Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Apolipoprotein ε4 Is Associated with Lower Brain Volume in Cognitively Normal Chinese but Not White Older Adults

  • Jennifer S. Yokoyama ,

    jyokoyama@memory.ucsf.edu

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Allen K. L. Lee,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Leonel T. Takada,

    Affiliations Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America, Hospital das Clinicas, University of Sao Paulo Medical School, Sao Paulo, Brazil

  • Edgar Busovaca,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Luke W. Bonham,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Steven Z. Chao,

    Affiliations Department of Neurology, Veterans Affairs Health Care system, Palo Alto, California, United States of America, Department of Neurology and Neurological Science, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, California, United States of America

  • Marian Tse,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Jing He,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America

  • Christopher G. Schwarz,

    Affiliation Department of Radiology, Mayo Clinic and Foundation, Rochester, Minnesota, United States of America

  • Owen T. Carmichael,

    Affiliations Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America, Department of Computer Science, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America

  • Brandy R. Matthews,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, Indiana, United States of America

  • Anna Karydas,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Michael W. Weiner,

    Affiliations Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Center for Imaging of Neurodegenerative Diseases, San Francisco, California, United States of America, Department of Radiology, University of California San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Giovanni Coppola,

    Affiliation Departments of Psychiatry & Neurology, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Charles S. DeCarli,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America

  • Bruce L. Miller,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  •  [ ... ],
  • Howard J. Rosen

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • [ view all ]
  • [ view less ]

Abstract

Studying ethnically diverse groups is important for furthering our understanding of biological mechanisms of disease that may vary across human populations. The ε4 allele of apolipoprotein E (APOE ε4) is a well-established risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and may confer anatomic and functional effects years before clinical signs of cognitive decline are observed. The allele frequency of APOE ε4 varies both across and within populations, and the size of the effect it confers for dementia risk may be affected by other factors. Our objective was to investigate the role APOE ε4 plays in moderating brain volume in cognitively normal Chinese older adults, compared to older white Americans. We hypothesized that carrying APOE ε4 would be associated with reduced brain volume and that the magnitude of this effect would be different between ethnic groups. We performed whole brain analysis of structural MRIs from Chinese living in America (n = 41) and Shanghai (n = 30) and compared them to white Americans (n = 71). We found a significant interaction effect of carrying APOE ε4 and being Chinese. The APOE ε4xChinese interaction was associated with lower volume in bilateral cuneus and left middle frontal gyrus (Puncorrected<0.001), with suggestive findings in right entorhinal cortex and left hippocampus (Puncorrected<0.01), all regions that are associated with neurodegeneration in AD. After correction for multiple testing, the left cuneus remained significantly associated with the interaction effect (PFWE = 0.05). Our study suggests there is a differential effect of APOE ε4 on brain volume in Chinese versus white cognitively normal elderly adults. This represents a novel finding that, if verified in larger studies, has implications for how biological, environmental and/or lifestyle factors may modify APOE ε4 effects on the brain in diverse populations.

Introduction

Ethnic diversity is important in medical research because differences in genetic background, environment and other sociocultural aspects (diet, language, access to care, etc.) may influence disease risk and manifestations. These differences have important implications for clinical management, particularly when there are established associations between ethnicity and risk for a disease as well as treatment response. Examples include genetic risk for isolated, late-onset cardiac amyloidosis in African Americans [1], gefitinib response in Japanese women with non-small cell lung carcinoma [2], and genetic contributions to asthma severity and bronchodilator response in admixed Hispanic populations [35]. An understanding of the differential effects of genetic factors across diverse populations and their effects on underlying biology is critical for furthering research and informing medical practice.

Apolipoprotein E ε4 (APOE ε4) is a well-known risk factor affecting the likelihood and age of onset of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [612], with a dose dependence characteristic (i.e., two alleles are associated with an increased risk compared to one) [12,13]. Even in clinically normal elderly, there is evidence that APOE genotype affects likelihood of cognitive decline [1416], and affects brain structure and function as measured by structural [1719] and functional [2025] neuroimaging, and neuropathology [26].

Frequency of the APOE ε4 allele varies across ancestral populations, with highest frequency in African populations (e.g., ~0.3 in Nigerians), medium frequency in European populations (e.g., ~0.14 in the UK) and lowest frequency in East Asian populations (e.g., ~0.07 in Chinese) [27]. The influence of APOE ε4 on AD varies across ethnic groups [6,2831]. In addition to genetic differences across ethnicities, environmental and lifestyle factors also likely modulate how APOE ε4 alters the risk of AD. For example, Farrer, et al. [6] found that APOE ε4 showed weaker risk effects in African American and Hispanic individuals but stronger effects in Japanese when compared to white individuals.

Nearly 3.4 million ethnic Chinese live in America (in 2010 [32]), but they are still underrepresented in dementia studies [33,34]. In studies from China, APOE ε4 has been correlated with AD risk [7,3537], as well as cognitive decline and memory performance in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [3840]. Neuroimaging studies found smaller hippocampal volumes in symptomatic ε4 carriers [35,41], but not among cognitively normal ε4-carrying controls [35]. Little to no research has been done to directly compare APOE ε4 effects between Chinese and white individuals.

In this study, we sought to investigate the role APOE ε4 genotype plays on brain structure in cognitively normal Chinese older adults, and to compare those patterns with a cohort of white Americans. We chose to study cognitively normal older adults for two reasons. First, structural changes in ε4 carriers—particularly in the hippocampal formation—may appear as early as infanthood [42] and adolescence [18], though measurable cognitive changes may only occur decades later [16]. Second, measures of cognitive impairment across diverse populations may be complicated by differences in language and culture [43,44]. By studying the baseline effects of APOE ε4 in older adults from ethnically diverse populations we could assess whether there are differential effects of ε4 on brain anatomy that may have implications for AD risk.

Methods

Subjects

All American participants were members of on-going studies in aging and cognition at the Memory and Aging Center (MAC) at the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF). Procedures for recruitment, enrollment, and for determining eligibility and ethnicity for Chinese Americans have been described in detail [33]. These participants are recruited through the Chinese Outreach portion of the MAC Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (ADRC) from the San Francisco community through a variety of methods including: clinic assessments at the UCSF MAC and two clinic sites in Chinatown (the Chinatown Public Health Center and Chinese Hospital); lectures to local health care providers and community members; participation in community events; publications in mass media; word of mouth. The Chinese Outreach team consists of three bilingual and bicultural staff (one neurologist and two research assistants) that staff all research visits for Chinese Americans who speak primarily Chinese (Cantonese or Mandarin) or are bilingual Chinese/English. They translate all brochures and consent forms into Chinese and administer cognitive testing in Chinese. Chinese participants from Shanghai were recruited from the Jing’an district as described [41]. Study eligibility included diagnosis of clinically normal, available genetic information, and available MRI scans. White participants were selected to be as similar as possible to the Chinese (American + Shanghai) cohort with regard to age, sex, education, APOE ε4 allele distribution, and MR image acquisition field strength. For detailed inclusion and exclusion criteria see S1 Text. All subjects provided IRB-approved, written informed consent prior to participation, and all tests were approved by the University of California, San Francisco Committee on Human Research.

Clinical Evaluation

All participants underwent a thorough multidisciplinary evaluation, including neurological exam, medical history, informant interview and cognitive testing. English-speaking research participants were tested using a standard battery to assess a broad range of cognitive domains including memory, executive and language [45,46]. The cognitive testing battery used at the MAC for Chinese speaking individuals is comprised of several measures validated for use in Chinese and others translated locally from English tasks to assess the same cognitive domains as the English-language battery. Research participation also included nursing evaluation that encompasses the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR [47]), and laboratory evaluation (described in [33,45]). Evaluation for Shanghai participants was administered as described41. For more details, see S1 Text.

Image Acquisition and White Matter Hyperintensity (WMH) quantification

American participants underwent T1-weighted imaging using 1.5T (N = 4 Chinese, N = 4 white), 3T (N = 11 Chinese, N = 59 white), and 4T (N = 26 Chinese, N = 8 white) systems, with previously described sequences [48]. FLAIR image acquisition is provided in S1 Text. Automated WMH quantification in American participants was performed as described in Supporting Information [49]. Shanghai Chinese image acquisition and WMH quantification was performed as described [41]. Participants’ first available MRI was used for analysis and occurred within one year of their neuropsychological exam [41].

Genotyping

Genomic DNA was extracted from peripheral blood using standard protocols (Gentra PureGene Blood Kit, QIAGEN, Inc.—USA, Valencia, CA). APOE genotyping (rs429358 and rs7412) was conducted using a TaqMan Allelic Discrimination Assay on an ABI 7900HT Fast Real-Time PCR system (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA) according to manufacturer’s instructions.

Statistical Analysis

Two-tailed Pearson’s chi-squared analysis (nominal data) or ANOVA (interval data) was used to evaluate associations between demographic information or WMH volume and ethnic group byε4 carrier status. Two-tailed repeated measures ANOVA was used to test for significant change in neuropsychological test scores in Chinese participants for which longitudinal data was available. Analyses were performed in Stata/MP (v10.1, StataCorp LP, College Station, TX).

A standard image preprocessing and DARTEL warping pipeline was applied, and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was performed on all structural MRIs using vlsm2.5 [50,51]. A general linear model (glm) was fit at each voxel (222,513 tests) to assess two primary effects: 1) the interaction of Chinese ethnicity with APOE ε4 carrier status (including main effects of ethnicity and ε4 carrier status) in all groups combined, and 2) ε4 carrier status as predictors of brain volume in the Chinese group separately. Secondary analyses tested the ε4 carrier effect in each of the three subgroups (US Chinese, Shanghai Chinese, Whites) to clarify the main effect in whites and rule out confounding due to country of residence in Chinese. Additional covariates in all models were age at time of scan, sex, total intracranial volume (TIV), scan type, and site of data collection. Detailed models are provided in S1 Text. VBM glms were one-tailed, assuming anti-correlation (i.e., carrying APOE ε4 is associated with smaller volume). Raw VBM results were examined at Puncorrected<0.001. Correction for multiple testing was performed using 1000 permutations on whole brain to determine the study-specific distribution of maximum T-scores and cluster sizes at a voxel-wise threshold of Puncorrected<0.001; family-wise error (FWE) corrected significance was established at PFWE≤0.05.

Results

Clinical characterization

A total of 142 images were analyzed from 41 Chinese Americans, 30 Shanghai Chinese, and 71 whites, with mean ages of 62, 72 and 68 years, respectively (P<0.0001, Table 1). There were a total of 31 APOE ε4-carriers as follows: eleven Chinese Americans (N = 10 with 1 allele, N = 1 with 2 alleles), 3 Shanghai Chinese (N = 3 with 1 allele, N = 0 with 2 alleles) and 17 whites (N = 17 with 1 allele, N = 0 with 2 alleles). Full genotype breakdown is provided in S1 Table. Mean education (in years) was significantly lower in Chinese individuals (P<0.0001, Table 1). Approximately 50% of the Chinese Americans were evaluated in Chinese (Mandarin or Cantonese).

thumbnail
Table 1. Sample demographics, by ethnic group and APOE ε4 carrier status.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.t001

Primary image analysis

In the main analysis of our study, we tested whether there was a differential effect of APOE ε4 in Chinese versus white individuals. In the entire biracial cohort, the APOE ε4xChinese interaction was associated with lower volume in bilateral cuneus and left middle frontal gyrus (Puncorrected<0.001, Fig. 1, S2 Table). At a relaxed threshold of Puncorrected<0.01, there were also suggestive findings in right entorhinal cortex and left hippocampus (S1 Fig.). After correction for multiple testing, the left cuneus remained significantly associated with the interaction effect (PFWE = 0.05). The interaction effect on these regions was, on average, two-fold the magnitude of the APOE ε4 main effect and 7-fold the magnitude of the main effect of race (S3 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Interaction of APOE ε4 with Chinese ethnicity.

Results from the interaction analysis of APOE ε4xChinese are shown for all individuals. Carrying APOE ε4 and being Chinese was associated with reduced volume in the (A) left cuneus, (B) right precuneus, (C) right cuneus and (D) left middle frontal gyrus (Puncorrected<0.001). Left cuneus remained significant after correction for multiple testing (PFWE = 0.05). Regions are labeled according to the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) Atlas, with volume (in mm3) and maximum T score provided for each cluster. Left side of image corresponds to left side of brain, with Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates provided for respective slices. T-maps are shown at Puncorrected<0.001 (T range 3.15–4.32), overlaid on a template brain in MRICron. Single clusters were extracted using xjView toolbox (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview). A summary of all findings is visualized on a rendered template brain in MRICron, with labels for each region as annotated above.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.g001

To assess the main effects of APOE ε4 in Chinese, we next performed VBM in each ethnic group separately. In the Chinese cohort, APOE ε4 was associated with reduced volume in left cuneus, right precuneus, and right parahippocampal gyrus (Puncorrected<0.001, Fig. 2, S2S3 Tables). After correction for multiple testing, reduced volume in the left cuneus remained significantly associated with carrying APOE ε4 (PFWE = 0.04). In whites, APOE ε4 carriers only showed reduced volume in the cerebellum (Puncorrected<0.001; Fig. 3, in yellow; S4 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Main effect of carrying APOE ε4 on brain structure in Chinese.

Results assessing the main effect of APOE ε4 in Chinese individuals only. Carrying APOE ε4 was associated with reduced volume in the (A) left cuneus, (B) right precuneus, and (D) right parahippocampal gyrus (Puncorrected<0.001). Left cuneus remained significant after correction for multiple testing (PFWE = 0.04). Regions are labeled according to the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) Atlas, with volume (in mm3) and maximum T score provided for each cluster. Left side of image corresponds to left side of brain, with Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates provided for respective slices. T-maps are shown at Puncorrected<0.001 (T range 3.23–5.20), overlaid on a template brain in MRICron. Single clusters were extracted using xjView toolbox (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview). A summary of all findings is visualized on a rendered template brain in MRICron, with labels for each region as annotated above.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Main effect of carrying APOE ε4 on brain structure in Chinese and white subgroups.

T-maps from cohort-specific analyses are shown at Puncorrected<0.01 overlaid on a template brain in MRICron. Whites are shown in yellow (T range 2.39–4.84), Chinese Americans in blue (T range 2.44–4.91), and Shanghai Chinese are in red (T range 2.50–3.14). Left side of image corresponds to left side of brain, with Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates provided for respective slices. Both Chinese groups showed suggestive volume reductions in precuneus/cuneus in APOE ε4 carriers versus non-carriers. Chinese Americans also showed lower volume in the hippocampal formation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.g003

Secondary analysis of other possible confounds

To ensure that the APOE ε4 effect in the Shanghai participants was not markedly different than what we saw in the Chinese Americans, we performed secondary analysis on each Chinese subgroup separately. Chinese Americans and Shanghai Chinese but not whites both showed evidence of lower volume in cuneus and hippocampal formation (Puncorrected<0.01; Fig. 3, in blue and red; S4 Table) suggesting that the country a participant is living in is not a major confound.

We also assessed whether there were any baseline neuroanatomical differences between Chinese and white individuals by performing VBM on a subset of non- ε4 carriers that were matched for demographics. Our results showed differential neuroanatomic patterns consistent with speaking Chinese [52,53] or implicated in language and speech execution [5456]. Importantly, none of the regions implicated in the APOE ε4 X Chinese interaction demonstrated changes in this post hoc VBM, suggesting that our primary findings are specific to being Chinese and carrying the ε4 risk allele. Refer to S1 Text for details.

Given previous association of APOE ε4 with WMH burden in cognitively impaired individuals [41], we assessed WMH volume in a subset of individuals for which FLAIR data was available to see if WMH volume was mediating the effect of APOE ε4 on brain volume in our cognitively normal Chinese groups. A total of 64 whites (N = 15 ε4 carriers, N = 49 non-carriers), 30 Chinese Americans (N = 9 carriers, N = 21 non-carriers) and 29 Shanghai Chinese (N = 3 carriers, N = 26 non-carriers) had WMH volumes available for analysis. WMH volume was significantly higher in Shanghai Chinese compared to whites (P<0.0001) and Chinese Americans (P<0.005), but there was no significant difference between the Chinese and white cohorts collected in the U.S. (S2 Fig.). Testing the main effect of ethnicity, WMH volume was not significant after adjusting for sample site (P = 0.58, t = 0.56). APOE ε4 was not a significant predictor of WMH volume across all ethnic groups (P = 0.41, F = 0.69) or after accounting for Chinese ethnicity (P = 0.46, t = -0.75). In the interaction model, WMH volume was significantly associated with age (P = 0.008, t = 2.70, beta = 0.17±0.06) and site (P = 0.007, t = -2.73) but not with the APOE ε4xChinese interaction (P = 0.68, t = -0.42), Chinese ethnicity (P = 0.56, t = 0.59), APOE ε4 carrier status (P = 0.85, t = 0.19), sex (P = 0.62, t = 0.50), TIV (P = 0.10, t = 1.68) or scan type (P = 0.60, t = 0.53). This suggested that WMH volume is not a mediator of the APOE ε4 effect specific to Chinese.

We also conducted all of the imaging analyses with education as a covariate because the Chinese American cohort had a significantly lower level of education (Table 1). The results did not differ from those without correction for education. Thus the possibility that education is mediating this effect is very unlikely.

Finally, in order to address the possibility that the Chinese participants were misclassified as cognitively normal, we assessed longitudinal data that was available for a subset of Chinese American samples collected at the MAC. Of 41 Chinese Americans, 32 returned for the first follow-up visit. Nine (22%) were only evaluated at one visit for the following reasons: six declined further participation and three were lost to follow-up. The attrition rate in the Chinese Americans was similar to that in the white participants, which was 23.9%. Cognitive assessment scores for the Chinese Americans returning for follow-up are shown in Table 2. Cognitive scores remained stable between Time 1 (baseline) and follow-up time points (repeated measures ANOVA, two-tailed P>0.05).

thumbnail
Table 2. Chinese American cognitive and functional scores over time.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.t002

Discussion

In this study, we present results for a gene-ethnicity interaction effect on brain anatomy in cognitively normal older adults. We found an association of the APOE ε4 allele with volume loss in bilateral cuneus, with suggestive findings in parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex and hippocampal formation, which was only demonstrable in Chinese participants. The entorhinal cortex and hippocampus are among regions most associated with degeneration in AD, and have been suggested to be an early site of atrophy associated with APOE ε4 [18,42]. The cuneus has also been implicated in conversion and progression of AD [57,58]. Taken together, our results are consistent with atrophy patterns associated with AD, for which APOE ε4 is a known risk factor.

As discussed below, a gene-atrophy association for APOE ε4 has been demonstrated previously; however, to our knowledge, this is the first time that the ε4-carrier effect on neuroanatomy has been stronger in one ethnic group compared with another. This finding is consistent with several studies that have identified a differential AD risk effect of APOE ε4 across diverse populations [6,2831]. However, whether genotype-associated differences in neuroanatomy directly reflect disease risk remains unclear. For example, previous work has shown alterations in connectivity in the functional network most affected by AD in APOE ε4 carriers versus non-carriers; these differences were observed in the absence of differences in gray matter volume in adolescent and young adults [59,60], and in older individuals in the absence of cognitive symptoms or amyloid deposition in the brain [61]. There could be baseline effects of APOE ε4 on the brain that alter its vulnerability to disease but are modified by other factors such as cognitive reserve. The fact that Chinese APOE ε4 carriers have volume loss in AD-associated brain regions yet maintain normal cognition is intriguing and, if verified in larger studies, suggests that biological, environmental, and/or lifestyle modifiers specific to Chinese may promote cognitive protection.

Two possible biological explanations for the present findings will be discussed. One possibility is that Chinese carriers are more susceptible to disease due to preclinical changes in brain structure associated with carrying APOE ε4. This would suggest that biological and/or environmental components moderate the APOE ε4 effect in this population, resulting in greater volume loss in AD-specific brain regions, which could result in higher risk to Chinese ε4 carriers of developing dementia later in life. However, longitudinal follow-up in our cohort of Chinese Americans suggested no evidence of decline in cognitive performance in 2–3 years of follow-up.

An alternative–and perhaps more intriguing–hypothesis is that Chinese APOE ε4 carriers are resistant to the risk for dementia conferred by the ε4 allele based on the presence of modifying factors specific to this population. This would be consistent with the fact that the penetrance of ε4 is not complete; there are some individuals who have two copies of the risk allele but never develop AD [11]. It is possible that the white ε4 carriers in our cohort also had volume loss in AD regions and subsequently advanced to MCI or AD status, such that the remaining white ε4 carriers in our cognitively normal cohort will continue to maintain their cognitive status for the duration of their life. In contrast, in Chinese there may be some form of moderation or compensation that allows maintenance of cognitive ability despite presence of ε4-associated volume loss, thereby delaying or preventing onset of dementia as compared to similarly at-risk whites. For example, Chinese individuals may demonstrate the normal effect of APOE ε4 in memory-specific systems, but other brain systems could be compensating for these changes; this could take the form of enhanced brain function in particular cognitive domains, or lifestyle factors that bolster cognitive reserve. Further study to identify modifiers that protect Chinese from cognitive decline despite APOE ε4-conferred loss in brain volume could provide a unique opportunity to identify factors that promote cognitive resilience against AD.

There are some caveats to the present study. In addition to a limited sample size, many of the Chinese American participants were evaluated with the Chinese language version of our neuropsychiatric assessment, which is not directly comparable to the English language evaluation administered to English-speaking Chinese Americans. Thus it is possible that Chinese-speaking individuals may have subtle impairments in cognitive performance that would have been detected by our English testing battery. However, the proportion of English tests administered to the Chinese American APOE ε4 carriers versus non-carriers (73% and 40%, respectively) is not significantly different, so any discrepancies related to testing language are likely balanced between the two genotype groups. Another potential confound is associated with the ages in the study groups. It is possible that the white cohort—with a mean age of 68 years of age—has simply had more time to convert to cognitive impairment as compared to the slightly younger (mean age 62) Chinese American subgroup. The Shanghai Chinese subgroup was older than both American groups (mean age 72); the low prevalence of APOE ε4 carriers in this group may be indicative that those who were going to convert to MCI/AD have already done so, though longitudinal data detailing this was not available for the present study. Age was included in the analysis as a covariate, so our findings should not be mediated by age, though better matching of groups would be ideal. In addition, there was no significant difference in age between APOE ε4 carriers versus non-carriers (Chinese American: P = 0.07; Shanghai Chinese: P = 0.19; white American: P = 0.10), further suggesting that the genotype effects we observed are not age-dependent. Finally, the present study was not powered to test whether the APOE ε4 effect we have observed applies equally to Chinese living in America and in China. Future studies including a larger number of ε4 carriers will be required to address this open question.

We assessed WMH volume as a potential confounder for the APOE ε4xChinese interaction but found no association of WMH volume with genotype or ethnicity after controlling for site of sample ascertainment. The Shanghai Chinese subgroup demonstrated significantly higher WMH volume than both American groups, even after controlling for age, consistent with previous findings [41]. Given the consistency of structural findings in both Chinese subgroups despite differences in WMH burden, it appears that WMH does not mediate this genotype-ethnicity effect. A limitation of this analysis was that WMH was quantified using two different methods, which may not be directly comparable. Future studies measuring all individuals with the same neuroimaging and processing protocol will allow for more thorough investigations of neuroanatomical mediators of APOE ε4 effects.

Another question arises regarding why we did not find any effect of APOE ε4 in our white cohort. Previous studies of the preclinical effect of ε4 on neuroanatomy have been inconsistent (reviewed in [20]); although many groups have found anatomical differences in cognitively normal youth and adults based on ε4 carrier status [18,42], others have not [17,25,62]. These negative findings include two studies of N = 164 [17] and N = 198 [62] predominantly white healthy controls from the well-characterized Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (for more information see www.adni-info.org/). Factors that might influence the ability to detect this effect in normal elderly include the approach used to classify individuals as cognitively normal, as well as other co-morbid genetic and lifestyle factors that may influence the characteristics of the cohort.

In summary, we have demonstrated an APOE ε4 relationship to brain anatomy in cognitively normal older adults with a stronger effect in Chinese individuals. A critical next step will be to evaluate an independent cohort of Chinese and whites for replication of this finding. This will require expanding recruitment efforts to collect more samples and imaging data from diverse populations as well as expanding international collaborations, along with development of uniform approaches to clinical assessment. Longitudinal assessment will be key to evaluate the implications of any differences identified across ethnicities. This work has great potential for furthering our understanding of the factors contributing to risk for dementia across races and cultures.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Genotypes Summarized by Sub-Group.

Genotypes for individuals are summarized for each possible combination APOE ε allele type.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Results of Primary Analyses by Voxel-Based Morphometry.

Results for the two primary analyses by voxel-based morphometry are displayed above. Regions are labeled according to their placement within the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas of the human brain. For each finding, the volume of the cluster and coordinates of the voxel within the cluster with the highest T-score are provided as X, Y, and Z values in the MNI152 coordinate system. Finally, the maximum T-score within each cluster, unadjusted P-value, and corrected P-value are provided. L—Left. R—Right.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Regression Results for Regions of Interest Found in Primary Analyses.

After discovering seven regions of interest (ROI) by voxel-based morphometry, we confirmed our results by linear regression analyses. A regression was performed for each ROI. For the four ROIs found in the Chinese X APOE ε 4 interaction analysis, we used age, sex, total intracranial volume (TIV), scan type, race, APOE ε 4 carrier status, and APOE ε 4 X Chinese status as independent variables. For the three ROIs found in the all Chinese APOE ε 4 main effect analysis, we used age, sex, TIV, scan type, and APOE ε 4 carrier status as independent variables. The regression coefficient (Coef., ß), standard error (Std. Err.), and accompanying P-value are presented for each independent variable as predictors of ROI volumes. All tests were two-tailed. L—Left. R—Right.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Results of Secondary Analyses by Voxel-Based Morphometry.

Results for the three secondary analyses by voxel-based morphometry are displayed above. Regions are labeled according to their placement within the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas of the human brain. For each finding, the volume of the cluster and coordinates of the voxel within the cluster with the highest T-score are provided as X, Y, and Z values in the MNI152 coordinate system. Finally, the maximum T-score within each cluster, unadjusted P-value, and corrected P-value are provided. L—Left. R—Right.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.s004

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Interaction of APOE ε4 with Chinese ethnicity.

Results from the interaction analysis of APOE ε4xChinese are shown for all individuals. Left side of image corresponds to left side of brain, with Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates provided for respective slices. T-maps are shown at the stated P-value thresholds, overlaid on a template brain in MRICron. Suggestive signal in hippocampal formation, with T-map shown at Puncorrected<0.01 (T range 2.35–4.32).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.s005

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. White matter hyperintensity volumes.

Mean±SE white matter hyperintensity volumes (in mm3) are shown for each subgroup, with whites in yellow, Chinese Americans in blue and Shanghai Chinese in red. **P<0.005, ***P<0.0001 via 2-tailed Tukey-Kramer post hoc pair wise comparisons.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.s006

(DOCX)

S1 Text. This file contains supporting methodological descriptions of cohort inclusion/exclusion criteria, clinical evaluation, FLAIR image acquisition, white matter hyperintensity quantification, and statistical analysis.

It also contains a description of the secondary analysis of ethnicity as a confound.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118338.s007

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all research participants without which this study would not be possible.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JSY BLM HJR. Performed the experiments: JSY AKLL LTT EB LWB SZC MT JH BRM AK GC CSD. Analyzed the data: JSY AKLL LTT EB LWB JH CGS OTC. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: EB SZC MT JH CGS OTC AK MWW GC CSD BLM. Wrote the paper: JSY AKLL LTT EB LWB SZC MT JH CGS OTC BRM AK MWW GC CSD BLM HJR.

References

  1. 1. Jacobson DR, Pastore RD, Yaghoubian R, Kane I, Gallo G, et al. (1997) Variant-sequence transthyretin (isoleucine 122) in late-onset cardiac amyloidosis in black Americans. N Engl J Med 336: 466–473. pmid:9017939
  2. 2. Paez J, Janne P, Lee JE, Tracy S (2004) EGFR mutations in lung cancer: correlation with clinical response to gefitinib therapy. Science (80-) 304: 1497–1500. pmid:15118125
  3. 3. Burchard EG, Avila PC, Nazario S, Casal J, Torres A, et al. (2004) Lower bronchodilator responsiveness in Puerto Rican than in Mexican subjects with asthma. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 169: 386–392. pmid:14617512
  4. 4. Choudhry S, Ung N, Avila PC, Ziv E, Nazario S, et al. (2005) Pharmacogenetic differences in response to albuterol between Puerto Ricans and Mexicans with asthma. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 171: 563–570. pmid:15557128
  5. 5. Salari K, Choudhry S, Tang H, Naqvi M, Lind D, et al. (2005) Genetic admixture and asthma-related phenotypes in Mexican American and Puerto Rican asthmatics. Genet Epidemiol 29: 76–86. pmid:15918156
  6. 6. Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, Hyman B, Kukull WA, et al. (1997) Effects of Age, Sex, and Ethnicity on the Association Between Apolipoprotein E Genotype and Alzheimer Disease: A Meta-analysis. JAMA J Am Med Assoc 278: 1349–1356.
  7. 7. Hong CJ, Liu TY, Liu HC, Wang SJ, Fuh JL, et al. (1996) Epsilon 4 allele of apolipoprotein E increases risk of Alzheimer’s disease in a Chinese population. Neurology 46: 1749–1751. pmid:8649585
  8. 8. Seshadri S, Fitzpatrick AL, Ikram MA, DeStefano AL, Gudnason V, et al. (2010) Genome-wide analysis of genetic loci associated with Alzheimer disease. JAMA 303: 1832–1840. pmid:20460622
  9. 9. Strittmatter WJ, Saunders AM, Schmechel D, Pericak-Vance M, Enghild J, et al. (1993) Apolipoprotein E: high-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 90: 1977–1981. pmid:8446617
  10. 10. Poirier J, Davignon J, Bouthillier D, Kogan S, Bertrand P, et al. (1993) Apolipoprotein E polymorphism and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 342: 697–699. pmid:8103819
  11. 11. Verghese PB, Castellano JM, Holtzman DM (2011) Apolipoprotein E in Alzheimer’s disease and other neurological disorders. Lancet Neurol 10: 241–252. pmid:21349439
  12. 12. Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, et al. (1993) Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science 261: 921–923. pmid:8346443
  13. 13. Yoshizawa T, Yamakawa-Kobayashi K, Komatsuzaki Y, Arinami T, Oguni E, et al. (1994) Dose-dependent association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset, sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 36: 656–659. pmid:7944299
  14. 14. Wisdom NM, Callahan JL, Hawkins KA (2011) The effects of apolipoprotein E on non-impaired cognitive functioning: A meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging 32: 63–74. pmid:19285755
  15. 15. Deary IJ, Whiteman MC, Pattie A, Starr JM, Hayward C, et al. (2002) Cognitive change and the APOE epsilon 4 allele. Nature 418: 932. pmid:12198535
  16. 16. Caselli RJ, Dueck AC, Osborne D, Sabbagh MN, Connor DJ, et al. (2009) Longitudinal modeling of age-related memory decline and the APOE epsilon4 effect. N Engl J Med 361: 255–263. pmid:19605830
  17. 17. Fan M, Liu B, Zhou Y, Zhen X, Xu C, et al. (2010) Cortical thickness is associated with different apolipoprotein E genotypes in healthy elderly adults. Neurosci Lett 479: 332–336. pmid:20573574
  18. 18. Shaw P, Lerch JP, Pruessner JC, Taylor KN, Rose AB, et al. (2007) Cortical morphology in children and adolescents with different apolipoprotein E gene polymorphisms: an observational study. Lancet Neurol 6: 494–500. pmid:17509484
  19. 19. Wishart HA, Saykin AJ, McAllister TW, Rabin LA, McDonald BC, et al. (2006) Regional brain atrophy in cognitively intact adults with a single APOE epsilon4 allele. Neurology 67: 1221–1224. pmid:17030756
  20. 20. Scarmeas N, Stern Y (2006) Imaging studies and APOE genotype in persons at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. CurrPsychiatry Rep 8: 11–17.
  21. 21. Filbey FM, Slack KJ, Sunderland TP, Cohen RM (2006) Functional magnetic resonance imaging and magnetoencephalography differences associated with APOE&epsiv;4 in young healthy adults. Neuroreport 17: 1585–1590. pmid:17001273
  22. 22. Mondadori CRA, De Quervain DJF, Buchmann A, Mustovic H, Wollmer MA, et al. (2007) Better memory and neural efficiency in young apolipoprotein E ε4 carriers. Cereb Cortex 17: 1934–1947 pmid:17077159
  23. 23. Reiman EM, Reiman EM, Chen K, Chen K, Alexander GE, et al. (2004) Functional brain abnormalities in young adults at genetic risk for late-onset Alzheimer’s dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101: 284–289. pmid:14688411
  24. 24. Caselli RJ, Chen K, Lee W, Alexander GE, Reiman EM (2008) Correlating cerebral hypometabolism with future memory decline in subsequent converters to amnestic pre-mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 65: 1231–1236. pmid:18779428
  25. 25. Filippini N, Zarei M, Beckmann CF, Galluzzi S, Borsci G, et al. (2009) Regional atrophy of transcallosal prefrontal connections in cognitively normal APOE epsilon4 carriers. J Magn Reson Imaging 29: 1021–1026. pmid:19388128
  26. 26. Ghebremedhin E, Schultz C, Braak E, Braak H (1998) High frequency of apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele in young individuals with very mild Alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Exp Neurol 153: 152–155. pmid:9743577
  27. 27. Corbo RM, Scacchi R (1999) Apolipoprotein E (APOE) allele distribution in the world. Is APOE*4 a “thrifty” allele? Ann Hum Genet 63: 301–310. pmid:10738542
  28. 28. Crean S, Ward A, Mercaldi CJ, Collins JM, Cook MN, et al. (2011) Apolipoprotein E ε4 prevalence in Alzheimer’s disease patients varies across global populations: a systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 31: 20–30. pmid:21124030
  29. 29. Kwon OD, Khaleeq A, Chan W, Pavlik VN, Doody RS (2010) Apolipoprotein E polymorphism and age at onset of Alzheimer’s disease in a quadriethnic sample. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 30: 486–491. pmid:21252542
  30. 30. Venketasubramanian N, Sahadevan S, Kua EH, Chen CPL, Ng TP (2011) Interethnic differences in dementia epidemiology: Global and Asia-Pacific perspectives. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 30: 492–498. pmid:21252543
  31. 31. Tang MX, Maestre G, Tsai WY, Liu XH, Feng L, et al. (1996) Relative risk of Alzheimer disease and age-at-onset distributions, based on APOE genotypes among elderly African Americans, Caucasians, and Hispanics in New York City. Am J Hum Genet 58: 574–584. pmid:8644717
  32. 32. United States Census Bureau (n.d.) Commerce USDo. Available: http://factfinder2.census.gov.
  33. 33. Chao SZ, Lai NB, Tse MM, Ho RJ, Kong JP, et al. (2011) Recruitment of Chinese American elders into dementia research: the UCSF ADRC experience. Gerontologist 51 Suppl 1: S125–S133. pmid:21565814
  34. 34. Hou CE, Yaffe K, Pérez-Stable EJ, Miller BL (2006) Frequency of dementia etiologies in four ethnic groups. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 22: 42–47. pmid:16682792
  35. 35. Wang PN, Lirng J, Lin K, Chang F, Liu H (2006) Prediction of Alzheimer’s disease in mild cognitive impairment: A prospective study in Taiwan. Neurobiol Aging 27: 1797–1806. pmid:16321457
  36. 36. Lung F-W, Yen Y-C, Chou L-J, Hong C-J, Wu C-K (2005) The allele interaction between apolipoprotein epsilon2 and epsilon4 in Taiwanese Alzheimer’s disease patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand 111: 38–43. pmid:15636592
  37. 37. Chuang WL, Hsieh YC, Wang CY, Kuo HC, Huang CC (2010) Association of apolipoproteins e4 and c1 with onset age and memory: a study of sporadic Alzheimer disease in Taiwan. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 23: 42–48. pmid:20145290
  38. 38. Wang PN, Hong C, Lin KN, Liu HC, Chen WT (2011) APOE ε4 increases the risk of progression from amnestic mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease among ethnic Chinese in Taiwan. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 82: 165–169. pmid:20660919
  39. 39. Zhao S, Guo C, Wang M, Chen W, Wu Y, et al. (2011) A clinical memory battery for screening for amnestic mild cognitive impairment in an elderly chinese population. J Clin Neurosci 18: 774–779. pmid:21435882
  40. 40. Ma F, Wang J, Miao R, Zhao W, Wang Q (2011) Association between apolipoprotein E ε4 and longitudinal cognitive decline: Nested case-control study among chinese community-dwelling elders. Neuropsychobiology 64: 102–109. pmid:21701228
  41. 41. He J, Iosif A-M, Lee DY, Martinez O, Chu S, et al. (2010) Brain structure and cerebrovascular risk in cognitively impaired patients: Shanghai Community Brain Health Initiative-pilot phase. Arch Neurol 67: 1231–1237. pmid:20937951
  42. 42. Dean DC, Jerskey B a, Chen K, Protas H, Thiyyagura P, et al. (2014) Brain differences in infants at differential genetic risk for late-onset Alzheimer disease: a cross-sectional imaging study. JAMA Neurol 71: 11–22. pmid:24276092
  43. 43. Baldereschi M, Amato MP, Nencini P, Pracucci G, Lippi A, et al. (1994) Cross-national interrater agreement on the clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia. WHO-PRA Age-Associated Dementia Working Group, WHO-Program for Research on Aging, Health of Elderly Program. Neurology 44: 239–242. pmid:8309565
  44. 44. Prince M, Acosta D, Chiu H, Scazufca M, Varghese M (2003) Dementia diagnosis in developing countries: A cross-cultural validation study. Lancet 361: 909–917. pmid:12648969
  45. 45. Rosen HJ, Alcantar O, Rothlind J, Sturm V, Kramer JH, et al. (2010) Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive self-appraisal in neurodegenerative disease. Neuroimage 49: 3358–3364. pmid:19961939
  46. 46. Kramer JH, Jurik J, Sha SJ, Rankin KP, Rosen HJ, et al. (2003) Distinctive neuropsychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol 16: 211–218. pmid:14665820
  47. 47. Morris JC (1993) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43: 2412–2414. pmid:8232972
  48. 48. Sturm VE, Yokoyama JS, Seeley WW, Kramer JH, Miller BL, et al. (2013) Heightened emotional contagion in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease is associated with temporal lobe degeneration. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110: 9944–9949. pmid:23716653
  49. 49. Schwarz C, Fletcher E, Decarli C, Carmichael O (2009) Fully-automated white matter hyperintensity detection with anatomical prior knowledge and without FLAIR. Lecture Notes in Computer Science (including subseries Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence and Lecture Notes in Bioinformatics). Vol. 5636 LNCS. pp. 239–251.
  50. 50. Bates E, Wilson SM, Saygin AP, Dick F, Sereno MI, et al. (2003) Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nat Neurosci 6: 448–450. pmid:12704393
  51. 51. Wilson SM, Brambati SM, Henry RG, Handwerker DA, Agosta F, et al. (2009) The neural basis of surface dyslexia in semantic dementia. Brain 132: 71–86. pmid:19022856
  52. 52. Kochunov P, Fox P, Lancaster J, Tan LH, Amunts K, et al. (2003) Localized morphological brain differences between English-speaking Caucasians and Chinese-speaking Asians: new evidence of anatomical plasticity. Neuroreport 14: 961–964. pmid:12802183
  53. 53. Crinion JT, Green DW, Chung R, Ali N, Grogan A, et al. (2009) Neuroanatomical markers of speaking Chinese. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 4108–4115. pmid:19530216
  54. 54. Chesson AL (1983) Aphasia following a right thalamic hemorrhage. Brain Lang 19: 306–316. pmid:6192868
  55. 55. Murdoch BE, Chenery HJ, Wilks V, Boyle RS (1987) Language disorders in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain Lang 31: 122–137. pmid:2437993
  56. 56. Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Cox RW, Rao SM, et al. (1997) Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 17: 353–362. pmid:8987760
  57. 57. Kinkingnéhun S, Sarazin M, Lehéricy S, Guichart-Gomez E, Hergueta T, et al. (2008) VBM anticipates the rate of progression of Alzheimer disease: A 3-year longitudinal study. Neurology 70: 2201–2211. pmid:18448872
  58. 58. Caroli A, Testa C, Geroldi C, Nobili F, Barnden LR, et al. (2007) Cerebral perfusion correlates of conversion to Alzheimer’s disease in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Neurol 254: 1698–1707. pmid:17990057
  59. 59. Filippini N, MacIntosh BJ, Hough MG, Goodwin GM, Frisoni GB, et al. (2009) Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 7209–7214. pmid:19357304
  60. 60. Xu G, McLaren DG, Ries ML, Fitzgerald ME, Bendlin BB, et al. (2009) The influence of parental history of Alzheimer’s disease and apolipoprotein E epsilon4 on the BOLD signal during recognition memory. Brain 132: 383–391. pmid:18829694
  61. 61. Sheline YI, Morris JC, Snyder AZ, Price JL, Yan Z, et al. (2010) APOE4 allele disrupts resting state fMRI connectivity in the absence of amyloid plaques or decreased CSF Aβ42. J Neurosci 30: 17035–17040. pmid:21159973
  62. 62. Hostage CA, Roy Choudhury K, Doraiswamy PM, Petrella JR (2013) Dissecting the Gene Dose-Effects of the APOE ε4 and ε2 Alleles on Hippocampal Volumes in Aging and Alzheimer’s Disease. PLoS One 8.