Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Women Favour Dyadic Relationships, but Men Prefer Clubs: Cross-Cultural Evidence from Social Networking

  • Tamas David-Barrett ,

    tamas.david-barrett@psy.ox.ac.uk

    Affiliation Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Rd, Oxford, OX1 3UD, United Kingdom

  • Anna Rotkirch,

    Affiliation Population Research Institute, Väestöliitto, Kalevankatu 16, 00101, Helsinki, Finland

  • James Carney,

    Affiliation Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Rd, Oxford, OX1 3UD, United Kingdom

  • Isabel Behncke Izquierdo,

    Affiliation Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Rd, Oxford, OX1 3UD, United Kingdom

  • Jaimie A. Krems,

    Affiliation Department of Social Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, 85287, United States of America

  • Dylan Townley,

    Affiliation Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Rd, Oxford, OX1 3UD, United Kingdom

  • Elinor McDaniell,

    Affiliation Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Rd, Oxford, OX1 3UD, United Kingdom

  • Anna Byrne-Smith,

    Affiliation Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Rd, Oxford, OX1 3UD, United Kingdom

  • Robin I. M. Dunbar

    Affiliation Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Rd, Oxford, OX1 3UD, United Kingdom

Abstract

The ability to create lasting, trust-based friendships makes it possible for humans to form large and coherent groups. The recent literature on the evolution of sociality and on the network dynamics of human societies suggests that large human groups have a layered structure generated by emotionally supported social relationships. There are also gender differences in adult social style which may involve different trade-offs between the quantity and quality of friendships. Although many have suggested that females tend to focus on intimate relations with a few other females, while males build larger, more hierarchical coalitions, the existence of such gender differences is disputed and data from adults is scarce. Here, we present cross-cultural evidence for gender differences in the preference for close friendships. We use a sample of ∼112,000 profile pictures from nine world regions posted on a popular social networking site to show that, in self-selected displays of social relationships, women favour dyadic relations, whereas men favour larger, all-male cliques. These apparently different solutions to quality-quantity trade-offs suggest a universal and fundamental difference in the function of close friendships for the two sexes.

Introduction

The recent literature on both the evolution of sociality and the network dynamics of human and animal societies [18] suggests that large social groups cannot be fully connected: they have a layered structure that is generated by emotionally supported social relationships [915]. Although there are structural aspects to social organisation, individual behaviour is crucially shaped by dyadic relationships. It may be that preferred patterns of relationships vary by gender in a way that typically reflects sex differences in reproductive strategies [1619].

For instance, friendships or close and prolonged affiliation with non-kin are characterised by homophily, so that people typically choose friends of the same age and gender (for recent reviews see [2022]). Sex differences in reproductive strategies also shape adult social behaviour [20,2326] and may reflect different trade-offs between the quantity and quality of friendships. Thus, it has been suggested that females invest more heavily in a few, high-quality and time-consuming friendships, while males prefer groups with less investment per member, and higher group cohesion [2730].

There is reason to believe these gender differences have evolutionary roots. First, sex differences in friendship emerge early [31], are quite apparent already in small children and appear to increase with age [21,32,33]. Second, similar gender patterns exist in some non-human primates [13,34]. One obvious explanation is that male peer sociality evolved to enable hunting, coalitionary support for within-group dominance, and/or defence in larger groups, while women’s peer sociality is to a greater extent shaped by their higher investment in reproduction and child rearing, as well as their historically more frequent experience of out-migration and the need to integrate into a patrilocal society with few, if any, kin [18,20,25]. Patrilocality is also the norm for chimpanzees and bonobos, lending further support to female transfer being the norm throughout hominin evolution [35,36].

However, the scope of human gender differences is disputed. Several studies found few sex differences in the number of close non-related friends that an individual turns to for help and assistance [4,3742] while others detect crucial differences in the quantity and intensity of male and female peer ties [16,23,24,30,32,4345]. Sex differences also depend on which component of friendship is being studied, as well as the age and culture of the subjects. For instance, the review by Rose and Rudolph [21] found that girls have a greater preference for extended dyadic interactions and prosocial behaviour, while boys interact more in peer groups with a high network density and clear dominance hierarchy. But gender differences are negligible concerning the expectations males and females have of friends and in the symmetrical reciprocity they expect from them [22]. In any case, documented sex differences tend to be small or medium-size rather than large [22,46].

All reviews stress the need for more friendship research on adults and on people from other than ‘WEIRD’ [47] societies [21,22,46]. Most research on friendships has involved children or teenagers, and there is to date only limited and mixed evidence for gender differences in adult human friendships [20,38,4850]. Here, we use data from a social media site to explore gender differences in close peer relations. We hypothesised that social relations among same-aged adults would exhibit gender homophily and would vary by gender, such that men would exhibit higher numbers of friends compared to women.

Methods and Data

To investigate close friendships in the two sexes, we used Facebook Profile Pictures following the example of recent literature that deals with social networking data [5154]. Facebook is the most popular global social networking site, the primary function of which is to fulfil psychosocial needs for belonging and self-presentation [55]. Upon signing up, each user may choose a Profile Picture, which represents the user to the rest of the Facebook community, is public, and appears at the top of the user’s profile and as the icon next to the user’s name wherever he or she posts on the site. Each user can have only one Profile Picture at a time. Usually the picture features the user only. When the picture displays peers they tend to be the user with friends or acquaintances [51]. The choice of profile pictures is related to the user’s desired impression formation [55]. This impression is not, however, too detached from the real world: Facebook user profiles have been found to reflect actual rather than idealised identities [56]. We thus assume that Profile photographs of peers are likely to align with behavioural inclinations, and thus provide a reliable proxy for relationship preferences.

Data collection

We used random search terms to select 309 users (seeds) of Facebook who shared their batch of friends publicly. (The 309 users had on average 362 friends.) In the first wave of data collection, we located the Profile Pictures of all the friends of each of these users (111,863 Profile Pictures in total); each photograph was categorised with respect to the type of the picture, and the number and gender of the persons displayed (see Table 1). Data collection took place between July 2011 and January 2012.

Additional information was collected in two further waves. In the first of these (wave 2), geographical data based on geographical location was collected to control for cultural differences. The coders were provided with a list of geographic regions: Europe (56 seeds), Central and South Asia (16), Latin America (9), Middle East and North Africa (14), North America (14), South East Asia (43), Sub-Saharan Africa (7), Australia (9), East Asia (19), or ‘can’t tell region’ (21). In the second (wave 3), we used a finer classification of the number of people shown in Profile Pictures: the first two waves classified all pictures with four or more individuals as a single category, but in wave 3 this was extended to specify individual numbers up to 20.

The coding was done by 8 coders, all but two of whom were research assistants at the University of Oxford. All coders bar one (the lead author) were blind during the coding phase, knowing only that the project was to study gender differences on social networking sites. Coders were instructed to avoid any discussion of the project amongst themselves during the coding phase. All results presented in this paper are robust to the elimination of each of the coders, each of the regions, and the type of Profile Pictures displayed by the seeds.

We used only publicly available pictures. No pictures or other information associated with this research was either separately downloaded or stored. The research project was approved by the Central University Research Ethics Committee of University of Oxford, and each coder received a full research ethics briefing before joining the coding team.

Data exclusion

As adult interpersonal processes were our focus here, we excluded all Profile Pictures that contained a non-human figure, a baby or child, people of markedly different ages (as judged by the coders), pictorial collages (i.e. pictures composed of multiple, distinct or coloured photos), a person or people whose gender(s) were unidentifiable or that did not contain a person. 81,246 Profile Pictures remained, of which 19,984 (26%) contained more than one person. For an overview of the data see Fig. 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. The ratio of men in Profile Pictures (with same age adults only) as a function the number of persons in the picture.

The value corresponding to each n adds up to 1. The size of the disks denotes the share in the ratio at that particular bin. Crossing points on the grid are the only possible points given the discrete nature of the data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118329.g001

Results

Gender homophily

First, we studied gender homophily in profile pictures. As expected, if the Profile Pictures displayed three or more persons (7.6% of Profile Pictures displaying adults), they tended to be the same gender, confirming our gender homophily hypothesis (Fig. 1).

The gender ratio on Profile Pictures persons exhibited a tri-modal distribution (see Fig. 2). In Profile Pictures with 5–12 persons, the frequencies for women-only, men-only, and equal gender ratios are 15.5%, 40.6%, 15.7%. (Note that for n<5 the trimodality cannot exist, while for very large n the male groups dominate to such an extent that only one modality is left.) These are above the frequencies for all other combinations: pictures with both genders but unequally represented (such as one woman and two men) were significantly less common. Given that the population mean is approximately balanced, this finding suggests a strong gender homophily.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Relative frequencies of different gender ratios for groups where 5< = n< = 12.

The distribution shows a trimodal pattern with women-only, gender-equal and men-only gender ratios significantly above zero.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118329.g002

This preference for same-sex friends was further supported by the subsample that contains Profile Pictures displaying fewer than 5 people. While pictures with two persons showed a higher mixed than same gender frequency (sample average (2F+2M)/1F1M-1 = -0.123 (bootstrapping mean -0.122 and s.d. 0.044), likely indicative of pictures of romantic couples, both the 3-person pictures (sample average 2(3F+3M)/(1F2M+2F1M)-1 = 4.34 (bootstrapping mean 4.36 and s.d. 0.39) and the 4-person pictures (sample average 3(4F+4M)/(1F3M+2F2M+3F1M)-1 = 4.78 (bootstrapping mean 4.75 and s.d. 0.51) had a much higher same-gender frequency compared to mixed-gender frequency (Fig. 3A).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Bootstrap histograms.

Panel (a): the ratio of same gender Profile Pictures compared to mixed gender profile pictures (probability corrected); green, red, and blue lines correspond to n = 2, 3, and 4. Panel (b): the number of people on a Profile Picture men (red) or women (blue) appear on; straight lines: same gender with n between 2 and 4, dashed lines: mixed gender with n between 2 and 4, dotted lines: mixed gender with n between 2 and 20. Panel (c): the ratio of the frequency of same-gender pictures between the genders; green: pictures with 1 person, red: 2 persons, brown: 3 persons, and blue: 4 persons. (100,000 bootstrapping repeats.)

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118329.g003

Male propensity towards displaying more people

Studying the gender composition of peer groups indicated that, when Profile Pictures display a large group of people, they tend to be all male, which was in line with our expectations. In groups of three or four persons, men and women had the same propensity to appear in same-gender Profile Pictures (Table 1). However, larger groups were predominantly male, and increasingly so as the group size grows (Fig. 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. The ratio of same-gender women-only to men-only frequencies (nF/nM as function of n).

Gray lines denote a one-standard-deviation band from bootstrapping. Above n>4, men-only groups dominate, while women-only groups become extremely rare. The linear OLS coefficient of nF/nM as a function of n is negative, with R2 = 0.86, and p<0.01

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118329.g004

The male propensity to be part of larger groups was further supported by the fact that men appear together with more people than do women (Fig. 3B). For groups with 2–4 peers of the same gender, men appeared in pictures with 2.47 people on (bootstrapping mean 2.47, s.d. 0.01), as opposed to 2.35 of women (bootstrapping mean 2.35, s.d. 0.01), which are significantly different from each other (p<0.0001). For groups with 2–20 peers of any gender, men appeared with 2.90 (bootstrapping mean 2.90, s.d. 0.07), as opposed to 2.54 of women (bootstrapping mean 2.54, s.d. 0.05) which are also significantly different from each other (p<<0.0001). This suggests that independently of gender combinations and group size, men tend to appear with a larger number of people displayed in Profile Pictures.

Women focus on dyadic relationships

Finally, we detected an unexpectedly strong female focus on same gender dyads. Women not only tended to appear in Profile Pictures displaying a smaller number of people, as expected in the second research hypothesis, but there was also a strong preference towards pictures containing two women (Fig. 3C). There were 50.8% more pictures with two female peers than pictures with two male peers (bootstrapping mean 51.5, s.d. 14.8). This is especially remarkable given that same-gender pictures with 1, 3, or 4 or more people had an almost perfect gender balance: there were only 6.4% more pictures with one woman only in our dataset than with one man only (bootstrapping mean 7.2, with s.d. 14.1), only 1.1% more pictures with three women than with three men (bootstrapping mean 1.7, s.d. 10.8), and 1.0% fewer pictures with four women compared to four men (bootstrapping mean 0.5, s.d. 12.5).

Cultural variation

Although there was substantial variation across the different world regions, the main patterns of our findings were present in each of them(Fig. 5A-C), with only the magnitude of the effect varying. While a selection bias may theoretically have affected the relatively small local variation of our results, we find this unlikely: the seeds were randomly selected and at the time of the data collection 17% of the global adult population was using Facebook. Furthermore, measures for other than our three main findings reported above were less uniform among the world regions (Fig. 5D).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Cultural variation in the main finding.

Panel (a): the ratio between same-gender and mixed gender pictures of different group size (corrected by the probability of appearance, see text). Panel (b): number of close friends (same gender, with groups size 2 to 4). Panel (c): the ratio between same-gender Profile Pictures as a function of group size (1F/1M normalised to 0). Panel (d): cultural variation in the proportion of single person pictures within all Profile Pictures in a given global region. (Region codes of Panels a-c: green: Central and South Asia, blue: Europe, dashed blue: Latin America, dashed green: Middle East and North Africa, dotted blue: North America, dotted green: South-East Asia, red: Sub-Saharan Africa, black: Australia, dashed black: East Asia.)

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118329.g005

Follow-Up Studies

It is possible that the relative prevalence of female-female Profile Pictures in comparison to male-male ones may not reflect friendship behaviour but either (a) a female preference for putting up pictures of two people of any gender, or (b) a reluctance among men to display pictures with two male friends, especially in regions where homophobia is common. We tested both of these alternative explanations.

First, we tested whether there is a gender difference in the preference for Profile Pictures containing two people only. We randomly selected 960 new profiles on Facebook with two same-aged individuals on them. Out of the 960, we were unable to determine the gender of the account user in 11 cases. Out of the remaining 949 cases, 493 pictures belonged to a man and 456 belonged to a woman. Given the fact that the overall Facebook participation gender ratio is almost balanced, this result suggests that the probability of women having a strong preference to put up pictures of two people of any gender is negligible.

Second, we tested if homophobia in the country where the seed owner of the Facebook account lived would affect the ratio between female and male same-gender two-person pictures. (To correct for the fact that different countries have different gender ratios among Facebook users, we used the (2F/1F)/(2M/1M)-1 measure.) As a measure of homophobia, we used the country level geographical codes of our data, and the corresponding homophobia index for these countries as calculated by Pew Global [57]. As the homophobia index of Pew Global covers only the largest 52 countries, we could not test this hypothesis on our entire database. For the remaining seeds, countries were coded to be low on homophobia if they scored between 0 and 40 for the question “Homosexuality is a way of life that should be accepted by society” and to be high homophobic countries if they scored between 60 and 100. This gave us 66 seeds living in countries with low homophobia and 47 seeds in countries with high homophobia. The (2F/1F)/(2M/1M)-1 mean for the two groups were 0.63 and 0.52, respectively (Fig. 6). As the difference is not statistically significant (p = 0.12), and is in the opposite direction to that predicted by the hypothesis, we can conclude that male homophobia is not associated with the difference between 2F vs 2M frequencies in our dataset.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Homophobia is not related to the frequency of 2F Profile Pictures, bootstrapping distributions.

Blue line: highly homophobic countries; red line: countries with low homophobia (see text for definitions). Dashed black line: the (2F/1F)/(2M/1M)-1 mean of the entire database. (100,000 bootstrapping repeats.)

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118329.g006

Discussion

Human societies are complex, large-scale communities of multi-generational social networks. At base, however, these networks are built of the small-scale personal networks of individuals. Our data shed light on the dynamics of these personal networks by providing strong cross-cultural evidence for the universality of a male propensity to prefer a higher number friendships compared to women. While large women-only groups were almost non-existent in self-selected Profile Pictures, males were more likely to present themselves as part of large all-male groups—arguably an essential element of male-male coalitional competition. Our results are broadly in line with many previous studies of human friendship [27,30,58,59] which also found strong gender homophily and male preference for coalitions. This difference in the preferred number of friends may signal different solutions to the quantity-quality trade-off in social ties. The emotional quality of a relationship is a positive function of the time invested in it [39,60], and the closer and more time-demanding a relationship one has, the less time can be devoted to others [11,24]. At the same time, the amount of social capital available that individuals have to distribute among the members of their personal social networks is limited [61,62]. It thus appears as if women build a ‘dense’ network, while men make alliances based on ‘loose’ networks. We also found gender similarity in the preferences for three and four friends, which may explain part of the inconclusive results in previous studies on sex differences in friendship numbers.

The male propensity to form coalitions could have emerged from the sexual division of labour in ancestral environments. It was a male responsibility to defend the group against attack from outsiders, and to do so successfully it was necessary that men band together [20,24]. In males but not females, then, out-group defence called for coalitional cooperation and behaviour.

Our finding that women prefer to picture themselves with fewer friends, and thus appear more often to focus their social capital on only one person at a time, suggests a strong female preference for dyadic relations. The social benefits of such a female dyadic social style are harder to pin down, but three alternative hypotheses might be suggested. First, females may have developed a propensity to form dyadic same-sex friendships as a response to the challenges of their social environments. Given the likelihood of ancestral patrilocality [35,36,63,64], adolescent females would often have entered communities where they had few or no close kin. For females especially, the presence of kin is fitness-enhancing, as has been repeatedly shown for both anthropoid primates [65,66] and humans [67,68]. Thus the formation of emotionally intense, exclusive and “sisterly” dyadic bonds may have been a means to essentially replace kin [25] and to defend against male and inter-female aggression in the new community where she did not have female kin [25,69,70]. This mirrors the case of patrilocal bonobos (Pan paniscus), where females enter foreign communities in adolescence and integrate into their new group through intense bond formation with another (typically older) female [71]. A second explanation posits that, since females are the driving agents in human pair-bond formation, it may be a female-specific sexual strategy to form exclusive dyadic relationships. In this framework, the high frequency of female-female dyads in women’s lives might be a by-product of a preference for pairbonding [12]. A third explanation focuses on females’ unique capacity for intense empathic relationships, derived from the mother-infant bond. In this model, heightened female empathy creates an emphasis on individual relationships as a consequence of the psychological toolbox of mothering [72,73]. In comparison, males generally neither have nor require this capacity, and hence they form less emotionally close bonds, those of friendship included.

These three explanations for gender differences in social style—patrilocality, pair-bonding and maternal empathy—are difficult to tease apart. Not only is the evolutionary origin of all primate bonding likely to have arisen out of the mother-infant relationship [74], whatever forces shaped female friendship thereafter, different ultimate causes (e.g., defence against aggression in a patrilocal society, or assistance among maternal kin) may have used similar proximate mechanisms (e.g., high reliance on intimate disclosure) making it hard to dissociate them. Furthermore, recent studies of close friendship as a function of age suggests that women switch their primary focus from female-female friendships to pairbonding and then to mother-daughter bonding at different stages of the life cycle [75].

The importance for a female of maintaining close relationships once she has left her natal group sheds light on the strategies that women use during intrasexual aggression (notably exclusion and relationship-ending gossip [29,69]). If a female’s bonds to friends and her spouse are crucial for accessing resources—from food to information—then breaking these bonds and/or excluding the female all together can radically affect that individual’s fitness, to the benefit of her competitors.

There are, inevitably, some potential limitations to our data. We cannot be sure that co-appearance on Profile Pictures always reflects real-life social ties. Future research is needed in order to assess gender differences in offline sociality. However, no existing research suggests that profile pictures would include imagined or random social relations to any significant extent (not least because the other person is likely to object) and our results are in line with other recent findings from online social communities [50]. Displaying Profile Pictures with two or more people compared to only one person may also reflect some unknown personal psychological characteristics or specific life-events of account users; however, such possible characteristics should not affect our main results.

In summary, our results point to striking gender differences in intimate friendship strategies: women prefer close dyadic bonds (with evolutionary origins in either pairbonding or social insurance purposes, or both), whereas men use their bonding capacity to build multimale groups (in effect, clubs). This concurs with work on chimpanzees, where females form tight, kin-based networks and males make loose, easy-to-break alliances [76]. Since similar gendered bonds are found in our closest primate relatives, they may long predate the evolution of our species. Among cercopithecine primates, females are disproportionately more likely to invest in core female ‘friendships’ with matrilineal relatives as group size gets larger, apparently in order to maximise the effectiveness with which these relationships function as social buffers [1,12,65]. By contrast, chimpanzees [34] and humans may show a tendency to form close friendships with unrelated females in addition to those they might form with close female relatives.

Supporting Information

S1 Data. Anonymised profile picture frequency database.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118329.s001

(CSV)

Acknowledgments

We thank Elena Denaro, Joshua de Gastyne, Erin Simmons, and Cathal Power for assistance in data collection.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: TDB. Performed the experiments: TDB AR DT EM ABS. Analyzed the data: TDB. Wrote the paper: TDB AR JC IBI JK RD.

References

  1. 1. Lehmann J, Dunbar RIM (2009) Network Cohesion, Group Size and Neocortex Size in Female-bonded Old World Primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 276: 4417–4422. pmid:19793756
  2. 2. Hill RA, Dunbar RIM (2003) Social network size in humans. Human Nature-an Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 14: 53–72.
  3. 3. Krause J, Lusseau D, James R (2009) Animal social networks: an introduction. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63: 967–973.
  4. 4. Apicella CL, Marlowe FW, Fowler JH, Christakis NA (2012) Social networks and cooperation in hunter-gatherers. Nature 481: 497–501. pmid:22281599
  5. 5. Kanai R, Bahrami B, Roylance R, Rees G (2012) Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure. Proc Biol Sci 279: 1327–1334. pmid:22012980
  6. 6. David-Barrett T, Dunbar RI (2012) Cooperation, behavioural synchrony and status in social networks. J Theor Biol 308: 88–95. pmid:22609470
  7. 7. David-Barrett T, Dunbar RIM (2013) Processing power limits social group size: computational evidence for the cognitive costs of sociality. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 280.
  8. 8. David-Barrett T, Dunbar RIM (2014) Social elites can emerge naturally when interaction in networks is restricted. Behavioral Ecology 25: 58–68.
  9. 9. Zhou WX, Sornette D, Hill RA, Dunbar RIM (2005) Discrete hierarchical organization of social group sizes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 272: 439–444. pmid:15734699
  10. 10. Hamilton MJ, Milne BT, Walker RS, Burger O, Brown JH (2007) The complex structure of hunter-gatherer social networks. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 274: 2195–2202. pmid:17609186
  11. 11. Sutcliffe A, Dunbar R, Binder J, Arrow H (2012) Relationships and the social brain: integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. British Journal of Psychology.
  12. 12. Dunbar RIM (2012) Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.
  13. 13. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2006) Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus) II. Variation in the quality and stability of social bonds. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology 61: 197–204.
  14. 14. Adiseshan A, Adiseshan T, Isbell LA (2011) Affiliative relationships and reciprocity among adult male bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) at Arunachala Hill, India. Am J Primatol 73: 1107–1113. pmid:21905059
  15. 15. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2012) The Evolutionary Origins of Friendship. Annual Review of Psychology, Vol 63 63: 153–177. pmid:21740224
  16. 16. Tiger L (1974) Sex-specific friendship. In: Leyton E, editor. The Compact: Selected Dimensions of Friendship: St John's Memorial University of New Foundland. pp. 42–48.
  17. 17. Wrangham R (2000) Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In: Kappeler PM, editor. Primate males: causes and consequences of variation in group composition Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press. pp. 248–258.
  18. 18. Benenson JF, Alavi K (2004) Sex differences in children's investment in same-sex peers. Evolution and Human Behavior 25: 258–266.
  19. 19. Chapais B (2008) Primeval kinship: how pair-bonding gave birth to human society. Cambridge, Mass.; London: Harvard University Press. xv, 349 p. p.
  20. 20. Geary DC, Byrd-Craven J, Hoard MK, Vigil J, Numtee C (2003) Evolution and development of boys' social behavior. Developmental Review 23: 444–470.
  21. 21. Rose AJ, Rudolph KD (2006) A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin 132: 98–131. pmid:16435959
  22. 22. Hall JA (2011) Sex differences in friendship expectations: A meta-analysis. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 28: 723–747.
  23. 23. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S (2003) The friendship questionnaire: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33: 509–517. pmid:14594330
  24. 24. Vigil JM (2007) Asymmetries in the friendship preferences and social styles of men and women. Human Nature-an Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 18: 143–161.
  25. 25. Campbell A (2002) A mind of her own: the evolutionary psychology of women. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 393 p. p.
  26. 26. Palchykov V, Kaski K, Kertesz J, Barabasi AL, Dunbar RIM (2012) Sex differences in intimate relationships. Scientific Reports 2.
  27. 27. Fehr BA (1996) Friendship processes. Thousand Oaks, Calif; London: Sage. xv, 240 p p.
  28. 28. Guyer AE, McClure-Tone EB, Shiffrin ND, Pine DS, Nelson EE (2009) Probing the neural correlates of anticipated peer evaluation in adolescence. Child Dev 80: 1000–1015. pmid:19630890
  29. 29. Benenson JF, Markovits H, Thompson ME, Wrangham RW (2011) Under Threat of Social Exclusion, Females Exclude More Than Males. Psychological Science 22: 538–544. pmid:21403174
  30. 30. Benenson JF, Quinn A, Stella S (2012) Boys affiliate more than girls with a familiar same-sex peer. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 113: 587–593. pmid:22981686
  31. 31. Fabes RA, Hanish LD, Martin CL (2003) Children at play: The role of peers in understanding the effects of child care. Child Development 74: 1039–1043. pmid:12938698
  32. 32. Belle D (1989) Gender differences in children's social networks and supports. In: Belle D, editor. Children's social networks and social supports. Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.
  33. 33. Savin-Williams RC (1980) Friendship and social relations in children In: Foot HC, Chapman AJ, Smith JR, editors. Social interactions of adolescent females in natural groups. New York: John Wiley. pp. 343–364.
  34. 34. Langergraber K, Mitani J, Vigilant L (2009) Kinship and social bonds in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Primatol 71: 840–851. pmid:19475543
  35. 35. Foley RA, Lee PC (1989) Finite Social Space, Evolutionary Pathways, and Reconstructing Hominid Behavior. Science 243: 901–906. pmid:2493158
  36. 36. Wrangham RW (1987) The significance of African apes for reconstructing human social evolution. In: Kinzey WG, editor. Primate Models of Hominid Evolution Albany, New York: SUNY Press.
  37. 37. Bruckner E, Knaup K (1993) Womens and Mens Friendships in Comparative Perspective. European Sociological Review 9: 249–266.
  38. 38. Sheets VL, Lugar R (2005) Friendship and gender in Russia and the United States. Sex Roles 52: 131–140.
  39. 39. Roberts SGB, Dunbar RIM, Pollet TV, Kuppens T (2009) Exploring variation in active network size: Constraints and ego characteristics. Social Networks 31: 138–146.
  40. 40. Burleson BR (1997) A different voice on different cultures: Illusion and reality in the study of sex differences in personal relationships. Personal Relationships 4: 229–241.
  41. 41. Oxley NL, Dzindolet MT, Miller JL (2002) Sex differences in communication with close friends: Testing Tannen's claims. Psychological Reports 91: 537–544. pmid:12416849
  42. 42. Rotkirch A, Lyons M, David-Barrett T, Jokela M (2014) Gratitude for help among adult friends and siblings. Evol Psychol 12: 673–686. pmid:25300047
  43. 43. Caldwell MA, Peplau LA (1982) Sex-Differences in Same-Sex Friendship. Sex Roles 8: 721–732.
  44. 44. Duck S, Wright PH (1993) Reexamining Gender Differences in Same-Gender Friendships—a Close Look at 2 Kinds of Data. Sex Roles 28: 709–727.
  45. 45. Benenson JF, Heath A (2006) Boys withdraw more in one-on-one interactions, whereas girls withdraw more in groups. Developmental Psychology 42: 272–282. pmid:16569166
  46. 46. Hruschka DJ (2010) Friendship: development, ecology, and evolution of a relationship. Berkeley, Calif.; London: University of California Press. xiv, 383 p. p.
  47. 47. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A (2010) The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 61–+. pmid:20550733
  48. 48. Samter W, Whaley BB, Mortenson ST, Burleson BR (1997) Ethnicity and emotional support in same-sex friendship: A comparison of Asian-Americans, African-Americans, and Euro-Americans. Personal Relationships 4: 413–430.
  49. 49. Ueno K, Adams RG (2006) Adult friendship: A decade review. In: Noller P, Feeney JA, editors. In Close relationships (functions, forms and processes). New York and Hove: Psychology Press. pp. 151–170.
  50. 50. Durant KT, McCray AT, Safran C (2012) Identifying gender-preferred communication styles within online cancer communities: a retrospective, longitudinal analysis. PLoS One 7: e49169. pmid:23155460
  51. 51. Hum NJ, Chamberlin PE, Hambright BL, Portwood AC, Schat AC, et al. (2011) A picture is worth a thousand words: A content analysis of Facebook profile photographs. Computers in Human Behavior 27: 1828–1833.
  52. 52. Walther JB, Van der Heide B, Kim SY, Westerman D, Tong ST (2008) The role of friends' appearance and behavior on evaluations of individuals on facebook: Are we known by the company we keep? Human Communication Research 34: 28–U60.
  53. 53. Wang SS, Moon SI, Kwon KH, Evans CA, Stefanone MA (2010) Face off: Implications of visual cues on initiating friendship on Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior 26: 226–234.
  54. 54. Mendelson AL, Papacharissi Z (2010) Look at us: Collective Narcissism in College Student Facebook Photo Galleries. In: Papacharissi Z, editor. The Networked Self: Identity, Community and Culture on Social Network Sites: Routledge.
  55. 55. Nadkarni A, Hofmann SG (2012) Why do people use Facebook? Personality and Individual Differences 52: 243–249. pmid:22544987
  56. 56. Back MD, Stopfer JM, Vazire S, Gaddis S, Schmukle SC, et al. (2010) Facebook Profiles Reflect Actual Personality, Not Self-Idealization. Psychological Science 21: 372–374. pmid:20424071
  57. 57. PewGlobal (2012) Pew Research Global Attitudes Project. http://www.pewglobal.org.
  58. 58. Ip GWM, Chiu CY, Wan C (2006) Birds of a feather and birds flocking together: Physical versus behavioral cues may lead to trait- versus goal-based group perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90: 368–381. pmid:16594825
  59. 59. McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM (2001) Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology 27: 415–444.
  60. 60. Roberts SGB, Dunbar RIM (2011) The costs of family and friends: An 18-month longitudinal study of relationship maintenance and decay. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32: 186–197.
  61. 61. Saramaki J, Leicht EA, Lopez E, Roberts SG, Reed-Tsochas F, et al. (2014) Persistence of social signatures in human communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111: 942–947. pmid:24395777
  62. 62. Roberts SGB, Dunbar RIM, Pollet TV, Kuppens T (2009) Exploring variation in active network size: Constraints and ego characteristics. Social Networks 31: 138–146.
  63. 63. Fortunato L (2011) Reconstructing the History of Residence Strategies in Indo-European-Speaking Societies: Neo-, Uxori-, and Virilocality. Human Biology 83: 107–128. pmid:21453007
  64. 64. Seielstad MT, Minch E, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1998) Genetic evidence for a higher female migration rate in humans. Nature Genetics 20: 278–280. pmid:9806547
  65. 65. Silk JB (2007) Social components of fitness in primate groups. Science 317: 1347–1351. pmid:17823344
  66. 66. Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, et al. (2009) The benefits of social capital: close social bonds among female baboons enhance offspring survival. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 276: 3099–3104. pmid:19515668
  67. 67. Essock-Vitale SM, McGuire MT (1985) Women's lives viewed from an evolutionary perspective. II. Patterns of helping. Ethology and Sociobiology 6.
  68. 68. Scelza BA (2011) Female Mobility and Postmarital Kin Access in a Patrilocal Society. Human Nature-an Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 22: 377–393. pmid:22388944
  69. 69. Heim P, Murphy S, Golant SK (2003) In the company of women: indirect aggression among women: why we hurt each other and how to stop. New York: Jeremy P Tarcher/Putnam.
  70. 70. Heim P, Murphy S, Golant SK (2001) In the company of women: turning workplace conflict into powerful alliances. New York: J.P. Tarcher/Putnam. 335 p. p.
  71. 71. Furuichi T (2011) Female Contributions to the Peaceful Nature of Bonobo Society. Evolutionary Anthropology 20: 131–142. pmid:22038769
  72. 72. Fehr B (2004) Intimacy expectations in same-sex friendships: A prototype interaction-pattern model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86: 265–284. pmid:14769083
  73. 73. Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RAR, et al. (2000) Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review 107: 411–429. pmid:10941275
  74. 74. Shultz S, Opie C, Atkinson QD (2011) Stepwise evolution of stable sociality in primates. Nature 479: 219–222. pmid:22071768
  75. 75. Palchykov V, Kaski K, Kertesz J, Barabasi AL, Dunbar RI (2012) Sex differences in intimate relationships. Sci Rep 2: 370. pmid:22518274
  76. 76. de Waal FBM (1984) Sex-Differences in the Formation of Coalitions among Chimpanzees. Ethology and Sociobiology 5: 239–255.