evolutionary trajectories of variation in both intra- and intraclutch egg color variability (Tables 1, 2).

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of published studies using observational tests of the relationship between intraclutch egg appearance variability and rejection rate by hosts of obligate brood parasitic birds.a

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121213.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary of published studies on egg rejection responses (relative to controls) to experimental brood parasitism, where the methodology included manipulations to increase intraclutch egg appearance variation.a

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121213.t002

For hosts of non-mimetic parasites, however, predictions about the effect of intra- and interclutch color variation are not yet well defined, and the effect of brood parasitism on intraclutch color variation is rarely addressed (but see [35]) for hosts of these types of brood parasites (Table 1, 2). We maintain and formalize (Table 3), that in the absence of egg mimicry, intraclutch color variation, and therefore also interclutch color variation, is relatively unconstrained by hosts’ need to recognize and reject foreign eggs, and is therefore free to vary in response to alternative selective pressures, physiological factors, and ecological factors including maternal condition, diet, and/or local predation pressures [4043]. Alternatively, patterns of egg color variation may result from selective pressure from past inter- [44] or intraspecific [45] parasitism. While identifying the exact mechanism is beyond the scope of our study, we note that increased between-clutch egg color variation cannot be a definitive signature of selective pressure to reject eggs of a non-mimetic parasite, despite that the opposite trend more robustly indicates selective pressure to reject mimetic parasitic eggs. Similarly, experimentally increasing intraclutch color variation in these hosts is not predicted to affect rates of parasitic egg rejection (Table 3). Overall, hypotheses pertaining to the effects of parasitism on intraclutch color, and effects of intraclutch color on rejection rates, have gone largely untested in hosts of non-mimetic parasites, as there is little intraspecific variation in response to parasitism for these hosts [36]. Two studies to date have addressed the relationship between rejection rate and degree of intraclutch variation in a host of brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater, a generalist brood parasite) host. Peer et al. [36] found that cowbird egg rejection was more likely when intraclutch variation was lower for common grackles (Quiscalus quiscula). However, in a subsequent comparison between acceptor versus rejector hosts laying blue and maculate beige eggs, respectively, intraclutch color variation did not vary with rejector status across either group [35].

thumbnail
Table 3. Summary of predictions for egg color variation and responses to experimental increase in intraclutch color variation based on different cognitive mechanisms underlying egg recognition, as a result of coevolution per se with mimetic versus nonmimetic brood parasites.a

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121213.t003

Here we test for patterns of egg color variation within versus between unmanipulated non-mimetic brood parasite host clutches, and test for effects of experimentally increasing or decreasing intraclutch egg color variation on the likelihood of parasitic egg rejection. We combine observational and experimental approaches to analyze within and between-clutch color variation in a population of American robins (Turdus migratorius), a robust egg-rejecting host of obligate parasitic brown-headed cowbirds [2,46]. American robins are one of only ~26 hosts of extremely generalist [47,48] brown-headed cowbirds [49] to reject artificial and real cowbird eggs in up to 100% of trials where nests are experimentally parasitized [2,46]. We compare inter- and intraclutch color variation across the entire avian visual spectrum by combining spectrophotometric measures of egg color with statistical models describing the birds’ own spectral sensitivities [50]. Robins lay immaculate blue-green eggs, and cowbird eggs do not closely mimic those of their hosts [1] (but see [15]). Additionally, using artificial parasitism in combination with egg color manipulation, we experimentally test predictions associated with the role of intraclutch color variation in eliciting egg rejection (Table 3). Critically, we assess the effects of both experimentally increasing and decreasing intraclutch color variation but note that our theoretical considerations (Table 3) make a prediction of no effect on egg rejection rates for robins under either type of experimental treatment. If, however, robins evolved to reject foreign eggs due to selective pressures imposed by mimetic brood parasites (including intraspecific brood parasitism [45]), then we predict a decrease in egg rejection rates following experimental increases in intraclutch variability, if robins employ at least one of two known cognitive mechanisms used by rejecters to identify foreign eggs (Table 3).

Materials and Methods

Study site and nests

This study took place in and around Ithaca, Tompkins County, NY, USA from May-July in the breeding seasons 2010–2012. Nests were located through searches in and around buildings, bridges, barns, and clearing edges, especially in residential areas and farmland. Additional nests were located by enlisting the help of local residents using classified advertisements, signboards, and local internet communities.

Egg color measurement and avian visual modeling

During the 2012 breeding season, we quantified eggshell color for complete, unmanipulated American robin clutches. All color measurements were taken on either the day of, or the first day following clutch completion, as blue-green color may fade over the course of incubation [51]. We quantified egg color across the entire avian visual spectrum by measuring spectral reflectance using a high resolution spectrometer with deuterium tungsten halogen light source and 455μm solarization-resistant shielded cable (Ocean Optics Jaz portable spectrometer with ultraviolet-visible (UV-VIS) light source, Ocean Optics Inc., Dunedin, FL, USA). Measurements were taken holding the fiber optic probe perpendicular to the egg surface. The spectrometer was calibrated using a Spectralon light reflectance standard (WS-1, Ocean Optics, Inc., which reflects > 95% of UV and visible light), and a black-box standard, which measures baseline noise in the spectrophotometer. The spectrometer was re-calibrated after measuring every third egg throughout sampling. The relative reflectance at each wavelength was calculated automatically with reference to the light and dark standards. To minimize measurement error, each egg was measured nine times, including three measurements each at the blunt pole, middle, and sharp pole, which were then averaged to yield one spectrum per egg.

To estimate degree of color variation both within and between unmanipulated robin clutches with respect to the spectral sensitivities of avian photoreceptors [52], we used the Vorobyev and Osorio [53] model for tetrachromatic vision in AVICOL v5 avian visual modeling software [54]. American robins are an ultraviolet-sensitive (UVS) species [5557], but detailed spectral sensitivity data are not as yet available. We therefore extracted spectral sensitivity data for a congener, the European blackbird T. merula, from the published data in Hart et al. [58] using Vistametrix software (Vista Metrix 1.3, SkillCrest LLC) and ranging from 330–700nm. AVICOL requires sensitivity data ranging from 300–700 nm; we set photoreceptor absorbance for 300–330 nm to 0, sensu [59,60]. Relative cone densities were set to ultraviolet sensitive (UVS): 1.0, short-wavelength sensitive (SWS): 1.78, medium-wavelength sensitive (MWS): 2.21, long-wavelength sensitive (LWS): 1.96, and Weber fraction was set to 0.1 [58] sensu [59]. As the ability to discriminate different colors is influenced by environmental light [53] (but see [61]), we used published ambient light irradiance data for broken canopy forest [53], which may most closely simulate the variable forest-edge light environments in which many American robins nest, even when breeding in sub/urban sites [62].

Prior to analysis, we applied a correction to each egg spectra using triangular smoothing over 30 nanometers, available as a function within AVICOL, to attenuate the effect of spectrometer noise on the visual model. AVICOL extracts receptor catch quanta specific to each single-cone receptor type, and combines these with the known spectral sensitivities of the model taxon’s visual system (here T. merula) to quantify photoreceptor activity across the entire avian spectral sensitivity range and quantify birds’ abilities to distinguish between any two colors as the perceptual distance between spectra (ΔS), or JNDs (‘just noticeable differences’). By definition, JND values greater than 1.0 indicate a chromatic difference that is discriminable based on the published estimates of T. merula spectral sensitivities [63]. AVICOL can also be used to extract discriminability based on achromatic contrasts using the sum of the sensitivities of the MWS and LWS cones, as these are similar to the sensitivities of rods and principal double-cone cells in the avian retina [58].

For the sensory analysis, we extracted photoreceptor catches for each of the four avian single-cone receptors, and normalized these to 1 within the total reflectance of each egg, such that for each egg, we have calculated the proportion of total receptor catch that is attributable to each photoreceptor. We compared mean quantum catches for each photoreceptor across all nests using independent univariate ANOVAs, with the proportionate receptor catches (PrUVS, PrSWS, PrMWS, PrLWS) as response variables, and Nest ID as predictor. Likewise, we compared mean achromatic quantum catches among nests, repeating the above approach with achromatic quantum catch data for each egg, and comparing means across nests.

Finally, we compared discriminable difference values between eggs sharing a nest and eggs not sharing a nest as JNDs. To do this, we calculated JNDs differentiating each egg from every other egg in the data set. Then, to avoid pseudoreplication, we randomly selected among these paired comparisons such that each egg was included in the analysis only once. We compared mean within-nest JNDs to mean between-nest JNDs using univariate ANOVA, with type of comparison (within nest/between nest) as a predictor and JNDs as response.

Egg rejection experiment

To experimentally test whether intraclutch color variation contributes to the ability of American robins to recognize and reject foreign eggs, we manipulated egg color within clutches, experimentally decreasing or increasing egg shell color variability [37]. We altered host eggs according to one of two treatments: in each, we removed eggs one at a time from nests, and painted each egg with one of two different blue/blue-green paints (acrylic, Artist’s Loft), chosen by spectrophotometric specifications of hue as determined by wavelength at peak reflectance, and of known [64], low (0–20% for cowbird-sized model eggs) rejection rates. Eggs were allowed to dry fully before being returned to the nest.

In order to increase the amount of color variation within a clutch (increased color variation treatment; IV), we painted two eggs with either pale robin-mimetic or vivid robin-mimetic paint (Fig. 1) at random, and the third egg was painted the second blue-green shade (Fig. 1; see also inset). The second treatment group consisted of nests where the amount of color variation within a clutch was artificially decreased by painting all 3 eggs in the clutch with the same paint shade (Vivid robin-mimetic paint, decreased color variation treatment; DV). We added a third, unmanipulated group (UNM) of nests using data from previous years sensu [65]. These nests consisted of clutches containing 2–4 eggs whose colors were not altered but where the eggs were handled and treated otherwise identical to IV and DV nests, and inspected with the same frequency and manner.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Representative egg color spectra, with experimentally manipulated nest (inset).

Representative spectra showing each of the three colors used in the egg rejection experiment, in addition to natural American robin egg spectrum. Pale-mimetic and vivid-mimetic paints were used to manipulate the color of real robin eggs. Blue paint was used to color plaster-of-Paris model parasitic eggs. The unmanipulated spectrum represents the average spectrum of real robin eggs. Inset shows a representative nest with experimentally increased variation in egg color, showing two natural robin eggs painted with vivid-mimetic paint, one painted with pale-mimetic paint, and one blue model egg.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121213.g001

Subsequent to manipulating host egg color, we returned the following day, and artificially parasitized nests with plaster-of-Paris model eggs painted a third shade of blue (Fig. 1), also of known rejection rate (58%) from previous experiments [64]. We chose a blue model parasitic egg because behavioral responses to model mimetic cowbird eggs are invariable in our robin population (rejected in 100% of experimental trials, [64]), whereas exposure to the blue model yielded an intermediate rejection rate (58%), with a variable host response [64]. Model ‘parasitic’ eggs mimicked the mass and dimensions of real cowbird eggs, measuring 2.6–3.3 g and with dimensions 21 x 16 mm [66] (as cited in [67]). Model cowbird eggs are known to be rejected at statistically similar rates to real eggs in artificial parasitism studies with American robins [2]. We employed a one-day latency between manipulation and experimental parasitism in order to ensure that no host eggs were rejected as a result of color manipulation (a single pale mimetic host egg was rejected at n = 1 nest, out of 27 total nests). Where a 4th egg was laid after manipulation, this was removed in order to keep clutch sizes consistent across IV and DV nests. In most cases, hosts were theoretically able to view these 4th eggs alongside painted eggs for a period of 1–4 hours before removal. Because the degree of variation remained greater in IV than in DV treatments even in nests where a 4th egg was laid, and because hosts were free to view their full unmanipulated clutches in cases where we discovered a clutch already containing 3 eggs, we do not consider that this limited exposure to additional natural eggs has effected the robins’ rejection decisions [65].

We monitored experimental and control nests by returning daily and visually determining the status of the artificial egg, using binoculars and small nest-mirrors as necessary. Eggs were considered rejected if they were not present in the nest on the day following a previous nest-check with the egg still present, except when hatching or predation may have occurred. Model eggs were considered accepted if they remained in the nest for 6 consecutive days (sensu [68]), after which nests were emptied, as painting the egg shells inhibits respiratory exchange and thereby prevents proper embryonic development. By disposing of the eggs immediately, we minimized the loss of parental investment and maximize the likelihood of renesting. For each nest we recorded the treatment, date of parasitism (Julian date), nesting stage (laying/incubation), and outcome of parasitism (accept/reject).

Frequency tables of treatment (IV, DV, control) and outcome (accept/reject) data were analyzed using Fisher’s exact test with Monte Carlo simulation based on 2000 replicates. We next evaluated possible effects of nesting stage, clutch size, and Julian date by including these as covariates in fitting a binomial logistic generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with treatment group and incubation stage as additional possible predictors, and year as a random variable. Experimental parasitism during the laying stage was defined as taking place at any time before or on the day the last egg was laid; at any point beyond it was considered as taking place during the incubation stage. We also fit a binomial logistic generalized linear model (GLM) with variables as listed above, but with year treated as a fixed effect in order to verify that results were not biased by parsing data across only three year levels (sensu [69]).

Because our hypotheses predict no effect of manipulating clutch color variation (Table 3), we have also included here a power analysis for our experimental manipulation. All analyses were conducted in R version 2.12.1. This study was conducted in accordance with guidelines for animal care and use as approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Hunter College of the City University of New York (permit number MH 2/13-T3). All manipulations took place on private property and with the explicit consent of the property owner.

Results

Avian visual modeling of egg color analysis

Mean quantum receptor catches for natural robin eggs differed significantly more between nests (n = 23) than expected based on variation within nests, for four avian single-cone photoreceptors (ultraviolet, UVS; short wavelength, SWS; medium wavelength, MWS; long wavelength, LWS), and for achromatic photoreceptors (Table 4). Mean chromatic discriminability as JNDs, (n = 35 comparisons) was greater between nests than would be expected based on variation within nests (Table 4).

Egg rejection experiment

We found no significant effect of experimentally increasing or decreasing intraclutch color variation on the probability of egg rejection (across all groups Fisher’s exact test, p = 0.59; with Monte Carlo simulation, p = 0.60; Fig. 2). Likewise, the probability of egg rejection did not differ between IV and DV nests (Fisher’s exact test, p = 1; with Monte Carlo simulation, p = 1). In a generalized linear mixed model, the likelihood of egg rejection was not statistically predicted by treatment, clutch size, Julian date of artificial parasitism, or incubation stage (binomial logistic regression; see Table 5). Results were qualitatively similar for a model using identical predictors, but where year was treated as a nominal variable and a fixed effect (all p > 0.05).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Summary of results of experimental parasitism following the manipulation of clutch contents.

Bars represent the rejection rates for parasitic eggs in each experimental group (+ binomial SE estimates). Sample sizes are indicated inside bars.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121213.g002

We also conducted a power analysis using the true effect size from the Fisher’s exact test above, as Cramer’s V. Based on Cramer’s V = 0.16 for our actual data set, statistical power = 0.10. To achieve statistical power of 0.8 for this low true effect size, n = 396 nest manipulations would be necessary.

Discussion

Our behavioral experiments showed that the degree of intraclutch color variation had no effect on hosts’ ability to reject parasitic eggs, because the rejection of our model parasitic eggs was independent of intraclutch color variation manipulation. Importantly, in this study we tested for effects of both increasing and decreasing intraclutch color variation. To our knowledge, the effect of decreasing intraclutch color variation has been addressed in only a single prior study, Bán et al. [38], in which investigators manipulated entire great reed warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) clutches, dying some eggs or entire clutches with the same color paint or with 3–5 different colors. As predicted for this host of a mimetic brood parasite, the common cuckoo (Table 3), the rejection rates of foreign egg colors in nests with more experimental intraclutch variability were decreased relative to rejection rates in nests with less variability. Notably, relative rejection rates across different color manipulations remained consistent across egg color treatments with different levels of mimicry [70], such that more mimetic blue eggs, for example, were always rejected least often, and less mimetic orange eggs were rejected most often. This implies that hosts use a relative color-based sensory threshold to make decisions whether or not to reject foreign eggs, but responses may be modified by context, as in multiple parasitism [38]. These conclusions highlight the need for further study testing for effects of both increasing and decreasing intraclutch color variation, in order to clearly test for effects on foreign egg rejection.

Some variation in response to experimental manipulation of intraclutch color variation for hosts of mimetic brood parasites (Table 2) may be the result of hosts’ differential use of cognitive mechanisms in the decision to reject foreign eggs (Table 3). Hosts may recognize parasitic eggs using one or more of the following cognitive mechanisms (or additional mechanisms not listed here, e.g. [71,72]): in 1) discordancy-based recognition, hosts use the current nest contents to assess egg identity, and remove egg(s) which are unlike the rest of the clutch [73,74]. In 2) template-based recognition, host females compare clutch contents to a template of their own eggs from memory, with each egg evaluated against the acceptance threshold anchored by this template [28,75]. Template based-recognition allows the discrimination and rejection of foreign eggs when no host eggs are available in the clutch due to multiple parasitism [38]. Finally, in 3) online self-referent phenotype matching, hosts use the current nest contents to assess egg identity, but rejection is not dependent on relative numbers of each egg type within the clutch, as each egg is compared with the hosts known eggs ([38], sensu [76]) as identified shortly after laying [12,76].

Even for hosts of mimetic parasites, isolating and testing the specific cognitive mechanisms driving parasitic egg rejection remain challenging. For example, Stevens et al. [39] concluded that tawny-flanked prinias (Prinia subflava) use both template-based recognition and discordancy in rejection decision-making, because the rejection of mimetic experimental eggs was mediated in part by the relative numbers of host and parasitic eggs in the clutch. However, they have also shown that prinias rarely reject their own eggs when clutch contents are manipulated such that these are in the minority. Here Stevens et al. [39] may have referred to discordancy without the predictable rejection of the egg in the minority in the clutch, and conflated this with differences in proportion of host versus parasite eggs in the nest. The need to clearly identify the cognitive mechanism underlying egg discrimination highlights the importance for protocols to be better designed to tease apart such closely tied proximate mechanisms, manipulating clutch contents such that specific and different predictions can be made and tested under each cognitive mechanism (Table 3).

However, differential use of cognitive mechanism cannot explain any variation among hosts of non-mimetic parasites because parasitism by a non-mimetic parasite per se is predicted to have no effect on the intraclutch color variation or rejection rates for hosts (Table 3). Likewise, investigations of intraclutch color variation and its effect on egg rejection will also be of limited utility in parsing cognitive mechanisms underlying egg rejection in these hosts [38,39,65]. If hosts of non-mimetic parasites utilize a discordancy-based recognition system [70], experimental manipulation of intraclutch variation cannot effect rejection unless clutch contents are modified specifically to make hosts eggs appear similar to parasite eggs, guaranteeing that these eggs are generalizable and recognizable as foreign and allowing the test to focus only on responses elicited by differences in egg number. Likewise, if hosts utilize template-based recognition [77], the characteristics of the existing clutch are again not relevant to decision-making, irrespective of intraclutch variation, unless the recognition template is updated frequently and/or parasitism rates are consistently high. If hosts utilize online self-referent phenotype matching [76], experimental manipulation of intraclutch color variation can only affect rejection rates if hosts are not allowed to view their own eggs at any point prior to manipulation.

In parallel with several other studies focusing on hosts of both mimetic and non-mimetic brood parasitic birds (Table 1, 2), our observations of natural egg coloration in the robins revealed significantly higher perceivable variation between clutches than within clutches, across the sensitivity ranges for all avian photoreceptors. However, increased inter- vs. intraclutch variation in eggshell coloration has been repeatedly detected not only amongst hosts (but see [35]), but also amongst non-hosts of brood parasitic birds [78,79], and thus cannot be a critical test of coevolutionary history with mimetic brood parasitism.

Overall, these results support predictions associated with coevolution between non-mimetic parasitic cowbirds and egg-rejecter robins. In addition, we also demonstrated that for hosts of non-mimetic parasites, parsing the cognitive mechanisms used to make rejection decisions is theoretically challenging. Further research should be focused toward devising new treatments and designs to tease apart the cognitive mechanisms driving parasitic egg rejection (e.g. [70]), particularly in hosts of non-mimetic parasites, where testing for effects of both increasing and decreasing intraclutch color variation does not provide informative tests between alternative cognitive mechanisms (Table 3).

Supporting Information

S1 Dataset. Behavioral outcomes of artificial parasitism.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121213.s001

(TXT)

S2 Dataset. Color spectra of unmanipulated robin eggs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121213.s002

(TXT)

Acknowledgments

For comments and advice we thank Zachary Aidala, Jennifer Basil, Tomas Grim, David Lahti, Lisa Manne, Lainga Tong, and Michael Webster.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: RC MEH. Performed the experiments: RC. Analyzed the data: RC MEH. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: RC MEH. Wrote the paper: RC MEH.

References

  1. 1. Davies NB. Cuckoos, Cowbirds, and other Cheats. Cuckoos, Cowbirds, and other Cheats. London: Poyser; 2000.
  2. 2. Rothstein SI. An experimental and teleonomic investigation of avian brood parasitism. Condor. 1975;77:250–71.
  3. 3. Davies NB. Cuckoo adaptations: trickery and tuning. J. Zool. 2011;284:1–14.
  4. 4. Sealy SG, Neudorf DL, Hobson KA, Gill SA. Nest defense by potential hosts of the Brown-headed Cowbird: methodological approaches, benefits of defense, and coevolution. Oxford Ornithol. Ser. 1998;9:194–211.
  5. 5. Feeney WE, Welbergen JA, Langmore NE. The frontline of avian brood parasite–host coevolution. Anim. Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2012;84:3–12.
  6. 6. Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Braa AT. Rejection behavior by common cuckoo hosts towards artificial brood parasite eggs. Auk. 1991;108:348–54.
  7. 7. Langmore NE, Kilner RM, Butchart SHM, Maurer G, Davies NB, Cockburn A, et al. The evolution of egg rejection by cuckoo hosts in Australia and Europe. Behav. Ecol. 2005;16:686–92.
  8. 8. Grim T. Ejecting chick cheats: a changing paradigm? Front. Zool. BioMed Central Ltd; 2011;8:14.
  9. 9. Rothstein SI. A model system for coevolution: avian brood parasitism. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1990;21:481–508.
  10. 10. Lorenzana JC, Sealy SG. Fitness costs and benefits of cowbird egg ejection by gray catbirds. Behav. Ecol. 2001;12:325–9.
  11. 11. Antonov A, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Kleven O, Honza M, Røskaft E. Eggshell strength of an obligate brood parasite: a test of the puncture resistance hypothesis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2006;60:11–8.
  12. 12. Moskát C, Hauber ME. Conflict between egg recognition and egg rejection decisions in common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) hosts. Anim. Cogn. 2007;10:377–86. pmid:17279422
  13. 13. Honza M, Šulc M, Jelínek V, Požgayová M, Procházka P. Brood parasites lay eggs matching the appearance of host clutches. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2014;281.
  14. 14. Davies NB, Brooke M de L. Cuckoos versus reed warblers: Adaptations and counteradaptations. Anim. Behav. 1988. p. 262–84.
  15. 15. Klippenstine DR, Sealy SG. Assessing generalized egg mimicry: A quantitative comparison of eggs of Brown-headed Cowbirds and grassland passerines. Wilson J. Ornithol. 2010;122:346–53.
  16. 16. Spottiswoode CN, Stevens M. How to evade a coevolving brood parasite: egg discrimination versus egg variability as host defences. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2011;278:3566–73. pmid:21490019
  17. 17. Spottiswoode CN, Stevens M. Host-parasite arms races and rapid changes in bird egg appearance. Am. Nat. 2012;179:633–48. pmid:22504545
  18. 18. Davies NB, Brooke MDL. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, and its hosts. I. Host egg discrimination. J. Anim. Ecol. 1989;58:207–24.
  19. 19. Davies NB, Brooke M de L. Cuckoos versus hosts. Experimental evidence for coevolution. In: Rothstein S, SK R, editors. Parasitic Birds and Their Hosts. Studies in Coevolution. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.
  20. 20. Øien IJ, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Evolution of variation in egg color and marking pattern in European passerines: adaptations in a coevolutionary arms race with the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus. Behav. Ecol. 1995;6:166–74.
  21. 21. Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Obligate brood parasites as selective agents for evolution of egg appearance. Evolution. 2002;56:199–205. pmid:11913664
  22. 22. Lahti DC. Evolution of bird eggs in the absence of cuckoo parasitism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2005;102:18057–62. pmid:16326805
  23. 23. Soler JJ, Moller AP. A comparative analysis of the evolution of variation in appearance of eggs of European passerines in relation to brood parasitism. Behav. Ecol. 1996;7:89–94.
  24. 24. Moskát C, Szentpeteri J, Barta Z. Adaptations by great reed warblers to brood parasitism: A comparison of populations in sympatry and allopatry with the common cuckoo. Behaviour. 2002;139:1313–29.
  25. 25. Avilés JM, Møller AP. Meadow pipit (Anthus pratensis) egg appearance in cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) sympatric and allopatric populations. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2003;79:543–9.
  26. 26. Karcza Z, Moskát C, Cherry MI, Kisbenedek T. Experimental manipulation of intraclutch variation in the great reed warbler shows no effect on rejection of parasitic eggs. Ethology. 2003;109:15–22.
  27. 27. Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Rudolfsen G, Honza M. Rejection of artificial cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) eggs in relation to variation in egg appearance among reed warblers (Acrocephalus scirpaceus). Proc. R. Soc. B. The Royal Society; 1999;266:1483–8.
  28. 28. Lotem A, Nakamura H, Zahavi A. Constraints on egg discrimination and cuckoo-host co-evolution. Anim. Behav. 1995;49:1185–209.
  29. 29. Procházka P, Honza M. Do Common Whitethroats (Sylvia communis) discriminate against alien eggs ? J. Ornithol. 2003;363:354–63. pmid:14579588
  30. 30. Avilés JM, Soler JJ, Soler M, Møller AP. Rejection of parasitic eggs in relation to egg appearance in magpies. Anim. Behav. 2004;67:951–8.
  31. 31. Lovaszi P, Moskát C. Break-down of arms race between the red-backed shrike (Lanius collurio) and common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus). Behaviour. 2004;141:245–62.
  32. 32. Cherry MI, Bennett ATD, Moskát C. Host intra-clutch variation, cuckoo egg matching and egg rejection by great reed warblers. Naturwissenschaften. 2007;94:441–7. pmid:17252240
  33. 33. Landstrom M, Heinsohn R, Langmore NE. Clutch variation and egg rejection in three hosts of the pallid cuckoo, Cuculus pallidus. Behaviour. 2010;147:19–36.
  34. 34. Soler JJ, Soler M, Møller AP. Host recognition of parasite eggs and the physical appearance of host eggs: the Magpie and its brood parasite the Great Spotted Cuckoo. Etologia. 2000;8:9–16.
  35. 35. Abernathy VE, Peer BD. Intraclutch variation in egg appearance of Brown-headed Cowbird hosts. Auk. 2014;131:467–75.
  36. 36. Peer BD, Rothstein SI, McCleery RA. Intraclutch egg variation constrains cowbird egg rejection in Common Grackles. Auk. 2010;127:759–64.
  37. 37. Moskát C, Avilés JM, Bán M, Hargitai R, Zölei A. Experimental support for the use of egg uniformity in parasite egg discrimination by cuckoo hosts. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2008;62:1885–90.
  38. 38. Bán M, Moskát C, Barta Z, Hauber ME. Simultaneous viewing of own and parasitic eggs is not required for egg rejection by a cuckoo host. Behav. Ecol. 2013;24:1014–21.
  39. 39. Stevens M, Troscianko J, Spottiswoode CN. Repeated targeting of the same hosts by a brood parasite compromises host egg rejection. Nat. Commun. Nature Publishing Group, a division of Macmillan Publishers Limited. 2013;4:2475. pmid:24064931
  40. 40. Weidinger K. Does egg colour affect predation rate on open passerine nests? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2001;49:456–64.
  41. 41. Moreno J, Osorno JL. Avian egg colour and sexual selection: does eggshell pigmentation reflect female condition and genetic quality? Ecol. Lett. 2003;6:803–6.
  42. 42. Avilés JM, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E, Møller AP. Environmental conditions influence egg color of reed warblers Acrocephalus scirpaceus and their parasite, the common cuckoo Cuculus canorus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2007;61:475–85.
  43. 43. Lovell PG, Ruxton GD, Langridge K, Spencer KA. Individual quail select egg-laying substrate providing optimal camouflage for their egg phenotype. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:260–4. pmid:23333313
  44. 44. Rothstein SI. Relic behaviours, coevolution and the retention versus loss of host defences after episodes of avian brood parasitism. Anim. Behav. 2001;61:95–107. pmid:11170700
  45. 45. Samas P, Hauber ME, Cassey P, Grim T. Host responses to interspecific brood parasitism: a by-product of adaptations to conspecific parasitism? Front. Zool. 2014;11:34. pmid:24834103
  46. 46. Briskie JV, Sealy SG, Hobson KA. Behavioral Defenses against Avian Brood Parasitism in Sympatric and Allopatric Host Populations. Evolution. 1992;46:334–40.
  47. 47. Friedmann H. The cowbirds: A study in the biology ofsocial parasitism. The cowbirds: A study in the biology of social parasitism. Springfield Illinois: Charles C. Thomas; 1929.
  48. 48. Friedmann H. Further information of the host relations of the parasitic cowbirds. Auk. 1971;88:239–55.
  49. 49. Peer BD, Sealy SG. Correlates Of Egg Rejection In Hosts Of The Brown-Headed Cowbird. Condor. 2004;106:580–99.
  50. 50. Cassey P, Honza M, Grim T, Hauber ME. The modelling of avian visual perception predicts behavioural rejection responses to foreign egg colours. Biol. Lett. 2008;4:515–7. pmid:18593669
  51. 51. Moreno J, Lobato E, Morales J. Eggshell blue-green colouration fades immediately after oviposition: a cautionary note about measuring natural egg colours. Ornis Fenn. 2011;88:51–6.
  52. 52. Bennett ATD, Théry M. Avian color vision and coloration: Multidisciplinary evolutionary biology. Am. Nat. JSTOR; 2007;169:S1–6.
  53. 53. Vorobyev M, Osorio D. Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proc. R. Soc. B. The Royal Society; 1998;265:351–8. pmid:9523436
  54. 54. Gomez D. AVICOL v.5, a program to analyse spectrometric data [Internet]. 2010. Available: http://sites.google.com/site/avicolprogram/.
  55. 55. Chen DM, Collins JS, Goldsmith TH. The ultraviolet receptor of bird retinas. Science. 1984;225:337–40. pmid:6740315
  56. 56. Chen DM, Goldsmith TH. Four spectral classes of cone in the retinas of birds. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 1986;159:473–9. pmid:3783500
  57. 57. Aidala Z, Huynen L, Brennan PLR, Musser J, Fidler A, Chong N, et al. Ultraviolet visual sensitivity in three avian lineages: paleognaths, parrots, and passerines. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2012;198:495–510. pmid:22534772
  58. 58. Hart NS, Partridge JC, Cuthill IC, Bennett ATD. Visual pigments, oil droplets, ocular media and cone photoreceptor distribution in two species of passerine bird: the blue tit (Parus caeruleus L.) and the blackbird (Turdus merula L.). J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2000;186:375–87. pmid:10798725
  59. 59. Igic B, Leuschner N, Parker KA, Ismar SMH, Gill BJ, Lovegrove TG, et al. Size dimorphism and avian-perceived sexual dichromatism in a New Zealand endemic bird, the whitehead Mohoua albicilla. J. Morphol. 2010;271:697–704. pmid:20058295
  60. 60. Igic B, Cassey P, Grim T, Greenwood DR, Moskát C, Rutila J, et al. A shared chemical basis of avian host-parasite egg colour mimicry. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2012;279:1068–76. pmid:21920975
  61. 61. Honza M, Procházka P, Morongová K, Čapek M, Jelínek V. Do nest light conditions affect rejection of parasitic eggs? A test of the light environment hypothesis. Ethology. 2011;117:539–46.
  62. 62. Sallabanks R, James F. American Robin (Turdus migratorius). Birds North Am. Online. 1999.
  63. 63. Osorio D, Vorobyev M. Colour vision as an adaptation to frugivory in primates. Proc. R. Soc. B. 1996;263:593–9. pmid:8677259
  64. 64. Croston R, Hauber ME. Spectral tuning and perceptual differences do not explain the rejection of brood parasitic eggs by American robins (Turdus migratorius). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2013. pmid:25013266
  65. 65. Moskát C, Bán M, Székely T, Komdeur J, Lucassen RWG, van Boheemen L, et al. Discordancy or template-based recognition? Dissecting the cognitive basis of the rejection of foreign eggs in hosts of avian brood parasites. J. Exp. Biol. 2010;213:1976–83. pmid:20472785
  66. 66. Bent A. Life histories of North American blackbirds, orioles, tanagers, and allies. US Natl. Museum Bull. 1958;211.
  67. 67. Lowther P. Brown-headed Cowbird (Molothrus ater). Birds North Am. Online. 1993. Available: http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/.
  68. 68. Honza M, Polačiková L, Procházka P. Ultraviolet and green parts of the colour spectrum affect egg rejection in the song thrush (Turdus philomelos). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2007;92:269–76.
  69. 69. Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, et al. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2009;24:127–35. pmid:19185386
  70. 70. Moskát C, Zölei A, Bán M, Elek Z, Tong L, Geltsch N, et al. How to spot a stranger’s egg? A mimicry-specific discordancy effect in the recognition of parasitic eggs. Herberstein M, editor. Ethology. 2014;120:616–26.
  71. 71. Akre KL, Johnsen S. Psychophysics and the evolution of behavior. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2014;29:291–300. pmid:24726346
  72. 72. Lyon BE. Egg recognition and counting reduce costs of avian conspecific brood parasitism. Nature. 2003;422:495–9. pmid:12673243
  73. 73. Rothstein SI. Mechanisms of avian egg recognition: possible learned and innate factors. Auk. 1974;91:796–807.
  74. 74. Lyon B. Mechanism of egg recognition in defenses against conspecific brood parasitism: American coots (Fulica americana) know their own eggs. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2007;61:455–63.
  75. 75. Hauber ME, Moskát C, Bán M. Experimental shift in hosts’ acceptance threshold of inaccurate-mimic brood parasite eggs. Biol. Lett. 2006;2:177–80. pmid:17148357
  76. 76. Hauber ME, Sherman PW. Self-referent phenotype matching: theoretical consideration and empirical evidence. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:609–16. pmid:11576676
  77. 77. Lotem A. Learning to recognize nestlings is maladaptive for cuckoo Cuculus canorus hosts. Nature. 1993;362:743–4.
  78. 78. Cassey P, Portugal SJ, Maurer G, Ewen JG, Boulton RL, Hauber ME, et al. Variability in avian eggshell colour: a comparative study of museum eggshells. PLOS ONE. 2010;5:e12054. pmid:20711258
  79. 79. Cassey P, Ewen JG, Marshall NJ, Vorobyev M, Blackburn TM, Hauber ME. Are avian eggshell colours effective intraspecific communication signals in the Muscicapoidea? A perceptual modelling approach. Ibis. 2009;151:689–98.