Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Enhanced Responsiveness to Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors during Lactation

  • Nicholas J. Jury ,

    Nick.Jury@nih.gov

    Current address: Laboratory of Behavioral and Genomic Neuroscience, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, National Institutes of Health, 5625 Fishers Lane, Room 2N09, Rockville, Maryland, 20852, United States of America

    Affiliation Neuroscience Graduate Program, College of Medicine, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, 45267, United States of America

  • Betsy A. McCormick,

    Affiliation Division of Reproductive Endocrinology and Infertility, Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, 45267, United States of America

  • Nelson D. Horseman,

    Affiliations Neuroscience Graduate Program, College of Medicine, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, 45267, United States of America, Department of Molecular and Cellular Physiology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, 45267, United States of America

  • Stephen C. Benoit,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America

  • Karen A. Gregerson

    Affiliations Neuroscience Graduate Program, College of Medicine, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, 45267, United States of America, Division of Pharmaceutical Sciences, James L. Winkle College of Pharmacy, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, 45267, United States of America, Department of Molecular and Cellular Physiology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, 45267, United States of America

Abstract

The physiology of mood regulation in the postpartum is poorly understood despite the fact that postpartum depression (PPD) is a common pathology. Serotonergic mechanisms and their dysfunction are widely presumed to be involved, which has led us to investigate whether lactation induces changes in central or peripheral serotonin (5-HT) systems and related affective behaviors. Brain sections from lactating (day 10 postpartum) and age-matched nulliparous (non-pregnant) C57BL/6J mice were processed for 5-HT immunohistochemistry. The total number of 5-HT immunostained cells and optical density were measured. Lactating mice exhibited lower immunoreactive 5-HT and intensity in the dorsal raphe nucleus when compared with nulliparous controls. Serum 5-HT was quantified from lactating and nulliparous mice using radioimmunoassay. Serum 5-HT concentrations were higher in lactating mice than in nulliparous controls. Affective behavior was assessed in lactating and non-lactating females ten days postpartum, as well as in nulliparous controls using the forced swim test (FST) and marble burying task (MBT). Animals were treated for the preceding five days with a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI, citalopram, 5mg/kg/day) or vehicle. Lactating mice exhibited a lower baseline immobility time during the FST and buried fewer marbles during the MBT as compared to nulliparous controls. Citalopram treatment changed these behaviors in lactating mice with further reductions in immobility during the FST and decreased marble burying. In contrast, the same regimen of citalopram treatment had no effect on these behaviors in either non-lactating postpartum or nulliparous females. Our findings demonstrate changes in both central and peripheral 5-HT systems associated with lactation, independent of pregnancy. They also demonstrate a significant interaction of lactation and responsiveness to SSRI treatment, which has important implications in the treatment of PPD. Although recent evidence has cast doubt on the effectiveness of SSRIs, these results support their therapeutic use in the treatment of PPD.

Introduction

Mood alterations during the postpartum and postpartum depression (PPD) adversely affect not only the mother, but also disrupt bonding and the health of the child [1]. The relationship between untreated maternal depression and negative infant outcomes, even through adolescence, are well established [2,3,4]. PPD (defined in the psychiatric nomenclature as a major depressive disorder with a specifier of onset during pregnancy and/or following childbirth) affects 10–20% of women who give birth [5,6,7,8].

From a biological perspective, it is an evolutionary imperative that female mammals cope with the physiological stresses of pregnancy, child birth, and lactation without suffering the debilitations inherent with PPD. From this biological perspective, attention naturally focuses on PPD as a disorder, and several studies have suggested specific mechanisms of PPD [9;10,11].

The control of mood and the etiology of depressive disorders in particular, are not completely understood. However, substantial evidence has accrued that serotonergic systems play a central role [1,12,13,14]. Genetic variants in components of the serotonergic system have been correlated with depression [15]. Altered function of the serotonin transporter (SERT) or tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) has been found in PPD subjects [1,14,15]. Levels of serotonin (5-HT) and its major metabolite, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), are significantly lower in the cerebrospinal fluid of depressed patients and in brain tissue of suicide victims [16,17,18]. Reduced availability of the 5-HT precursor, tryptophan, has also been found in depressed patients [19]. Moreover, SSRIs are the first line of pharmacotherapy in PPD and relieve depressive symptoms in most of these patients [4,20].

Although there is evidence that SSRIs are effective in treating PPD [4,21,22], there is still much debate about the effectiveness of SSRIs in treating depressive disorders. Two independent research consortiums conducted meta-analyses on clinical trials submitted to the Food and Drug Administration and determined that the therapeutic effect of the SSRIs were relatively small when compared to placebo in severely depressed patients [23,24]. In contrast, two other independent research teams conducted meta-analyses and concluded that SSRIs were effective in treating depressive symptoms when compared to placebo regardless of the severity of the depressive symptoms [25,26].

In 2004 a novel serotonergic biosynthetic system in the mammary gland was identified and found to be highly upregulated during late pregnancy and lactation [27]. This discovery provides a new context in which to consider whether serotonergic systems are altered in the postpartum, and ultimately whether the central and peripheral serotonergic systems influence one another during this time. This study presents our initial examination of these serotonin systems in the context of the lactating animal, using a selective SSRI (citalopram) with which to probe the behavioral responsiveness of the central serotonin system. Here we investigated the biochemical changes in central (dorsal raphe nucleus) and peripheral (serum) 5-HT systems in lactating mice using immunohistochemistry and radioimmunoassay, respectively. We also examined the effect(s) of sub-chronic SSRI treatment on affective state, as measured by depression-related (forced swim test, FST) and anxiety-related behaviors (marble burying task, MBT) in the postpartum. The present studies compared these behaviors among normal lactating and non-lactating dams without experimentally induced depression. This experimental design was chosen in lieu of a model of maternal depression in order to examine the effects of lactation on the serotonergic systems and affective behavior during a period of peak lactation, day 10 postpartum [27,28,29]. Herein the data demonstrate an enhanced responsiveness to SSRI treatment in lactating dams compared with non-lactating females. We also report changes in both central and peripheral 5-HT systems during lactation. The culmination of these results have clinical implications in the treatment of PPD.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

C57BL/6J mice (age 12–20 weeks) were used in these studies. The initial analysis of immunoreactive 5-HT was performed on brains from lactating dams (day 10 postpartum; Fig. 1A, top panel, see below) and age-matched virgin females. Virgin and lactating (day 10 postpartum) mice were also used for the initial analysis of serum 5-HT (Fig. 1A, bottom panel). For subsequent studies involving behavioral analyses, all females were housed with a stud male for two weeks, checked for vaginal plugs, then separated and housed individually [30,31] on a 12h:12h light:dark cycle with water and standard lab chow available ad libitum. Mated mice that did not produce a litter were assigned to the “nulliparous”, non-pregnant groups. The breeding pairs yielded a mating success rate of 47%, which is consistent with other literature reporting the mating behavior of the C57BL/6J inbred mouse line [32]. Mice that became pregnant and delivered pups had their litters culled to six pups on day 1 postpartum (day of parturition = day 0) to normalize for suckling stimulus, and these constituted the “lactating” groups. In behavioral experiment 1(see Fig. 1B), a third group of dams was included (“postpartum-nonlactating”), which were mice that had their entire litters removed immediately after delivery (postpartum day 0). All procedures were reviewed and approved by the University of Cincinnati’s Institution for Animal Care and Use Committee.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Schemas for experiments assessing behavior after multiple days of citalopram treatment.

A: Preliminary Experiment (Virgin vs. Lactating): 1) Top panel: virgin and lactating (day 10 postpartum) mice were perfused and brain tissue was collected for immunohistochemical analysis of 5-HT. 2) Bottom panel: virgin and lactating (day 10 postpartum) mice were terminated and trunk blood was collected for determination of serum 5-HT via radioimmunoassay. B: Behavioral Experiment 1 in which three groups were studied: 1) nulliparous females; 2) lactating mice; and 3) postpartum-nonlactating mice. Only the FST was administered in this study. C: Behavioral Experiment 2 examined only two groups: 1) nulliparous females and 2) lactating mice. This study included “control” behavioral tests (home cage activity and motor performance task) and an alternate test of mood-related behavioral assessment—the marble burying test (MBT)

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.g001

Experimental Designs

Preliminary Experiment: Dorsal Raphe 5-HT Immunohistochemistry Virgin vs. Lactating Mice. Female C57BL/6J mice ∼12–16 weeks in age (n = 3 virgin, n = 4 lactating) were used for the initial immunohistochemical analyses for 5-HT. Animals were deeply anesthetized with an i.p. injection of ketamine-xylazine (90mg/kg and10mg/kg, respectively) 10 minutes prior to the perfusion procedure (Fig. 1A, top panel). Animals underwent transcardial perfusion with 10mL of 0.01M phosphate buffered saline (PBS) followed by 20mL of 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains were removed (Fig. 1A, top panel), post-fixed overnight in 4% PFA, and then stored in 30% sucrose at 4°C until they sank. Brains were sectioned on a freezing microtome at 30 μm coronal slices between -4.84 Bregma and -5.02 Bregma, according to a mouse brain stereotaxic atlas [33].

Brain slices were immunostained for 5-HT as free-floating sections, after which they were mounted on slides. Briefly, sections were rinsed with ice-cold 0.01 M PBS (5 min, 3x), then incubated for 60 minutes in blocking buffer (0.01 M PBS containing 3% rabbit serum, 2% BSA, and 0.4% Triton X-100), followed by additional washes (0.01M PBS; 5 min, 3x). Sections were then incubated for 16 hours (room temp) with rat monoclonal anti-5-HT (1:200 in 0.01 M PBS + 0.4% Triton X-100). Following rinses in ice-cold 0.01 M PBS (5 min, 3x), sections were incubated for 60 min (room temp) with biotinylated rabbit anti-rat IgG (1:5000 in 0.01 M PBS + 0.4% Triton X-100). Sections were then processed according to manufacturer’s instructions, using the Vectastain Elite ABC immunoperoxidase system and Ag/Ab complexes were visualized with Ni2+-DAB enzyme substrate. Brain slices from virgin and lactating mice were exposed to the Ni2+-DAB solution for identical periods of time (5 min; in the same solution). Negative controls omitted the primary antibody from the 16-hr incubation.

Stained brain sections were analyzed using the open source NIH Image software on a Macintosh computer. The border of the dorsal raphe was defined [33] and background determined on neighboring tissue. In the thresholding mode (background subtracted) the number of stained cell bodies within the dorsal raphe and their optical densities were documented for each section. Rat polyclonal anti-5-HT and biotinylated rabbit anti-rat antibodies were purchased from Chemicon International (Billerica, MA) and Vectastain Elite ABC immunoperoxidase system from Vector Laboratories Ltd (Burlingame, CA).

Preliminary Experiment: Serum 5-HT Blood Collection and Radioimmunoassay Virgin vs. Lactating Mice. Trunk blood samples were collected from virgin (n = 30) and lactating (n = 24, day 10 postpartum) mice ∼12–20 weeks of age (Fig. 1A, bottom panel). Blood samples were allowed to clot overnight at 4°C to release 5-HT from platelets. Serum was separated by centrifugation at 12,000 rpm for 15 minutes at 4°C, and stored at -80°C until assayed. 5-HT levels were determined in duplicate using a commercial RIA kit according to the manufacturer’s instructions. The sensitivity of the RIA kit for serum 5-HT was 6.7 ng/mL and the interassay coefficient of variation was less than 5.0%. 5-HT RIA Fast Track kits were purchased from Rocky Mountain Diagnostics (Colorado Springs, CO). Citalopram was purchased from Tocris Biosciences (Ellisville, MO). Fluoxetine and all other chemicals, unless otherwise noted, were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO).

Forced Swim Test Pilot Experiment. Citalopram dose and duration used in the experiments described below were based on published literature, which has used male mice almost exclusively [34,35]. To confirm the reproducibility of the published results and the FST technique by our laboratory, a preliminary study was conducted to confirm the efficacy of this sub-chronic SSRI treatment (S1A Fig.). Male mice (age ∼12–16 weeks, n = 15 per group) received daily i.p. injections of citalopram, (5mg/kg/day) or vehicle (0.9% saline) between 08:00h and 09:30h for 5 days. Each animal was subjected to the FST thirty minutes after the final injection.

Behavioral Experiment 1: Subchronic Citalopram and Forced Swim Test. Using the FST as an assay of depressive-like behavior, three groups of female mice were tested: Group 1, nulliparous (non-pregnant, age ∼12–18 weeks; n = 30 vehicle, n = 17 citalopram); Group 2, lactating (age ∼12–18 weeks; n = 20 vehicle, n = 18 citalopram); and Group 3, postpartum-nonlactating (age ∼12–18 weeks; n = 7 vehicle; n = 7 citalopram; see Fig. 1B). On day 6 postpartum, dams of Groups 2 and 3, and age-matched nulliparous females (Group 1) began receiving daily i.p. injections of SSRI (citalopram, 5mg/kg/day) or vehicle (0.9% saline). Injections were administered between 08:00h and 09:30h for 5 days. Thirty minutes after the fifth injection on day 10 postpartum, a period of peak lactation [27,28,29], each animal was subjected to the FST.

Behavioral Experiment 2: Subchronic Citalopram and Marble Burying Task. The effect of SSRI treatment was tested again, using the marble burying task (MBT), an assay of anxiety-like behavior. In addition, two other behaviors (home cage activity and motor performance) were analyzed to determine if differences were specific to affective behavior. Only lactating dams (age ∼12–16 weeks; n = 8 per group) and age-matched nulliparous females (n = 10 per group) were used in this study (see Fig. 1C), and all animals received daily i.p. injections of citalopram (5mg/kg/day) or vehicle for five days. As in behavioral experiment 1 (Fig. 1B), injections in the lactating dams began on day 6 postpartum. Between the fourth and fifth injection, home cage activity was monitored for 24 hours (see Fig. 1C). Thirty minutes after the last injection, each animal was assessed with the MBT and then tested for motor performance using a rotarod (see Fig. 1C).

Behavioral Experiment 3: Increased Citalopram Dosage and Longer Administration. Nulliparous female mice did not exhibit any changes in FST behavior following a five day treatment with citalopram (5mg/kg/day). In light of these results we decided to increase the dose of citalopram from 5 mg/kg to 30 mg/kg, and from five days of treatment to ten days in nulliparous mice (age ∼15 weeks; n = 6 per group) prior to the FST (see S1B Fig.).

Behavioral Experiment 4: Acute Fluoxetine and Forced Swim Test. In order to rule out a citalopram-specific effect in the nulliparous mice, we decided to treat with fluoxetine, another SSRI with different pharmacokinetics [50]. Lactating mice (day 10 postpartum, age ∼16 weeks; n = 14 vehicle, n = 10 (10mg/kg), n = 9 (40mg/kg)) were treated with a single dose of either fluoxetine (10mg/kg or 40mg/kg), or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST (see S1C Fig., left panel). The higher dose (40mg/kg) was found to be effective in reducing immobility during the FST in lactating mice (see S3A Fig.). Nulliparous mice (age ∼ 12–14 weeks; n = 11 per group) were given a single injection of fluoxetine (40mg/kg) or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST (see S1C Fig., right panel).

Behavioral Tests

Mice were removed from the housing area and taken to a separate room for all behavioral testing. Mice were allowed to acclimate to the testing room for approximately one hour.

Forced Swim Test. The FST was always administered between 09:00h and 11:30h. Mice were placed in a swim tank (height = 30cm, diameter = 20cm) containing water (25°C ± 1°C) at a height of 20cm for a total of six minutes. All sessions were recorded on video and scored by two independent observers who were blinded to the treatments. The total time of immobility was recorded. The first two minutes were not used in the determination of the total immobility time as per Porsolt et al. [36]. A mouse was deemed to be immobile when it was floating and making only minor movements to keep its head above water. Tanks were emptied and rinsed clean after each animal.

Home Cage Activity. In behavioral experiment 2 (see Fig. 1C), locomotor activity within the home cage was monitored for 24 hours beginning 30 minutes after the penultimate injection of vehicle or citalopram. The home cage was placed in a SmartFrame stainless steel rack (Hamilton-Kinder Scientific Company, Poway, CA) and infrared photobeam interruption recorded both vertical and horizontal movements. Data were collected and analyzed using HMM100 MotorMonitor software and the total numbers of basic movements were broken into 60-minute time intervals.

Marble Burying Task. For the marble burying task (MBT) in behavioral experiment 2 (see Fig. 1C), each test mouse was placed into a larger, novel cage with a 5cm layer of sawdust on top of the bedding. In the lactating group, each dam was transferred to the novel cage without their pups (the home cages containing the pups were placed into a separate room so the ultrasonic vocalizations of the pups did not influence the behavioral testing). Mice were allowed to acclimate in the novel cage for 30 minutes after which 20 clean, transparent glass marbles (1.5 cm diameter) were placed on top of the sawdust, in five rows of four marbles each, with the marbles spaced equally apart. At the end of 20 minutes, the number of buried marbles (at least two-thirds covered with sawdust) was recorded [37,38]. This design was chosen following a preliminary experiment in which the effects of novel cage versus separation from pups on marble burying behavior in lactating dams was assessed (see S4 Fig.). In that experiment, marbles were either introduced into the home cage (pups remaining with some dams and removed from others), or in a novel cage setting (some dams having been transferred with their pups and others without their pups).

Motor Performance Task. Motor performance was determined using a rotarod (see Fig. 1C). Mice were placed on a stationary drum (∼62 mm diameter) that then began to revolve at a constant speed. The total amount of time the mouse remained on the rotarod was recorded during each of three trials administered 30 minutes apart. The first (acclimation) trial used a speed of 16 rpm for 120 seconds, or until the mouse failed to remain on the revolving drum. The second and third trials used a speed of 20 rpm for up to 300 seconds [39].

Blood Collection and Assays

Serum 5-HT: Measurement following FST and MBT. 20 minutes after the FST (Behavioral Experiment 1; Fig. 1B) or rotarod test (Behavioral Experiment 2; Fig. 1C), animals were terminated and trunk blood collected. The blood samples were allowed to clot overnight at 4°C to release serotonin (5-HT) from platelets. Serum was separated by centrifugation at 12,000 rpm for 15 minutes at 4°C, and stored at -80°C until assayed. 5-HT levels were determined in duplicate using a commercial RIA kit according to the manufacturer’s instructions. The sensitivity of the RIA kit for serum 5-HT was 6.7 ng/mL and the interassay coefficient of variation was less than 5.0%. 5-HT RIA Fast Track kits were purchased from Rocky Mountain Diagnostics (Colorado Springs, CO). Citalopram was purchased from Tocris Biosciences (Ellisville, MO). Fluoxetine and all other chemicals, unless otherwise noted, were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO).

Data Analysis

Data obtained in each experiment were analyzed using either one-way or two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Bonferonni post hoc test or Dunn’s post hoc test for comparison of the means. In some experiments when only two means were compared, a two-tailed Student’s t-test (unpaired) was used. All data shown are presented as the mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). All experiments were designed to achieve a statistical power of 80 percent. Means were considered to be significantly different when p<0.05. All statistical tests were performed using Graph Pad Prism statistical software.

Results

Preliminary Experiment: Dorsal Raphe 5-HT Immunohistochemistry Virgin vs. Lactating Mice

In all sections of the dorsal raphe, immunoreactive 5-HT was noticeably less in the lactating mice as compared to virgin mice, examples of which are presented in Fig. 2, A and B. The differences in 5-HT immunoreactivity were particularly evident in the lateral “wings” of the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN)—the ventrolateral aspect of the periaqueductal gray (VLPAG). Image analysis of the staining indicated that both the number of stained cells and the intensity (over background) of staining were significantly reduced in the DRN of lactating mice (Fig. 2, C and D [t = 12.48, df = 6, p<0.01]).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Serotonin immunostaining of the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) immediately caudal to oculomotor nucleus.

(Bregma -4.48 mm). Representative examples of sections processed for 5-HT immunostaining and visualized with Ni2+-DAB (blue-black stain) from virgin female mice (A; n = 3) and lactating mice (B; n = 4). C: Average optical density of cell bodies (background-subtracted; threshold detection) in the field as demarcated by the dashed lines in A and B. D: Average number of detectable stained cells in the field. Aq, Aqueduct of Sylvius; DRD, dorsal raphe-dorsal part; DRV, dorsal raphe-ventral part; VLPAG, ventrolateral aspect of the periaqueductal gray; mlf, medial longitudinal fasciculus. Student’s t-test comparison of the means: *p<0.05, **p<0.01 compared to virgin controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.g002

Preliminary Experiment: Serum 5-HT Virgin vs. Lactating Mice

Serum 5-HT was significantly elevated in lactating mice compared with virgin female mice (Fig. 3 [t = 2.220, df = 52, p<0.05]). Nearly all circulating 5-HT is sequestered in the dense granules of the platelets and is released into the serum upon clotting [40].

thumbnail
Fig 3. Serum levels of serotonin in virgin female and lactating C57Bl/6J mice.

Trunk blood was allowed to clot overnight, releasing platelet 5-HT stores. Lactating mice had significantly higher levels of serum 5-HT than virgin females. Student’s t-test comparison of the means: *p<0.05 compared to virgin controls (n = 24–30 per group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.g003

Forced Swim Test Pilot Experiment

Male mice treated with citalopram via injection for five days (5mg/kg/day), showed a significant [(t = 2.550, df = 28, p<0.05)] reduction in immobility time in the FST as compared to vehicle-treated controls (S2A Fig.), as has been widely reported in the literature [34,35].

Behavioral Experiment 1: Subchronic Citalopram and Forced Swim Test

In female mice subjected to the FST there was a significant main effect of lactation state (Fig. 4), such that the lactating females overall had lower immobility times than either of the non-lactating groups as determined by two-way ANOVA [F(2,93) = 11.23, p<0.001]. There was also a main effect of drug treatment [F(1,93) = 5.033, p = 0.0272] on immobility during the FST, but there was no interaction between lactation and drug treatment. In response to citalopram the lactating females showed a significant response to the SSRI as demonstrated by a decrease in immobility time [(t = 2.820, df = 36, p<0.05, compared with vehicle-treated)]. However, neither of the non-lactating groups (nulliparous or pup-deprived) responded to citalopram (Fig. 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. In the FST, lactating mice respond to regimen of citalopram treatment that has no effect on nulliparous or postpartum-nonlactating female mice.

Mice received daily injections of vehicle or citalopram (5mg/kg/day, i.p.) for 5 days. Total immobility time (excluding the first two minutes of the test). Bonferonni post hoc test comparison of the means: *p<0.05 compared to vehicle-treated lactating mice; ***p<0.001 overall effect of lactation determined by 2-way ANOVA (n = 7–30 per group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.g004

Behavioral Experiment 2: Subchronic Citalopram and Marble Burying Task

To verify the differential responsiveness of lactating and non-lactating mice to SSRIs, and also to determine if these responses are specific to affective behavior, multiple behaviors were assessed following five days of citalopram treatment (see Fig. 1C).

Marble Burying Task

Affective behavior was assessed using the MBT, a measurement of anxiety-like behavior. Separation of dams from their pups during the MBT caused elevated anxiety-like behavior (more marbles buried) [41,42,43], which was independent of whether they remained in the home cage, or were moved to a novel cage (S4 Fig.). Lactating mice kept with their pups buried so few marbles (1.2 ± 0.7, with several dams burying no marbles at all) that an effect of SSRI treatment would be impossible to measure. Thus, we chose to test citalopram on dams moved to a novel cage without their pups (Fig. 5). Similar to results in the FST, non-lactating females exhibited no behavioral changes in the MBT following citalopram treatment (five days, 5mg/kg/day). Two-way ANOVA revealed an interaction between lactation and drug treatment on marble burying behavior [F(1,32) = 13.11, p = 0.0010, Fig. 5]. Lactating females experienced a significant reduction in anxiety-like behavior in response to citalopram (Fig. 5 [t = 4.857, df = 16, p<0.0001]). Notably, the marble-burying activity of citalopram-treated lactating females in the absence of their pups was similar to that in mothers with their pups present (Fig. 5).

thumbnail
Fig 5. In the marble-burying test, lactating mice respond to citalopram treatment that has no effect on nulliparous mice.

All mice were moved to a novel cage for testing. Lactating mice were tested while separated from their pups. Student’s t-test comparisons of the mean: *p<0.05 compared to vehicle-treated nulliparous mice; ***p<0.001 compared to vehicle-treated lactating group (n = 5–10 per group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.g005

Home Cage Activity

SSRI treatment did not significantly alter locomotor activity measured during the 24 hour period between the 4th and 5th injection of citalopram in either lactating or nulliparous mice. Not surprisingly, lactating dams had significantly less overall home-cage activity than nulliparous mice (Fig. 6, inset [F(1,64) = 64.4; p<0.0001]). Since rodents are nocturnal animals, the home cage activity was divided into light and dark phases and reanalyzed. SSRI treatment did not alter locomotor activity in either lactating or nulliparous controls during the light or dark phases of the cycle (Fig. 6). As expected, nulliparous animals exhibited an increase in locomotor activity during the dark phase. However, lactating dams (day 9–10 postpartum) did not exhibit any significant changes in locomotor activity between the dark and light phases (Fig. 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Citalopram has no effect on home cage activity of either nulliparous or lactating mice.

Activity was monitored over a 24-h period and data is shown for light and dark periods separately or for entire 24-h period (Inset). Citalopram treatment did not affect home cage activity in any of the groups. Note significant increase in activity of nulliparous animals during the dark period. Bonferonni post hoc test for comparison of the means: *p<0.05, **p<0.01 activity in dark vs. corresponding light period. 2-way ANOVA***p<0.001 lactating vs. corresponding nulliparous group (n = 8–10 per group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.g006

Motor Performance Task

SSRI treatment also had no effect on motor performance as assessed by the rotarod task in either lactating mice (day 10 postpartum) or nulliparous mice (Fig. 7). The data obtained from the rotarod task were analyzed using the Friedman test followed by a Dunn’s post-hoc analysis, which indicated that both lactating and nulliparous mice spent significantly more time on the rotarod during Trial 3 than Trial 1 for both vehicle and citalopram (5mg/kg/day) treatment groups (Lactating: q = 4.884, df = 68, p<0.0001; Nulliparous: q = 5.885, df = 68, p<0.0001, Fig. 7).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Citalopram has no effect on motor performance in either nulliparous or lactating mice.

The time spend on the rotarod in each of 3 trials is shown. All animals demonstrated improved ability to stay on the rotarod with each subsequent trial. Inset: Means of the slopes for each animal’s performance (time over trial number, n = 8–10 per group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.g007

Serum 5-HT: Measurement following FST and MBT

Circulating 5-HT levels were also analyzed in the mice from the behavioral experiments. Two-way ANOVA (drug treatment X lactation state) of these data determined there were significant main effects of both drug treatment [F(1,88) = 10.75; p<0.01] and lactation state [F(2,88) = 6.079; p<0.01], but the interaction between the two factors was not quite significant [F(2,88) = 2.507; p = 0.08]. As found in the mice used for the preliminary experiment (Fig. 3), lactating mice (vehicle-treated controls) again had significantly elevated levels of serum 5-HT (Fig. 8, [t = 2.698, df = 34, p<0.05]). Serum 5-HT was not significantly different between nulliparous and postpartum-nonlactating female mice. In addition, citalopram treatment significantly reduced serum 5-HT concentration in lactating mice (Fig. 8, [t = 3.618, df = 28, p<0.01]), but had no effect on serum 5-HT in either the nulliparous or postpartum-nonlactating groups.

thumbnail
Fig 8. Serum 5-HT from female mice at various reproductive states, following five days of treatment with vehicle or SSRI (citalopram, 5mg/kg/day).

All mice were age-matched and mated. Blood was collected from postpartum-nonlactating and lactating animals on day 10 postpartum. Trunk blood was allowed to clot overnight, releasing platelet 5-HT stores. Bonferonni post hoc test for comparison of means: *p<0.05 compared to vehicle-treated nulliparous group; **p<0.01 compared to vehicle-treated lactating group (n = 12–20 per group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.g008

Behavioral Experiment 3: Increased Citalopram Dosage and Longer Administration

Nulliparous female mice tested at a higher dose and longer duration of citalopram (30mg/kg/day, 10 days) also showed no response to citalopram in the FST (S2B Fig.).

Behavioral Experiment 4: Acute Fluoxetine and Forced Swim Test

Similarly, non-lactating females were insensitive to an acute dose of a second SSRI, fluoxetine (S3B Fig.), while lactating females responded to fluoxetine with a significant reduction in immobility during the FST (S3A Fig. [t = 3.210, df = 21, p<0.01]).

Discussion

The present study produced several novel findings. First, and perhaps most significantly, lactating mice, day 10 postpartum, a time of peak lactation [27,28,29] exhibit behavioral responsiveness to SSRI treatments that have no measurable effect on non-lactating female mice. Second, lactating mice have significantly elevated serum 5-HT stores when compared to non-lactating females. Moreover, SSRI treatment significantly reduced serum 5-HT stores in lactating dams, while having no effect on those of non-lactating females. Third, immunoreactive 5-HT is significantly decreased in the dorsal raphe of lactating mice compared with nulliparous females.

Lactating mice exhibited baseline affective behavior consistent with a more positive mood compared with nonlactating females. There is literature that has reported an increase in depressive-like behavior during the postpartum period [44]. Furthermore, additional studies reported an increase in anhedonia during the postpartum period [45,46]. We did not observe an increase in depressive-like behaviors during our experiments. Our experiments were conducted in C57BL/6J mice, whereas, the aforementioned studies were conducted in Sprague-Dawley Rats, which could account for the different results. It should be noted that the majority (80–90%) of women who give birth do not experience PPD [47]. In the clinical literature the women who do decide to breastfeed often report elevations in mood [47,48]. We did not utilize a model of induced maternal depression; and our results seem to support those reported in the clinical literature. Our lactating mice also exhibited an anxiolytic phenotype (buried fewer marbles in a home cage setting) when compared to age-matched nulliparous controls. Another research group also reported a decrease in anxiety-related behaviors during the postpartum, but in contrast to our results, these anxiolytic effects were independent of lactation [49].

Lactating mice exhibited an increased responsiveness to SSRI treatments and this was true with both the sub-chronic administration of citalopram (5mg/kg/day for 5 days) and the acute administration of fluoxetine (single injection of 40mg/kg). While both block 5-HT uptake via SERT, the two SSRIs differ in structure, metabolism, and pharmacokinetics [50]. Neither SSRI altered affective behavior in nulliparous female mice. Even longer treatment with a much higher dose of citalopram (30mg/kg/day for 10 days) did not change this lack of response in the nulliparous females.

Also not responsive to SSRI treatment were female mice that had gone through a full pregnancy but had their pups removed and were not lactating. These data indicate that it is the state of lactation and/or the presence of the pups, but not parity per se, that underlies the change in responsiveness to SSRIs. This does not necessarily mean that pregnancy is not required to prepare the central circuitry for the alterations observed in the lactating female. The brain undergoes dramatic changes that often begin during pregnancy in preparation for parturition and lactation. The supraoptic and paraventricular nuclei, sites of oxytocin cell bodies, undergo extensive neuro-glia remodeling [51]. Also during lactation, the tuberoinfundibular dopaminergic (TIDA) neurons that normally inhibit prolactin release begin to express enkephalin, which stimulates prolactin [52]. If changes were initiated by pregnancy, the enhanced responsiveness to SSRI clearly requires the active state of lactation.

As with the measures of affective behavior, only lactating dams had a significant decrease in serum 5-HT, reflecting platelet stores, in response to SSRI treatment. In the clinical literature, various parameters of platelet 5-HT have been examined as a potential peripheral marker of SSRI efficacy. This has arisen from the search for a biochemical marker that would have greater discriminatory power than the subjective rating scales used for measuring depression. The concentration of platelet 5-HT has consistently appeared useful as a marker in data from multiple laboratories. There is a single gene for SERT and the transporter expressed in platelets is the same as that expressed in neurons [53], so SSRI treatment blocks uptake of 5-HT into platelets as well. Because platelets do not contain 5-HT biosynthetic enzymes, blocking SERT results in a gradual loss of platelet serotonin stores. It has been demonstrated by independent laboratories that the magnitude of SSRI effect on platelet 5-HT directly correlates with their efficacy in alleviating depressive symptoms as determined by standard psychometric tools such as the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale [54,55]. Correlation of SSRI-induced decrease in platelet 5-HT levels with effects on affective behavior was also seen in the present mouse experiments. Citalopram treatment significantly reduced serum 5-HT concentration only in lactating dams (Fig. 8), and the lactating mice was the only group in which citalopram (or fluoxetine) significantly altered behaviors relevant to mood as assessed by the FST and MBT (Figs. 4 and 5).

The elevated serum 5-HT storage pools in lactating dams is striking, being up to 40% greater than that in nulliparous mice. The majority of serotonin in the body is produced by the enterochromaffin cells in the mucosa of the gut [56] where it regulates peristaltic and secretory reflexes [57,58]. Serotonin produced in the gut enters the bloodstream where it is rapidly transported into platelets via SERT [53] and stored in dense-core granules [40]. 5-HT is released upon platelet activation, and functions in vasoconstriction and thrombosis. High circulating 5-HT is found in certain pathologies, such as carcinoid tumors, but lactation appears to be a unique physiological state in which circulating 5-HT levels are elevated. Platelet 5-HT levels do not differ between sexes or among healthy people of varying age (with the exception of newborns) [59]. Also, there is no circadian rhythm in platelet 5-HT content despite a small circadian rhythm in plasma tryptophan [60]. It is conceivable that the elevated 5-HT during lactation is traceable to 5-HT synthesis in the mammary glands [27], but it is also the case that the intestine of female mammals undergoes marked mucosal hyperplasia during lactation [61]. Thus, the elevated serum (platelet stores) 5-HT during lactation may be supplied by multiple sources.

The vast majority of cell bodies of the central serotonergic system are located within the brainstem and caudal midbrain. Of particular interest are the cell bodies contained within the DRN, as these are the origins of the major ascending serotonergic pathways to the forebrain, including numerous limbic structures. The markedly lower 5-HT immunoreactivity in the dorsal raphe nuclei (DRN) of lactating dams as compared with nulliparous controls may indicate a change in central 5-HT activity, resulting in decreased storage pools of 5-HT. However, the quantification of 5-HT staining in the DRN must be interpreted in light of other evidence. For example, the predicted effects on affective behaviors are very different in the case of reduced 5-HT synthesis or increased 5-HT transport and release. The reduced 5-HT staining in the DRN of lactating mice was particularly striking in the lateral aspects of the DRN and VLPAG. Serotonergic neurons arising from these regions project to a distributed system involved in physiological and behavioral responses associated with stress and anxiety [62]. Retrograde labeling and double-staining experiments [96] have demonstrated that the serotonergic neurons projecting to the amygdala [63] and lateral septum [64] are concentrated at the level shown in Fig. 2, A and B (Bregma -4.84). Both the amygdala and the lateral septum are key components in the brain’s emotional circuitry that modulates affective behavior and stress responses [65,66]. Increased serotonergic neurotransmission to these regions could be expected to reduce depressive-like or anxiety-related behavior.

We do not propose that 5-HT acts alone in regulating affective behavior and mood during lactation. Numerous other neurohormones and transmitters alter affective states, many of which undergo dramatic changes during pregnancy and lactation. Obvious candidates during lactation include prolactin and oxytocin, the endocrine secretions of which are required for milk production and milk let-down in the mammary gland. Receptors for both hormones are present in a number of brain regions, including those associated with affective behavior, such as the amygdala and lateral septum [67,68]. Both hormones regulate behaviors associated with reproduction, such as pair-bonding and parenting [69,70]. In addition, oxytocin has been reported to have antidepressant and anxiolytic activities [71,72]. One study found anxiolytic action of oxytocin only in pregnant and lactating rats, and not in virgin females [73]. One possible explanation for the differential responsiveness to SSRIs between non-lactating and lactating mice could be the fact that there is a higher release of oxytocin from the paraventricular (PVN) and supraotic (SON) nuclei during lactation [74]. Oxytocin has been shown to be important in pair bonding behavior [69]. Furthermore, oxytocin-labeled fibers have been found within limbic structures that could modulate mood-like behaviors [75,76]. SERT labeled fibers have been identified within the PVN and SON of non-human primates [77]. There is some evidence that SSRIs can increase the release of oxytocin in humans [78]. It could be hypothesized that the SSRIs are acting through these SERT-containing fibers, and increasing the release of oxytocin, often considered a “feel-good” modulator, in the lactating mice. This might explain the difference in responsiveness to SSRIs between non-lactating and lactating female mice.

Intracerebroventricular administration of antisense oligonucleotides against the prolactin receptor was found to increase anxiety-related behavior in lactating rats as well as impair maternal behavior [79]. 5-HT serves as a mediator in the neuroendocrine reflexes that result in the secretion of oxytocin and prolactin, including suckling-induced and stress-induced release [80,81,82]. Thus, either or both of these two hormones may be involved in the differential behavioral responsiveness to SSRIs in lactating and nonlactating mice that we report here.

In addition to being exclusive to lactating dams, the effect of SSRI treatment was specific for measures of affective and mood-related behaviors. The FST is considered a measure of behavioral coping in animals presented with an inescapable stress and has predictive validity in detecting the effects of antidepressants [36]. Treatments or psychological states that elevate mood result in decreased time of immobility. The MBT is considered a measure of anxiety-related behavior and treatments that reduce anxiety in humans result in a decrease in the number of marbles buried. Both of these behavioral tests are commonly used in the pharmaceutical industry for screening antidepressants and anxiolytics [37,38,83,84]. One potential caveat to our experiments is that we administered the SSRI via an i.p. injection instead of via oral gavage. The altered metabolism that often occurs during pregnancy and lactation could dramatically decrease the concentration of the SSRI being administered orally. There is a possibility that our route of administration may have circumvented the increase in metabolism [85]. The clinical implications of our results should be interpreted based on the administration route. Future animal studies examining the interaction between lactation and SSRI efficacy should address this difference in administration route.

5-HT is involved in numerous central functions including motor skills, learning and memory, sleep-wakefulness cycles, and others. SSRI treatment had no effect on motor peformance in the rotorod test. This test also involves some learning over the three trials and SSRI treatment did not alter the rate at which motor performance improved in any of the animals (Fig. 7). Spontaneous locomotor activity in the home cage also was unaffected by SSRI in both the nulliparous and lactating mice. The absence of a circadian rhythm in home cage activity in lactating mice was both surprising and novel (Fig. 6). Of the few studies in which circadian rhythms were assessed in lactating animals, even fewer monitored spontaneous activity. Both nocturnal rats and hamsters [86,87] have a significant reduction in the difference between the levels of spontaneous activity during the light and dark periods in lactating animals as compared with virgin or pregnant animals. Yet a significant rhythm is still present. The diurnal Nile grass rat also exhibits a circadian rhythm in activity during lactation which is even greater than that of virgin females but less than that of pregnant females [88]. Although we expected a dampening of the difference in cage activity between the light and dark periods in lactating mice, due to time spent nursing in the nest, we were surprised at the total absence of a rhythm. By day 10 of lactation, pups are more than 70% of their weight at weaning [27,28,29] and should well tolerate the dam spending time outside the nest. Whether this arhythmicity in cage activity of lactating C57BL/6J mice is characteristic of other non-maternal behaviors and/or vegetative functions (e.g. hormone rhythms) would require further investigation.

Any potential relationship between breastfeeding and maternal mood has not been definitively established. Although most studies have found a positive correlation between breastfeeding and alleviation of PPD symptoms [48,89,90], a few studies have found a higher incidence of PPD among breastfeeding women [91,92]. A recent qualitative systematic review of the literature found that most of these latter studies have methodological limits and weaknesses that equivocate conclusions about how breastfeeding influences maternal depressive symptomatology [93]. However, the authors did conclude that there is unequivocal evidence that PPD negatively influences infant-feeding outcomes.

To our knowledge, only one other study has investigated the interaction of lactation and efficacy of SSRIs [94]. Pawluski and colleagues used a model of maternal adversity to examine the effects of stress during gestation and the effects of fluoxetine on behaviors during the postpartum. In most animal models, studies on the efficacy of antidepressant treatment have been done primarily in males and, to a much lesser extent, in virgin females. The few studies that have used lactating animals have not compared their responses to those of non-lactating females. The clinical literature also has not addressed whether breastfeeding can alter responsiveness to SSRIs. Those studies that have examined SSRI use in lactating mothers have been concerned with risk assessment for negative outcomes in babies exposed to the medications during breastfeeding. Current research focuses on two aspects of antidepressant use during breastfeeding: 1) how much of the medication passes into breast milk, and 2) does the medication affect the infant?

Conclusions

The clinical implications of these findings are significant. It is clear that untreated maternal depression has a highly negative impact on the development of the child [95], while breastfeeding is recognized as having a highly positive effect on child health and development. Yet, mothers with PPD often believe that they must make a choice between treatment and breastfeeding [97]. It may be that combined treatment and breastfeeding has the greatest benefit for both mother and child. Moreover, if the state of lactation does increase sensitivity to the antidepressant actions of SSRIs, then it is possible that lower doses of drug may be effective in treating PPD in many patients. What remains clear is that clinical studies on the potential interactions of lactation and SSRI efficacy are very much needed.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. Schemas for experiments assessing behavior after treatment with citalopram or fluoxetine.

A: FST Pilot Experiment: the effectiveness of subchronic treatment (5 days) with citalopram (5mg/kg/day) and determined replicability of previous published results. B: Behavioral Experiment 3: the effects of an increased dose (30mg/kg/day) and duration (10 days) of citalopram in nulliparous mice during the FST. C: Behavioral Experiment 4: 1) Left panel: lactating mice (day 10 postpartum) were treated with a single dose of fluoxetine (10mg/kg or 40mg/kg), or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST. 2) Right panel: nulliparous mice were given a single injection of fluoxetine (40mg/kg) or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.s001

(TIFF)

S2 Fig. Citalopram treatment reduces total immobility during FST in male C57Bl6 mice, but not in nulliparous female mice.

A. Male mice respond to regimen of citalopram treatment that has no effect on nulliparous or postpartum-nonlactating female mice (5 days @ 5mg/kg/day, i.p.; see Fig. 4; n = 15 per group). B. Nulliparous female mice do not respond to increased dose and duration of citalopram. Mice received daily injections of vehicle or citalopram (30mg/kg/day, i.p.) for 10 days (n = 6 per group). Student’s t-test for comparison of the means: **p<0.05 compared with vehicle-treated controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.s002

(TIFF)

S3 Fig. Lactating mice, but not nulliparous female mice, respond to acute fluoxetine treatment in the FST.

All mice received a single injection of vehicle or fluoxetine (Flx; 10 or 40 mg/kg) and subjected to FST 90 minutes later. A. Acute SSRI reduced total immobility time in the FST (n = 9–14 per group). B. Acute SSRI treatment (fluoxetine, 40mg/kg) had no effect on total immobility time nulliparous female C57/Bl6 mice (n = 11 per group). **p<0.01 compared vehicle-treated controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.s003

(TIFF)

S4 Fig. Separation from pups increases marble-burying activity of lactating female mice.

The number of marbles buried by lactating mice with pups or separated from their pups was measured either in their home cage or a novel cage. Bonferonni post hoc for comparison of means: ***p<0.001 vs. respective “with pups” group (n = 5–8 per group).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117339.s004

(TIFF)

Acknowledgments

The authors are deeply indebted to Dr. Heather Christensen and Michael Murawsky for analysis of the FST videos, and to Dr. Courtney Pinard and Dr. Hadley Bergstrom for critical review of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: NJJ BAM NDH SCB KAG. Performed the experiments: NJJ BAM. Analyzed the data: NJJ BAM KAG. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: NDH SCB KAG. Wrote the paper: NJJ BAM NDH SCB KAG.

References

  1. 1. Doornbos B, Fekkes D, Marit AC, de Jonge P, Korf J (2008) Sequential serotonin and noradrenalin associated processes involved in postpartum blues. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 32: 1320–1325. pmid:18502014
  2. 2. Halligan SL, Murray L, Martins C, Cooper PJ (2007) Maternal depression and psychiatric outcomes in adolescent offspring: a 13-year longitudinal study. J Affect Dis 97: 145–154. pmid:16863660
  3. 3. Hay DF, Pawlby S, Waters CS, Sharp D (2008) Antepartum and postpartum exposure to maternal depression: different effects on different adolescent outcomes. J Child Psychol Psyc 49: 1070–1088.
  4. 4. Pearlstein T, Howard M, Salisbury A, Zlotnick C (2009) Postpartum depression. Am J Obstet Gynecol 200: 357–364. pmid:19318144
  5. 5. O’Hara MW, Swain AM (1996) Rates and risk of postpartum depression—a meta-analysis. Int Rev Psychiatr 8: 37–54.
  6. 6. Yonkers KA, Ramin SM, Rush AJ, Navarrete CA, Carmody T, et al. (2001) Onset and persistence of postpartum depression in an inner-city maternal health clinic system. Am J Psychiat 158: 1856–1863. pmid:11691692
  7. 7. Dietz PM, Williams SB, Callaghan WM, Bachman DJ, Whitlock EP, et al. (2007) Clinically identified maternal depression before, during, and after pregnancies ending in live births. Am J Psychiat 164: 1515–1520. pmid:17898342
  8. 8. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 5th edition. American Psychiatric Publishing. Arlington, VA, USA. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.1875 pmid:25625597
  9. 9. Maguire J, Mody I (2008) GABAAR plasticity during pregnancy: relevance to postpartum depression. Neuron 59: 207–213. pmid:18667149
  10. 10. Pawluski JL, Lieblich SE, Galea LA (2009) Offspring-exposure reduces depressive-like behaviour in the parturient female rat. Behav Brain Res 197: 55–61. pmid:18760310
  11. 11. Brummelte S, Galea LA (2010) Chronic corticosterone during pregnancy and postpartum affects maternal care, cell proliferation and depressive-like behavior in the dam. Horm Behav 58: 769–779. pmid:20688070
  12. 12. Delgado PL, Price LH, Miller HL, Salomon RM, Licinio J, et al. (1991) Rapid serotonin depletion as aprovocative challenge test for patients with major depression: relevance to antidepressant action and the neurobiology of depression. Psychopharmacol Bull 27: 321–330. pmid:1775606
  13. 13. Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB (1994) Role of serotonin in the pathophysiology of depression: Focus on the serotonin transporter. Clin Chem 41:288–295.
  14. 14. Newport DJ, Owens MJ, Knight DL, Ragan K, Morgan N, et al. (2004) Alterations in platelet serotonin transporter binding in women with postpartum onset major depression. J Psychiat Res 38: 467–473. pmid:15380396
  15. 15. Lohoff FW (2010) Overview of the genetics of major depressive disorder. Cur Psychiat Rep 12: 539–546.
  16. 16. Hannah P, Adams D, Lee A, Glover V, Sandler M (1992) Links between early post-partum mood and post-natal depression. Brit J Psychiat 160: 777–80. pmid:1617360
  17. 17. van Praag HM (1984) Depression, suicide, and serotonin metabolism in the brain. In: Post RM and Ballenger JC (eds). Neurobiology of Mood Disorders. Williams & Wilkins: Baltimore, MD. pp 601–618.
  18. 18. Nordstrom P, Asberg M (1992) Suicide risk and serotonin. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 6: 6–21.
  19. 19. Cowen PJ, Parry-Billings M, Newsholme EA (1989) Decreased plasma tryptophan levels in major depression. J Affect Dis 16: 27–31. pmid:2521647
  20. 20. Yonkers KA (2003) Special issue related to the treatment of depression in women. J Clin Psychiat 64: 8–13.
  21. 21. Epperson N, Czarkowski KA, Ward-O’Brien D, Weiss E, Gueorguieva R, et al. (2001) Maternal sertraline treatment and serotonin transport in breast-feeding mother-infant pairs. Am J Psychiat 158: 1631–1637. pmid:11578995
  22. 22. Ng R C, Hirata CK, Yeung W, Haller E, Finley PR (2010) Pharmacologic treatment for postpartum depression: A systematic review. Pharmacotherapy 30:928–941. pmid:20795848
  23. 23. Kirsch I, Deacon BJ, Huedo-Medina TB, Scoboria A, Moore TJ, et al. (2008) Initial Severity and Antidepressant Benefits: A Meta-Analysis of Data Submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. PLOS Med 5: e45. pmid:18303940
  24. 24. Fournier JC, DeRubeis RJ, Hollon SD, Dimidjian S, Amsterdam JD, et al. (2010) Antidepressant drug effects and depression severity. J Am Med Assoc 303: 47–53.
  25. 25. Horder J, Matthews P, Waldmann R (2011) Placebo, prozac, and PLOS: significant lessons from psychopharmacology. J Psychopharmacol 25: 1277–1288. pmid:20571143
  26. 26. Gibbons RD, Hur K, Brown CH, Davis JM, Mann JJ (2012) Benefits from antidepressants: synthesis of 6-week patient-level outcomes from double blind placebo-controlled randomized trials of fluoxetine and venlafaxine. Arch Gen Psychiat 69: 572–579. pmid:22393205
  27. 27. Matsuda M, Imaoka T, Vomachka AJ, Gudelsky GA, Hou Z, et al. (2004) Serotonin regulates mammary gland development via an autocrine-paracrine loop. Dev Cell 6: 193–203. pmid:14960274
  28. 28. Johnson MS, Thomson SC, Speakman JR (2001) Limits to sustained energy intake: I. Lactation in the laboratory mouse mus musculus. J Exp Biol 204: 1925–1935. pmid:11441034
  29. 29. Christensen HR, Murawsky MK, Horseman ND, Wilson TA, Gregerson KA (2013) Completely humanized prolactin rescues infertility in prolactin knockout mice and leads to human prolactin expression in extrapituitary mouse tissues. Endocrinology 154: 4777–4789. pmid:24029242
  30. 30. Brown RE, Mathieson WB, Stapleton J, Neumann PE (1999) Maternal behavior in female C57BL/6J and DBA/2J inbred mice. Physiol and Beh 67:599–605.
  31. 31. Foldi CJ, Eyles DW, McGrath JJ, Burne THJ (2011) The effects of breeding protocol on in C57BL/6J mice on adult offspring behaviour. PLOS ONE 6: e18152. pmid:21448436
  32. 32. Lambert R (2007) Breeding strategies for maintaining colonies of laboratory mice: A Jackson Laboratory resource manual. Jackson Laboratories www.jax.org/jaxmice https://doi.org/10.1093/jxb/erm028 pmid:25506957
  33. 33. Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ (2001) The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 2nd ed. Elsevier Science Publishers. San Diego, CA. 71–73. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014088919546 pmid:19003317
  34. 34. Cervo L, Canetta A, Calcagno E, Burbassi S, Sachetti G, et al. (2005) Genotype-dependent activity of tryptophan hydroxylase-2 determines response to citalopram in a mouse model of depression. J Neuroscience 25: 8165–8172. pmid:16148224
  35. 35. Chenu F, Guiard BP, Bourin M, Gardier AM (2006) Antidepressant-like activity of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors combined with NK1 receptor antagonist in the mouse forced swimming test. Beh Brain Res 172: 256–263. pmid:16806519
  36. 36. Porsolt RD, Bertin A, Jalfre M (1977) Behavioral despair in mice: a primary screening test for antidepressants. Arch Int Pharmacod Ther 229: 327–336. pmid:596982
  37. 37. Njung'e K, Handley SL (1991) Effects of 5-HT uptake inhibitors, agonists and antagonists on the burying of harmless objects by mice; a putative test for anxiolytic agents. Brit J Pharmacol 104: 105–112.
  38. 38. Njung'e K, Handley SL (1991) Evaluation of marble-burying behavior as a model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Beh 38:63–67. pmid:2017455
  39. 39. Dawson PA, Steane SE, Markovich D (2004) Behavioral abnormalities of the hyposulphataemic Nas1 knock-out mouse. Beh Brain Res 154: 457–463. pmid:15313034
  40. 40. da Prada M, Picotti GB (1979) Content and subcellular localization of catecholamines and 5-hydroxytryptamine in human and animal blood platelets: monoamine distribution between platelets and plasma. Brit J Pharmacol 65: 653–662.
  41. 41. Huot RL, Gonzalez ME, Ladd CO, Thrivikraman KV, Plotsky PM (2004) Foster litters prevent hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis sensitization mediated by neonatal maternal separation. Psychoneuroendocrino 29: 279–289.
  42. 42. Michaels CC, Holtzman SG (2006) Neonatal stress and litter composition alter sucrose intake in both rat dam and offspring. Physiol Beh 89: 735–741. pmid:16996094
  43. 43. Kosten TA, Kehoe P (2010) Immediate and enduring effects of neonatal isolation on maternal behavior in rats. Int J Dev Neuroscience 28: 53–61. pmid:19782745
  44. 44. Craft RM, Kostick ML, Rogers JA, White CL, Tsutsui KT (2010) Forced swim test behavior in postpartum rats. Physiol Beh 96: 402–412.
  45. 45. Green AD, Barr AM, Galea LAM (2009) Role of estradiol withdrawal in ‘anhedonic’ sucrose consumption: A model of postpartum depression. Physiol Beh 97: 259–265.
  46. 46. Navarre BM, Laggart JD, Craft RM (2010) Anhedonia in postpartum rats. Physiol Beh 99: 59–66. pmid:19850056
  47. 47. Kendell RE, McGuire RJ, Connor Y, Cox JL (1981) Mood changes in the first three weeks after childbirth. J Affect Dis 3: 317–326. pmid:6459348
  48. 48. McCoy SJB, Beal JM, Shipman SBM, Peyton ME, Watson GH (2006) Risk factors for postpartum depression: a retrospective investigation at 4-weeks postnatal and a review of the literature. J Am Osteopath Assoc 106: 193–198. pmid:16627773
  49. 49. Lonstein JS (2005) Reduced anxiety in postpartum rats requires recent physical interactions with pups, but is independent of suckling or peripheral sources of hormones. Horm Beh 47: 241–255. pmid:15708752
  50. 50. Baumann P, Rochat B (1995) Comparative pharmacokinetics of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: a look behind the mirror. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 10: 15–21. pmid:8866761
  51. 51. Theodosis DT (2002) Oxytocin-secreting neurons: A physiological model of morphological neuronal and glial plasticity in the adult hypothalamus. Front Neuroendocrin 23: 101–135. pmid:11906204
  52. 52. Merchenthaler I (1993) Induction of enkephalin in tuberoinfundibular dopaminergic neurons during lactation. Endocrinology 133: 2645–2651. pmid:7694844
  53. 53. Lesch K-P, Wolozin BL, Murphy DL, Riederer P (1993) Primary structure of the human platelet serotonin uptake site: Identity with the brain serotonin transporter. J Neurochem 60: 2319–2322. pmid:7684072
  54. 54. Axelson DA, Perel JM, Birmaher B, Rudolph G, Nuss S, et al. (2005) Platelet serotonin reuptake inhibition and response to SSRIs in depressed adolescents. Am J Psychiat 162: 802–804. pmid:15800159
  55. 55. Maurer-Spurej E, Pittendreigh C, Misri S (2007) Platelet serotonin levels support depression scores for women with postpartum depression. J Psychiat Neuroscience 32:23–29.
  56. 56. Barter R, Pearse AG (1955) Mammalian enterochromaffin cells as the source of serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine). J Patho Bacteriol 69: 25–31. pmid:13243168
  57. 57. Grider JR, Kuemmerle JF, Jin JG (1996) 5-HT released by mucosal stimuli initiates peristalsis by activating 5-HT4/5-HT1p receptors on sensory CGRP neurons. Am J Physiol 270: G778–G782. pmid:8967488
  58. 58. Cooke HJ (2000) Neurotransmitters in neuronal reflexes regulating intestinal secretion. An NY Acad Sci 915: 77–80. pmid:11193603
  59. 59. Flachaire E, Beney C, Berthier A, Salandre J, Quincy C, et al. (1990) Determination of reference values for serotonin concentration in platelets of healthy newborns, children, adults, and elderly subjects by HPLC with electrochemical detection. Clin Chem 36: 2117–2120. pmid:2253357
  60. 60. Eynard N, Flachaire E, Lestra C, Broyer M, Zaidan R, et al. (1993) Platelet serotonin content and free and total plasma tryptophan in healthy volunteers during 24 hours. Clin Chem 39: 2337–2340. pmid:8222232
  61. 61. Hammond KA (1997) Adaptation of the maternal intestine during lactation. J Mamm Gland Biol Neoplas 2: 243–252.
  62. 62. Lowry CA, Hale MW, Evans AK, Heerkens J, Staub DR, et al. (2008) Serotonergic systems, anxiety, and affective disorder. Focus on the dorsomedial part of the dorsal raphe nucleus. Stress, Neurotransmitters, and Hormones: Ann NY Acad Sci 1148: 86–94. pmid:19120094
  63. 63. Kiyasova V, Fernandez SP, Laine J, Stankovski L, Muzerelle A, et al. (2011) A genetically defined morphologically and functionally unique subset of 5-HT neurons in the mouse raphe nuclei. J Neuroscience 31: 2756–2768.
  64. 64. Köhler C, Chan-Palay V, Steinbusch H (1982) The distribution and origin of serotonin-containing fibers in the septal area: a combined immunohistochemical and fluorescent retrograde tracing study in the rat. J Com Neurol 209: 91–111. pmid:6749914
  65. 65. Aggleton JP (1993) The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states. Trend Neurosciences 16: 328–333. pmid:7691009
  66. 66. Singewald GM, Rjabokon A, Singewald N, Ebner K (2011) The modulatory role of the lateral septum on neuroendocrine and behavioral stress responses. Neuropsychopharm 36: 793–804.
  67. 67. Bale TL, Davis AM, Auger AP, Dorsa DM, McCarthy MM (2001) CNS region-specific oxytocin receptor expression: importance in regulation of anxiety and sex behavior. J Neuroscience 21: 2546–2552. pmid:11264328
  68. 68. Bakowska JC, Morrell JI (1997) Atlas of the neurons that express mRNA for the long form of the prolactin receptor in the forebrain of the female rat. J Comp Neurol 386: 161–177. pmid:9295145
  69. 69. Insel TR, Hulihan TJ (1995) A gender-specific mechanism for pair bonding: oxytocin and partner preference formation in monogamous voles. Beh Neuroscience 109: 782–789. pmid:7576222
  70. 70. Bridges RS, Numan M, Ronsheim PM, Mann PE, Lupini CE (1990) Central prolactin infusions stimulate maternal behavior in steroid-treated nulliparous female rats. Proc Nat Acad Sci 87: 8003–8007. pmid:2236014
  71. 71. Arletti R, Bertolini A (1987) Oxytocin acts as an antidepressant in two animal models of depression. Life Sci 41: 1725–1530. pmid:3657379
  72. 72. McCarthy MM, McDonald CH, Brooks PJ, Goldman D (1996) An anxiolytic action of oxytocin is enhanced by estrogen in the mouse. Physiol Behav 60: 1209–1215. pmid:8916173
  73. 73. Neumann ID, Torner L, Wigger A (2000) Brain oxytocin: differential inhibition of neuroendocrine stress responses and anxiety-related behaviour in virgin, pregnant and lactating rats. Neuroscience 95: 567–575 pmid:10658637
  74. 74. Leng G, Caquineau C, Sabatier N (2005) Regulation of oxytocin secretion. Vit Horm 71: 27–58
  75. 75. Sofroniew MV (1980) Projections from vasopressin, oxytocin, and neurophysin neurons to neural targets in the rat and human. J Histochem Cytochem 28: 475–478. pmid:7381192
  76. 76. Fliers E, Guldenaar SE, van de Wal N, Swaab DF (1986) Extrahypothalamic vasopressin and oxytocin in the human brain; presence of vasopressin cells in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Brain Res 375: 363–367. pmid:3524745
  77. 77. Emiliano ABF, Cruz T, Pannoni V, Fudge JL (2007) The interface of oxytocin labeled cells and serotonin transporter-containing fibers in the primate hypothalamus: A Substrate for SSRIs therapeutic effects? Neuropsychopharmacol 32: 977–988.
  78. 78. Marazziti D, Baroni S, Giannaccini G, Betti L, Massimetti G, et al. (2012) A link between oxytocin and serotonin: supporting evidence from peripheral markers. Eur J Neuropsychopharmacol 22: 578–583. pmid:22297159
  79. 79. Torner L, Toschi N, Nava G, Clapp C, Neumann ID (2002) Increased hypothalamic expression of prolactin in lactation: involvement in behavioural and neuroendocrine stress response. Eur J Neuroscience 15: 1381–1389. pmid:11994132
  80. 80. Kordon C, Blake CA, Terkel J, Sawyer CH (1973) Participation of serotonin-containing neurons in the suckling-induced rise in plasma prolactin levels in lactating rats. Neuroendocrinol 13: 213–223.
  81. 81. Moos F, Richard P (1983) Serotonergic control of oxytocin release during suckling in the rat: opposite effects in conscious and anesthetized rats. Neuroendocrinol 36: 300–306.
  82. 82. Jørgensen H, Knigge U, Warberg J (1992) Effect of serotonin 5-HT1, 5-HT2, and 5-HT3 receptor antagonists on the prolactin response to restraint and ether stress. Neuroendocrinol 56: 371–377.
  83. 83. Cryan JF, Valentino RJ, Lucki I (2005) Assessing substrates underling the behavioral effects of antidepressants using the modified rat forced swimming test. Neuroscience Biobehav Rev 29: 547–569. pmid:15893822
  84. 84. Nicolas LB, Kolb Y, Prinssen EPM (2006) A combined marble burying—locomotor activity test in mice: A practical screening test with sensitivity to different classes of anxiolytics and antidepressants. Eur J Pharmacol 547: 106–115. pmid:16934246
  85. 85. Hebert MF (2013) Impact of pregnancy on pharmacokinetics of medications. J Pop Therap Clin Pharmacol 20: e350–e357.
  86. 86. Kittrell EMW, Satinoff E (1988) Diurnal rhythms of body temperature, drinking and activity over reproductive cycles. Physiol Beh 42: 477–484.
  87. 87. Scribner SJ, Wynne-Edwards KE (1994) Disruption of body temperature and behavior rhythms during reproduction in Dwarf hamsters (Phodopus). Physiology and Behavior 55: 361–369. pmid:8153179
  88. 88. Schrader JA, Walaszczyk EJ, Smale L (2009) Changing patterns of daily rhythmicity across reproductive states in diurnal female Nile grass rats (Arvicanthis niloticus). Physiol Beh 98: 547–556.
  89. 89. Misri S, Sinclair D, Kuan A (1997) Breast-feeding and postpartum depression: is there a relationship? Can J Psychiat 42: 1061–1065. pmid:9469238
  90. 90. Fergerson SS, Jamieson DJ, Lindsay M (2002) Diagnosing postpartum depression: can we do better? Am J Obstet Gynecol 186: 899–902. pmid:12015507
  91. 91. Alder EM, Cox JL (1983) Breast feeding and postnatal depression. J Psychosom Res 27: 139–144. pmid:6864598
  92. 92. Alder E, Bancroft J (1988) The relationship between breast feeding persistence, sexuality and mood in postpartum women. Psycholog Med 18: 389–396.
  93. 93. Dennis CL, McQueen K (2009) The relationship between infant-feeding outcomes and postpartum depression: a qualitative systematic review. Pediatrics 123: e736–e751. pmid:19336362
  94. 94. Pawluski JL, Charlier TD, Fillet M, Houbart V, Crispin HT, et al. (2012) Chronic fluoxetine treatment and maternal adversity differentially alter neurobehavioral outcomes in the rat dam. Beh Brain Res 228: 159–168. pmid:22173000
  95. 95. Dossett EC (2008) Perinatal Depression. Obstet Gynecol Clinics N Am 35: 419–434. pmid:18760228
  96. 96. Li YQ, Takada M, Matsuzaki S, Shinonaga Y, Mizuno N (1993) Identification of periaqueductal gray and dorsal raphe nucleus neurons projecting to both the trigeminal sensory complex and forebrain structures: a fluorescent retrograde double-labeling study in the rat. Brain Res 623:267–277. pmid:8221108
  97. 97. Kendall-Tackett K, Hale TS (2010) The use of antidepressants in pregnant and breastfeeding women: A review of recent studies. J Hum Lactation 26:187–195. pmid:19652194