Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Dopamine Receptors Antagonistically Regulate Behavioral Choice between Conflicting Alternatives in C. elegans

  • Daoyong Wang,

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Developmental Genes and Human Disease in Ministry of Education, Medical School of Southeast University, Nanjing, 210009, China

  • Yonglin Yu,

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Developmental Genes and Human Disease in Ministry of Education, Medical School of Southeast University, Nanjing, 210009, China

  • Yinxia Li,

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Developmental Genes and Human Disease in Ministry of Education, Medical School of Southeast University, Nanjing, 210009, China

  • Yang Wang,

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Developmental Genes and Human Disease in Ministry of Education, Medical School of Southeast University, Nanjing, 210009, China

  • Dayong Wang

    dayongw@seu.edu.cn

    Affiliation Key Laboratory of Developmental Genes and Human Disease in Ministry of Education, Medical School of Southeast University, Nanjing, 210009, China

Abstract

Caenorhabditis elegans is a useful model to study the neuronal or molecular basis for behavioral choice, a specific form of decision-making. Although it has been implied that both D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors may contribute to the control of decision-making in mammals, the genetic interactions between D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors in regulating decision-making are still largely unclear. In the present study, we investigated the molecular control of behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives (diacetyl and Cu2+) by D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors and their possible genetic interactions with C. elegans as the assay system. In the behavioral choice assay system, mutation of dop-1 gene encoding D1-like dopamine receptor resulted in the enhanced tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier compared with wild-type. In contrast, mutations of dop-2 or dop-3 gene encoding D2-like dopamine receptor caused the weak tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier compared with wild-type. During the control of behavioral choice, DOP-3 antagonistically regulated the function of DOP-1. The behavioral choice phenotype of dop-2; dop-1dop-3 triple mutant further confirmed the possible antagonistic function of D2-like dopamine receptor on D1-like dopamine receptor in regulating behavioral choice. The genetic assays further demonstrate that DOP-3 might act through Gαo signaling pathway encoded by GOA-1 and EGL-10, and DOP-1 might act through Gαq signaling pathway encoded by EGL-30 and EAT-16 to regulate the behavioral choice. DOP-1 might function in cholinergic neurons to regulate the behavioral choice, whereas DOP-3 might function in GABAergic neurons, RIC, and SIA neurons to regulate the behavioral choice. In this study, we provide the genetic evidence to indicate the antagonistic relationship between D1-like dopamine receptor and D2-like dopamine receptor in regulating the decision-making of animals. Our data will be useful for understanding the complex functions of dopamine receptors in regulating decision-making in animals.

Introduction

Dopamine regulates a variety of behavioral activities in both vertebrates and invertebrates. In mammals, dopamine can act through five receptors that are grouped into several classes. D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors usually have antagonistic effects on behaviors in mammals [1]. In Caenorhabditis elegans, there are four dopamine receptors: DOP-1, DOP-2, DOP-3, and DOP-4 [2]. DOP-1 and DOP-4 are D1-like dopamine receptors. DOP-2 and DOP-3 are D2-like dopamine receptors. DOP-4 is unique in invertebrate and distinct from mammalian D1-like dopamine receptors [3]. Previous study has demonstrated that DOP-1 and DOP-3 had antagonistic effects on basal slowing response in C. elegans [2]. DOP-1 and DOP-3 could further activate antagonistic Gαq and Gαo signaling pathways to regulate the basal slowing response [2].

C. elegans is a useful model system to study the neuronal or molecular basis for behaviors including decision-making [4]-[6]. In C. elegans, among the several forms of decision-making, behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives (diacetyl and Cu2+) can reflect the effects of multiple stimuli (attractant versus aversive stimuli) on behavioral plasticity in animals [7][9]. Nematodes normally show being attracted to attractants such as diacetyl which is sensed by AWA sensory neurons [10], but will avoid aversive cues such as Cu2+ ion which is sensed by ADL/ASH sensory neurons [11]. For the molecular mechanism of behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives, some signaling pathways have been raised to have important roles. HEN-1 (hesitation behavior) was first identified to play an important role in behavioral choice control in AIY interneurons [7]. HEN-1 and SCD-2 (suppressor of constitutive dauer formation), a target of FSN-1 (F-box synaptic protein), may function in the same genetic pathway to regulate behavioral choice [9], [12]. GCY-28 (guanylyl cylase)/CNG-1 (cyclic nucleotide gated channel) functioned in a parallel pathway in AIA interneurons with HEN-1/SCD-2 to regulate behavioral choice [9]. The insulin signaling pathway including daf-16, daf-2, and daf-18 was also shown to participate in the control of behavioral choice [13].

Decision-making is a complex cognitive process that is found to be impaired in a number of psychiatric diseases. Previous studies have demonstrated that both D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors may contribute to the decision-making impairments in human and animals [14][18]. However, the genetic interactions between D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors in regulating decision-making are still largely unclear. In the present study, we investigated the molecular control of behavioral choice by D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors and their possible genetic interactions with C. elegans as the in vivo assay system. Our data suggest the antagonistic functions between D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors in regulating the behavioral choice in nematodes. Our study will be useful for understanding the important function of dopamine signaling in regulating the decision-making in animals.

Results

Effects of mutations of genes encoding D1-like dopamine receptors on behavioral choice

In C. elegans, D1-like dopamine receptors contain DOP-1 and DOP-4. In the behavioral choice assay system (Fig. 1A), the dop-4 loss-of-function mutant (dop-4(ok1321)) showed the similar tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier to that of wild-type under both the well-fed and the starved conditions (Fig. 1B). In contrast, the dop-1 loss-of-function mutant (dop-1(vs100)) exhibited the enhanced tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier compared with wild-type under the well-fed conditions (Fig. 1B). In the assay system, when either diacetyl or Cu2+ ion was presented, index of dop-1(vs100) mutant was similar to that of wild-type animals (Fig. 1C and 1D), implying that the observed deficit in behavioral choice in dop-1 mutant is not due to the abnormality of chemotaxis to diacetyl or avoidance of Cu2+. However, when both Cu2+ ion and diacetyl were presented in the assay system, index of dop-1(vs100) mutant was higher than that of wild-type animals (Fig. 1C and 1D). Thus, the inhibition of Cu2+ avoidance by diacetyl may be stronger than the inhibition of diacetyl chemotaxis by Cu2+ ion in dop-1 mutants.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Effects of D1-like dopamine receptor on behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives.

(A) Assay model for the behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives. (B) Phenotypes of dop-1 and dop-4 mutants in the interaction assay under the well-fed or starved condition. In the assay system, the Cu2+ ion concentration was 100 mM, and the diacetyl concentration was 10−2. (C) Dose-response curves of wild-type N2 and dop-1 mutants to diacetyl with (+) or without (-) 100 mM of Cu2+ ion. Differences between groups were determined using two-way ANOVA. (D) Dose-response curves of wild-type N2 and dop-1 mutants to Cu2+ ion with (+) or without (-) 10−2 of diacetyl. Differences between groups were determined using two-way ANOVA. (E) Locomotion behavior of wild-type N2 and dop-1 mutants in the absence (-) of food under well-fed or starved condition. Locomotion behavior was assessed by the body bend. Bars represent mean ± S.E.M. **P <0.01 vs N2 (if not specially indicated).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.g001

Due to the sensation of starvation, starved wild-type nematodes show the higher index of behavioral choice than well-fed wild-type nematodes [7]. Like wild-type animals, dop-1(vs100) mutant changed their behavioral choice after starvation (Fig. 1B), demonstrating that the dop-1 mutant can sense the starvation like wild-type nematodes. In the behavioral choice assay system, if the examined nematodes are abnormal in locomotion behavior, the obtained index of behavioral choice may not be able to reflect the real ability of behavioral choice for the nematodes. In this study, we used the body bend to reflect the state of locomotion behavior of nematodes. Both the well-fed and the starved dop-1(vs100) mutants showed the similar body bends to those of wild-type in the NGM plates without food (Fig. 1E), suggesting the normal locomotion behavior of dop-1 mutant in the behavioral choice assay system. These data suggest that dop-1 mutant does have deficits in behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives.

Effects of mutations of genes encoding D2-like dopamine receptors on behavioral choice

In C. elegans, D2-like dopamine receptors contain DOP-2 and DOP-3. In the behavioral choice assay system, both dop-2 and dop-3 loss-of-function mutants (dop-2(vs105) and dop-3(vs106)) showed the weak tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier compared with wild-type under the well-fed conditions (Fig. 2A). In the assay system, index of dop-2(vs105) or dop-3(vs106) mutant was similar to that of wild-type animals when either diacetyl or Cu2+ ion was presented (Fig. 2B and 2C), implying that the observed deficit in behavioral choice in dop-2 or dop-3 mutant may be not due to the abnormality in diacetyl chemotaxis or Cu2+ avoidance. In the assay system, different from the dop-1(vs100) mutant, index of dop-2(vs105) or dop-3(vs106) mutant was lower than that of wild-type animals when both Cu2+ ion and diacetyl were presented in the assay system (Fig. 2B and 2C), implying that the inhibition of diacetyl chemotaxis by Cu2+ ion may be stronger than the inhibition of Cu2+ avoidance by diacetyl in dop-2 and dop-3 mutants.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Effects of D2-like dopamine receptor on behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives.

(A) Phenotypes of dop-2 and dop-3 mutants in the interaction assay under the well-fed or starved condition. In the assay system, the Cu2+ ion concentration was 100 mM, and the diacetyl concentration was 10−2. (B) Dose-response curves of wild-type N2 and mutants to diacetyl with (+) or without (-) 100 mM of Cu2+ ion. Differences between groups were determined using two-way ANOVA. (C) Dose-response curves of wild-type N2 and mutants to Cu2+ ion with (+) or without (-) 10−2 of diacetyl. Differences between groups were determined using two-way ANOVA. (D) Locomotion behavior of wild-type N2 and mutants in the absence (-) of food under the well-fed or starved condition. Locomotion behavior was assessed by the body bend. Bars represent mean ± S.E.M. **P <0.01 vs N2 (if not specially indicated).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.g002

dop-2(vs105) and dop-3(vs106) mutants could change their behavioral choice after starvation (Fig. 2A), demonstrating that the dop-2 and dop-3 mutants can sense the starvation like wild-type nematodes. The dop-2(vs105) and dop-3(vs106) mutants also had the normal locomotion behavior in the absence of food under the well-fed or starved condition (Fig. 2D), implying that the observed deficit in behavioral choice in dop-2 or dop-3 mutant may be not due to the abnormality in locomotion behavior in the behavioral choice assay system. Thus, both DOP-2 and DOP-3 participate in the control of behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives in nematodes.

Genetic interactions of D1-like dopamine receptor and D2-like dopamine receptor in regulating behavioral choice

We next examined the genetic interactions between D1-like dopamine receptor and D2-like dopamine receptor in regulating behavioral choice in C. elegans. The double mutant of dop-2(vs105); dop-3(vs106) showed the similar behavioral choice phenotype to that of dop-2(vs105) or dop-3(vs106) mutant (Fig. 3A), implying that these two D2-like dopamine receptor genes may function in the same genetic pathway to regulate the behavioral choice.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Genetic interactions of D1-like dopamine receptor with D2-like dopamine receptors in regulating behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives.

(A) Genetic interaction between DOP-2 and DOP-3 in regulating behavioral choice. (B) Genetic interaction between DOP-1 and DOP-2 in regulating behavioral choice. (C) Genetic interaction between DOP-1 and DOP-3 in regulating behavioral choice. (D) The behavioral choice phenotype of dop-2; dop-1dop-3 triple mutant. The behavioral choice was examined under the well-fed condition. In the assay system, the Cu2+ ion concentration was 100 mM, and the diacetyl concentration was 10−2. Bars represent mean ± S.E.M. **P<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.g003

The double mutant of dop-2(vs105); dop-1(vs100) showed the similar behavioral choice phenotype to that of dop-1(vs100) mutant (Fig. 3B). Different from the behavioral choice phenotype of dop-2(vs105); dop-1(vs100) mutant, the dop-1(vs100)dop-3(vs106) exhibited the similar behavioral choice phenotype to that of dop-3(vs106) mutant (Fig. 3C). These data suggest that the dop-3 mutation, but not the dop-2 mutation, can reverse the functions of dop-1 mutation in regulating behavioral choice.

The triple mutant of dop-2(vs105); dop-1(vs100)dop-3(vs106) showed the similar behavioral choice phenotype to that in dop-2(vs105) or dop-3(vs106) mutant, and not exhibited the similar behavioral choice phenotype to that in dop-1(vs100) mutant (Fig. 3D). These data imply that, in the behavioral choice control of nematodes without functions of dopamine receptors, the effects from mutation of genes encoding D1-like dopamine receptors may be suppressed by the mutation of genes encoding D2-like dopamine receptors.

Effects of mutations of genes encoding Gαq signaling pathway on behavioral choice

Previous study has suggested that D1-like dopamine receptors can activate Gαq signaling pathway in cells [2]. In C. elegans, the Gαq signaling pathway contains egl-30, eat-16, egl-8, and gpb-2 genes. In the behavioral choice assay system, the egl-8(md197) and gpb-2(sa603) mutants had the similar tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier to that of wild-type under both the well-fed and the starved conditions (Fig. 4A). In contrast, the egl-30(n686) and eat-16(ad702) mutants showed the enhanced tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier compared with wild-type under both the well-fed and the starved conditions (Fig. 4A). In the assay system, index of egl-30(n686) or eat-16(ad702) mutant was similar to that of wild-type animals when either diacetyl or Cu2+ ion was presented (Fig. 4B and 4C), implying that the observed deficit in behavioral choice in egl-30 or eat-16 mutant may be not due to the abnormality in diacetyl chemotaxis or Cu2+ avoidance. In the assay system, index of egl-30(n686) or eat-16(ad702)) mutant was higher than that of wild-type animals when both Cu2+ ion and diacetyl were presented in the assay system (Fig. 4B and 4C), suggesting that the inhibition of Cu2+ avoidance by diacetyl may be stronger than the inhibition of diacetyl chemotaxis by Cu2+ ion in egl-30 and eat-16 mutants. Both egl-30(n686) and eat-16(ad702) mutants were normal in the sensation of starvation (Fig. 4A), and showed the normal locomotion behavior in the absence of food under well-fed or starved condition (Fig. 4D). These results suggest that, among the members of Gαq signaling pathway, EGL-30 and EAT-16 regulate the behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives in nematodes.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Roles of Gαq signaling pathway in regulating behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives.

(A) Phenotypes of Gαq signaling pathway mutants in the interaction assay under the well-fed or starved condition. In the assay system, the Cu2+ ion concentration was 100 mM, and the diacetyl concentration was 10−2. (B) Dose-response curves of wild-type N2 and mutants to diacetyl with (+) or without (-) 100 mM of Cu2+ ion. Differences between groups were determined using two-way ANOVA. (C) Dose-response curves of wild-type N2 and mutants to Cu2+ ion with (+) or without (-) 10−2 of diacetyl. Differences between groups were determined using two-way ANOVA. (D) Locomotion behavior of wild-type N2 and mutants in the absence (-) of food under the well-fed or starved condition. Locomotion behavior was assessed by the body bend. Bars represent mean ± S.E.M. **P<0.01 vs N2 (if not specially indicated).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.g004

Effects of mutations of genes encoding Gαo signaling pathway on behavioral choice

Previous study has suggested that D2-like dopamine receptors can activate Gαo signaling pathway in cells [2]. In C. elegans, besides the gpb-2 gene, the Gαo signaling pathway contains goa-1, egl-10, and dgk-1 genes. In the behavioral choice assay system, the dgk-1(sy428) and gpb-2(sa603) mutants exhibited the similar tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier to that of wild-type under both the well-fed and the starved conditions (Fig. 5A). In contrast, the goa-1(sa723) mutant showed the enhanced tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier, and the egl-10(md176) mutant exhibited the weak tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier compared with wild-type under both the well-fed and the starved conditions (Fig. 5A). In the assay system, indexes of goa-1(sa723) and egl-10(md176) mutants were similar to that of wild-type animals when either diacetyl or Cu2+ ion was presented (Fig. 5B and 5C), implying that the observed deficit in behavioral choice in goa-1 or egl-10 mutant may be not due to the abnormality in diacetyl chemotaxis or Cu2+ avoidance. In the assay system, the index of goa-1(sa723) mutant was higher than that of wild-type animals and the index of egl-10(md176) mutant was lower than that of wild-type animals when both Cu2+ ion and diacetyl were presented in the assay system (Fig. 5B and 5C). That is, the inhibition of Cu2+ avoidance by diacetyl may be stronger than the inhibition of diacetyl chemotaxis by Cu2+ ion in goa-1 mutant, and the inhibition of diacetyl chemotaxis by Cu2+ ion may be stronger than the inhibition of Cu2+ avoidance by diacetyl in egl-10 mutant. Both goa-1(sa723) and egl-10(md176) mutants could normally sense the starvation (Fig. 5A), and showed the normal locomotion behavior in the absence of food under the well-fed or the starved condition (Fig. 5D). Therefore, among the members of Gαo signaling pathway, GOA-1 and EGL-10 are involved in the control of behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives in nematodes.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Roles of Gαo signaling pathway in regulating behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives.

(A) Phenotypes of Gαo signaling pathway mutants in the interaction assay under the well-fed or starved condition. In the assay system, the Cu2+ ion concentration was 100 mM, and the diacetyl concentration was 10−2. (B) Dose-response curves of wild-type N2 and mutants to diacetyl with (+) or without (-) 100 mM of Cu2+ ion. Differences between groups were determined using two-way ANOVA. (C) Dose-response curves of wild-type N2 and mutants to Cu2+ ion with (+) or without (-) 10−2 of diacetyl. Differences between groups were determined using two-way ANOVA. (D) Locomotion behavior of wild-type N2 and mutants in the absence (-) of food under the well-fed or starved condition. Locomotion behavior was assessed by the body bend. Bars represent mean ± S.E.M. **P<0.01 vs N2 (if not specially indicated).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.g005

Genetic interactions of DOP-1 with EGL-30 or EAT-16 in regulating behavioral choice

We next examined the genetic interactions of dop-1 gene with egl-30 or eat-16 gene in regulating the behavioral choice. In the behavioral choice assay system, all of the dop-1(vs100), egl-30(n686), and eat-16(ad702) mutants had the enhanced tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier. The behavioral choice phenotype of egl-30(n686); dop-1(vs100) double mutant was similar to that in dop-1(vs100) or egl-30(n686) mutant (Fig. 6A). Moreover, the behavioral choice phenotype of eat-16(ad702); dop-1(vs100) double mutant was similar to that in dop-1(vs100) or eat-16(ad702) mutant (Fig. 6A). Therefore, DOP-1 may act in the same genetic pathway with EGL-30 and EAT-16 to regulate the behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives in nematodes.

thumbnail
Figure 6. Genetic interactions of dopamine receptors with G-protein signaling pathways in regulating behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives.

(A) Genetic interactions of DOP-1 with Gαq signaling pathway in regulating behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives. (B) Genetic interactions of DOP-3 with Gαo signaling pathway in regulating behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives. The behavioral choice was examined under the well-fed condition. In the assay system, the Cu2+ ion concentration was 100 mM, and the diacetyl concentration was 10−2. Bars represent mean ± S.E.M. **P<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.g006

Genetic interactions of DOP-3 with GOA-1 or EGL-10 in regulating behavioral choice

We further examined the genetic interactions of dop-3 gene with goa-1 or egl-10 gene in regulating the behavioral choice. In the behavioral choice assay system, the dop-3(vs106) and egl-10(md176) mutants had the weak tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier, and the goa-1(sa734) mutant had the enhanced tendency to cross the Cu2+ barrier. The behavioral choice phenotype of goa-1(sa734); dop-3(vs106) double mutant was similar to that in goa-1(sa734) mutant (Fig. 6B). That is, mutation of goa-1 gene could reverse the behavioral choice phenotype caused by mutation of dop-3 gene. Moreover, the behavioral choice phenotype of egl-10(md176); dop-3(vs106) double mutant was similar to that in dop-3(vs106) or egl-10(md176) mutant (Fig. 6B). Therefore, DOP-3 may act in the same genetic pathway with GOA-1 and EGL-10 to regulate behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives in nematodes.

Neuronal circuit of DOP-1 or DOP-3 in regulating behavioral choice

In C. elegans, DOP-1 was expressed in sheath cells, and some neurons in the head including RIS interneuron [2], [19], and DOP-3 was expressed in some neurons in the head including ASE, SIA, and RIC neurons, mechanosensory neurons, and body-wall muscles [2], [20]-[21]. DOP-1 was specially expressed in the cholinergic neurons, whereas DOP-3 was strongly expressed in GABAergic neurons [2]. Both DOP-1 and DOP-3 were not expressed in command interneurons that affect the locomotion behavior [2]. The expression of DOP-1 was not overlapped with that of DOP-3 [2]. Our results showed that, in dop-1(vs100) mutant, expression of DOP-1 in cholinergic neurons could rescue its deficit in behavioral choice (Fig. 7A). In contrast, expression of DOP-1 in sheath cells did not rescue the deficit in behavioral choice of dop-1(vs100) mutant, and expression of DOP-1 in RIS neurons could only moderately rescue the deficit in behavioral choice of dop-1(vs100) mutant (Fig. 7A). Moreover, in dop-3(vs106) mutant, expression of DOP-3 in GABAergic neurons could rescue its deficit in behavioral choice (Fig. 7B). Interestingly, expression of DOP-3 in RIC or SIA neurons could also rescue the deficit in behavioral choice of dop-3(vs106) mutant (Fig. 7B). In contrast, expression of DOP-3 in ASE neurons could only moderately rescue the deficit in behavioral choice of dop-3(vs106) mutant (Fig. 7B).

thumbnail
Figure 7. Neuron-specific activity of DOP-1 or DOP-3 in regulating behavioral choice.

(A) Neuron-specific activity of DOP-1 in regulating behavioral choice. (B) Neuron-specific activity of DOP-3 in regulating behavioral choice. The behavioral choice was examined under the well-fed condition. In the assay system, the Cu2+ ion concentration was 100 mM, and the diacetyl concentration was 10−2. In the rescue experiments, hlh-17 promoter was used for glia-specific expression, unc-47 promoter was used for GABAergic neurons-specific expression, acr-2 promoter was used for cholinergic neuron-specific expression, gcy-28.d promoter was used for AIA-specific expression, ttx-3 promoter was used for AIY-specific expression, tbh-1 promoter was used for RIC-specific expression, gcy-7 promoter was used for ASE-specific expression, sro-1 promoter was used for expression in SIA neurons, and lim-6 promoter was used for expression in RIS neurons. Bars represent mean ± S.E.M. **P<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.g007

Besides these, we found that expression of DOP-1 or DOP-3 in AIY interneurons did not rescue the deficit in behavioral behavior in the corresponding mutant (Fig. 7). In contrast, expression of DOP-1 or DOP-3 in AIA interneurons could only moderately rescue the deficit in behavioral behavior in the corresponding mutant (Fig. 7). These results suggest that both DOP-1 and DOP-3 may not function in AIY or AIA interneurons to regulate the behavioral choice.

Discussion

Previous studies have demonstrated that dopamine receptors can regulate several behaviors including the locomotion, food response, enhancement of odor avoidance, mating behavior, and plasticity of mechanosensory response in C. elegans [22][26]. In the present study, we provide the evidence to further prove the crucial role of both D1-like dopamine receptor (DOP-1) and D2-like dopamine receptors (DOP-2 and DOP-3) in the control of behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives (diacetyl and Cu2+). DOP-2 and DOP-3 may function in the same genetic pathway to regulate behavioral choice. The observed deficits in behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives in dop-1, dop-2, and dop-3 loss-of-function mutants were not due to the abnormality in sensation of starvation, and locomotion behavior. Our results are consistent to a certain degree with the observed functions of D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors in regulating decision-making of mammalian animals. In mammals, D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors may play the prominent roles in regulating decision-making. In contrast, D3-like or D4-like dopamine receptors might only participate in the control of specific forms of decision-making in mammals [27][29]. In C. elegans, previous studies have further suggested that dopamine receptors can regulate the behavioral plasticity [22][23]. Therefore, D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors can regulate the two steps of informational processing including both the learning and the integration of sensory signals of nematodes.

In the assay system for behavioral choice, we used two stimuli, diacetyl and Cu2+. In C. elegans, AWA sensory neurons are involved in the sensation of diacetyl, and the ASH and ADL sensory neurons are involved in the sensation of Cu2+ ion [10][11]. Moreover, some neurotransmitters, including neuropeptide, serotonin, tyramine and octopamine, have been suggested to modulate the ASH-mediated aversive behaviors [30][33]. However, our data suggest that mutations of the examined dopamine receptors had the normal chemotaxis to diacetyl and avoidance of Cu2+, suggesting that the examined dopamine receptors may be not involved in the control of the chemotaxis to diacetyl or the avoidance of Cu2+.

Our data further demonstrate that D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors antagonistically regulate the behavioral choice. The D2-like dopamine receptor DOP-3 could antagonistically regulate the function of D1-like dopamine receptor DOP-1 in regulating the behavioral choice. The behavioral choice phenotype of dop-2(vs105); dop-1(vs100)dop-3(vs106) triple mutant further confirmed the important antagonistic function of D2-like dopamine receptor on D1-like dopamine receptor in regulating the behavioral choice in nematodes. It has been implied that both D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors may have antagonistic effects on decision-making in mammals. In rats, based on pharmacological manipulations, blockage of D1-like receptors in prefrontal cortex decreased preference for the risky decision-making; whereas blockage of D2-like receptors in prefrontal cortex increased the risky decision-making [15]. In the present study, we provide the direct genetic evidence to indicate the antagonistic relationship between D1-like dopamine receptor and D2-like dopamine receptor in regulating the decision-making of animals. Previous study has also demonstrated that mutation of dop-1 gene reversed the deficit in basal slowing response of the dop-3 mutant nematodes [2]. Therefore, the relationship between D1-like dopamine receptor and D2-like dopamine receptor in regulating the functions of nervous system of nematodes may be very complex.

Our data in the present study suggest that the dopamine signaling is involved in the control of behavioral choice in nematodes. However, the behavioral choice in C. elegans may be not solely dependent on the dopamine signaling. Other specific neurotransmitter signaling pathways may be also involved in the control of behavioral choice in nematodes. For example, it has been shown that mutations of glc-3 gene encoding a L-glutamate-gated chloride channel caused more nematodes to cross the Cu2+ barrier in the behavioral choice assay system [9].

Considering the fact that D1-like dopamine receptors can activate the Gαq signaling pathway, and D2-like dopamine receptors can activate the Gαo signaling pathway in cells of nematodes [2], we examined the roles of genes encoding Gαq signaling pathway or Gαo signaling pathway in regulating the behavioral choice. Our data suggest that, in the Gαq signaling pathway, EGL-30 and EAT-16 regulated the behavioral choice of nematodes. In C. elegans, egl-30 encodes the Gαq, and eat-16 encodes the regulator of G protein signaling (RGS) protein, a GTPase activating protein. In the Gαo signaling pathway, GOA-1 and EGL-10 regulated the behavioral choice of nematodes. In C. elegans, goa-1 encodes the Gαo, and egl-10 also encodes an RGS protein. These results suggest that both the Gαq signaling pathway and the Gαo signaling pathway are involved in the control of behavioral choice in nematodes.

Moreover, the genetic assay indicate that DOP-1 could function in the same genetic pathway with EGL-30 and EAT-16 in regulating the behavioral choice, and DOP-3 could function in the same genetic pathway with GOA-1 and EGL-10 in regulating the behavioral choice. In this study, we hypothesize that D2-like dopamine receptor/DOP-3 acting through the Gαo signaling pathway encoded by GOA-1 and EGL-10 may antagonize the function of D1-like dopamine receptor/DOP-1 acting through the Gαq signaling pathway encoded by EGL-30 and EAT-16 to regulate the behavioral choice in nematodes (Fig. 8).

thumbnail
Figure 8. Model for dopamine receptors in regulating behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives in nematodes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.g008

C. elegans is very suitable for understanding the neural signaling because of its simple and well-described nervous system [34]. Previous study has suggested that dopamine can counteract octopamine signaling in a neural circuit to regulate the food response [21]. Our work further revealed the neural circuit for D1-like dopamine receptor/DOP-1 or D2-like dopamine receptor/DOP-3 in regulating the behavioral choice in nematodes. Our data suggest that DOP-1 may function in cholinergic neurons to regulate the behavioral choice, and DOP-3 may function in GABAergic neurons to regulate the behavioral choice (Fig. 7). Nevertheless, the exact cholinergic or GABAergic neuron(s) in which DOP-1 or DOP-3 may function to regulate the behavioral choice is still unclear. Moreover, DOP-3 could further function in RIC or SIA neurons to regulate the behavioral choice (Fig. 7B). That is, DOP-1 and DOP-3 may function in different neurons to exert antagonistic effects on behavioral choice of nematodes (Fig. 8). Previous study has suggested that EGL-10, EAT-16, GOA-1, and EGL-30 can modulate the response of ASH sensory neurons to repellents in C. elegans [35]. Our results imply that DOP-1 and DOP-3 may only indirectly influence the function of ASH sensory neurons in regulating the response to repellents in nematodes. Previous study has also demonstrated that although DOP-3 is not expressed in ASH sensory neurons, dop-3 RNAi knockdown in ASH casued the octanol hypersensitivity [36]. Besides these, previous study has demonstrated that adenosine A2A receptor antagonism can attenuate the effects from dopamine D2 antagonism on decision-making in mammals [16]. The possible interactions of dopamine signaling with other signaling pathways in regulating the behavioral choice are also needed to be further investigated.

In conclusion, in the present study, our data suggest that both D1-like and D2-like dopamine receptors can modulate the behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives (diacetyl and Cu2+). During the behavioral choice control, D2-like dopamine receptor DOP-3 could antagonistically regulate the function of D1-like dopamine receptor DOP-1 in nematodes. The behavioral choice phenotype of dop-2; dop-1dop-3 triple mutant was similar to that in the single mutant of dop-2 or dop-3. In nematodes, DOP-3 might act through the Gαo signaling pathway encoded by GOA-1 and EGL-10, and DOP-1 might act through the Gαq signaling pathway encoded by EGL-30 and EAT-16 to regulate the behavioral choice. Therefore, we here provide the direct genetic evidence to indicate the antagonistic relationship between D1-like dopamine receptor and D2-like dopamine receptor in regulating the decision-making of animals. In human beings, defects in dopamine signaling may underlie the neuronal development related diseases such as schizophrenia and autism [37][39]. Our data will be helpful for understanding the complex functions of dopamine receptors in regulating behaviors such as decision-making in animals.

Materials and Methods

Strains and genetics

The strains of wild-type Bristol N2, dop-1(vs100)X, dop-2(vs105)V, dop-3(vs106)X, dop-4(ok1321)X, egl-30(n686)I, eat-16(ad702)I, egl-8(md1971)V, gpb-2(sa603)I, goa-1(sa734)I, egl-10(md176)V, and dgk-1(sy428)X were originally obtained from the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center (funded by the NIH National Center for Research Resource, USA). dop-1(vs100), dop-2(vs105), dop-3(vs106), and dop-4(ok1321) mutants are all animals with deletion mutations [2]. Nematodes were grown on nematode growth medium (NGM) plates seeded with Escherichia coli OP50 at 22°C as previously described [40]. Double mutant strains without additional marker mutations were constructed using standard genetic methods and verified by complementation testing.

Behavioral assays

Body bend was counted for 1 min. A body bend was defined as a change in the direction of the part of the nematodes corresponding to the posterior bulb of the pharynx along the y axis, assuming that the nematode was traveling along the x axis. Fifty nematodes were examined per treatment, and five replicates were performed.

The method for assay for behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives (an olfactory (diacetyl) and a gustatory (metal ion) stimuli) was performed as basically described [7], [9], [12]. Twenty-five microliters of copper acetate solution was spread on the middle of the 9-cm assay plates (10 mM HEPES [Ph 7.0], 1 mM MgSO4, 1 mM CaCl2, 50 mM NaCl, and 2% agar), which were placed at room temperature for 20-h to allow the diffusion before assay. Fifty nematodes were placed on one side of the metal barrier on the assay plate, and 2 µL diluted diacetyl (10−4–10−2) was spotted on the other side. After 90 min, the numbers of nematodes on the original side [B] and on the odorant side [A] were scored. The index was calculated as A/(A+B) ×100(%). The behavioral changes after starvation was analyzed using young adults starved on NGM plates without bacteria for 5-h. Ten replicates were performed.

DNA constructs and germline transformation

To generate entry vectors carrying promoter sequences, the promoter regions were amplified by PCR from C. elegans genomic DNA (2.0 kb for hlh-17 promoter used for glia-specific expression, 0.9 kb for unc-47 promoter used for GABAergic neurons-specific expression, 3.2 kb for acr-2 promoter used for cholinergic neuron-specific expression, 2.8 kb for gcy-28.d promoter used for AIA-specific expression, 0.9 kb for ttx-3 promoter used for AIY-specific expression, 3.0 kb for tbh-1 promoter used for RIC-specific expression, 1.4 kb for gcy-7 promoter used for ASE-specific expression, 2.3 kb for sro-1 promoter used for expression in SIA neurons, and 1.2 for lim-6 promoter used for expression in RIS neurons. The designed promoter primers were shown in S1 Table. And then these promoters were inserted into the pPD95_77 vector in the sense orientation. dop-1 (the isoform of F15A8.5a), or dop-3 (the isoform of T14E8.3b) cDNA was amplified by PCR. The sequences of the amplified cDNA were verified by sequencing, and then the cDNA was inserted into corresponding entry vectors carrying the appropriate promoter sequence. Germline transformation was performed as described [41] by coinjecting the testing DNA at a concentration of 10–40 µg/mL and the marker DNA of Pdop-1::rfp at a concentration of 60 µg/mL into the gonad of nematodes.

Statistical analysis

All data in this article were expressed as means ± standard error of the mean (S.E.M.). Graphs were generated using Microsoft Excel (Microsoft Corp., Redmond, WA). Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 12.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, USA). Differences between groups were determined using analysis of variance (ANOVA). If not specifically indicated, the differences between groups were determined using one-way ANOVA. The probability levels of 0.05 and 0.01 were considered statistically significant.

Supporting Information

S1 Table.

Information for the designed promoter primers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115985.s001

(DOC)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: Dayong Wang. Performed the experiments: Daoyong Wang YY YL YW. Analyzed the data: Daoyong Wang YL YW. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: Dayong Wang. Wrote the paper: Dayong Wang.

References

  1. 1. Gong W, Neill DB, Lynn M, Justice JB Jr (1999) Dopamine D1/D2 agonists injected into nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum differentially affect locomotor activity depending on site. Neuroscience 93:1349–1358.
  2. 2. Chase DL, Pepper JS, Koelle MR (2004) Mechanism of extrasynaptic dopamine signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Neurosci 7:1096–1103.
  3. 3. Sugiura M, Fuke S, Suo S, Sasagawa N, Van Tol HHM, et al. (2005) Characterization of a novel D2-like dopamine receptor with a truncated splice variant and a D1-like dopamine receptor unique to invertebrate from Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurochem 94:1146–1157.
  4. 4. Bendesky A, Tsunozaki M, Rockman MV, Kruglyak L, Bargmann CI (2011) Catecholamine receptor polymorphisms affect decision-making in C. elegans. Nature 472:313–318.
  5. 5. Li Z, Li Y, Yi Y, Huang W, Yang S, et al. (2012) Dissecting a central flip-flop circuit that integrates contradictory sensory cues in C. elegans feeding regulation. Nat Commun 3:776.
  6. 6. Faumont S, Lindsay TH, Lockery SR (2012) Neuronal microcircuits for decision making in C. elegans. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22:580–591.
  7. 7. Ishihara T, Iino Y, Mohri A, Mori I, Gengyo-Ando K, et al. (2002) HEN-1, a secretory protein with an LDL receptor motif, regulates sensory integration and learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Cell 109:639–649.
  8. 8. Ye H-Y, Ye B-P, Wang D-Y (2006) Learning and learning choice in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Neurosci Bull 22:355–360.
  9. 9. Shinkai Y, Yamamoto Y, Fujiwara M, Tabata T, Murayama T, et al. (2011) Behavioral choice between conflicting alternatives is regulated by a receptor guanyly cyclase, GCY-28, and a receptor tyrosine kinase, SCD-2, in AIA interneurons of Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci 31:3007–3015.
  10. 10. Bargmann CI, Hartwieg E, Horvitz HR (1993) Odorant-selective genes and neurons mediate olfaction in C. elegans. Cell 74:515–527.
  11. 11. Sambongi Y, Nagae T, Liu Y, Yoshimizu T, Takeda K, et al. (1999) Sensing of cadmium and copper ions by externally exposed ADL, ASE, and ASH neurons elicits avoidance response in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neuroreport 10:753–757.
  12. 12. Li Y-X, Wang Y, Hu Y-O, Zhong J-X, Wang D-Y (2011) Modulation of the assay system for the sensory integration of 2 sensory stimuli that inhibit each other in nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Neurosci Bull 27:69–82.
  13. 13. Jiu Y, Yue Y, Yang S, Liu L, Yu J, et al. (2010) Insulin-like signaling pathway functions in integrative response to an olfactory and a gustatory stimuli in Caenorhabditis elegans. Protein Cell 1:75–81.
  14. 14. Simon NW, Montgomery KS, Beas BS, Mitchell MR, LaSarge CL, et al. (2011) Dopaminergic modulation of risky decision-making. J Neurosci 31:17460–17470.
  15. 15. St OngeJR, Abhari H, Floresco SB (2011) Dissociable contributions by prefrontal D1 and D2 receptors to risk-based decision making. J Neurosci 31:8625–8633.
  16. 16. Pardo M, Lopez-Cruz L, Valverde O, Ledent C, Baqi Y, et al. (2012) Adenosine A2A receptor antagonism and genetic deletion attenuate the effects of dopamine D2 antagonism on effort-based decision making in mice. Neuropharmacology 62:2068–2077.
  17. 17. Paine TA, Asinof SK, Diehl GW, Frackman A, Leffler J (2013) Medial prefrontal cortex lesions impair decision-making on a rodent gambling task: reversal by D1 receptor antagonist administration. Behav Brain Res 243:247–254.
  18. 18. Reuter M, Felten A, Penz S, Mainzer A, Markett S, et al. (2013) The influence of dopaminergic gene variants on decision making in the ultimatum game. Front Hum Neurosci 7:242.
  19. 19. Tsalik EL, Niacaris T, Wenick AS, Pau K, Avery L, et al. (2003) LIM homeobox gene-dependent expression of biogenic amine receptors in restricted regions of the C. elegans nervous system. Dev Biol 263:81–102.
  20. 20. Etchberger JF, Lorch A, Sleumer MC, Zapf R, Jones SJ, et al. (2007) The molecular signature and cis-regulatory architecture of a C. elegans gustatory neuron. 21:1653–1674.
  21. 21. Suo S, Culotti JG, Van Tol HHM (2009) Dopamine counteracts octopamine signaling in a neural circuit mediating food response in C. elegans. EMBO J 28:2437–2448.
  22. 22. Sanyal S, Wintle RF, Kindt KS, Nuttley WM, Arvan R, et al. (2004) Dopamine modulates the plasticity of mechanosensory responses in Caenorhabditis elegans. EMBO J 23:473–482.
  23. 23. Kindt KS, Quast KB, Giles AC, De S, Hendrey D, et al. (2007) Dopamine mediates context-dependent modulation of sensory plasticity in C. elegans. Neuron 55:662–676.
  24. 24. Kimura KD, Fujita K, Katsura I (2010) Enhancement of odor avoidance regulated by dopamine signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci 30:16365–16375.
  25. 25. Correa P, LeBoeuf B, Garcia LR (2012) C. elegans dopaminergic D2-like receptors delimit recurrent cholinergic-mediated motor programs during a goal-oriented behavior. PLoS Genet 8:e1003015.
  26. 26. Omura DT, Clark DA, Samuel ADT, Horvitz HR (2012) Dopamine signaling is essential for precise rates of locomotion by C. elegans. PLoS ONE 7:e38649.
  27. 27. St OngeJR, Floresco SB (2009) Dopaminergic modulation of risk-based decision making. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:681–697.
  28. 28. Bardgett ME, Depenbrock M, Downs N, Points M, Green L (2009) Dopamine modulates effort-based decision making in rats. Behav Neurosci 123:242–251.
  29. 29. Ha RY, Namkoong K, Kang JI, Kim YT, Kim SJ (2009) Interaction between serotonin transporter promoter and dopamine receptor D4 polymorphisms on decision making. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 33:1217–1222.
  30. 30. Flavell SW, Pokala N, Macosko EZ, Albrecht DR, Larsch J, et al. (2013) Serotonin and the neuropeptide PDF initiate and extend opposing behavioral states in C. elegans. Cell 154:1023–1035.
  31. 31. Harris G, Mills H, Wraqq R, Hapiak V, Castelletto M, et al. (2010) The monoaminergic modulation of sensory-mediated aversive responses in Caenorhabditis elegans requires glutamatergic/peptidergic cotransmission. J Neurosci 30:7889–7899.
  32. 32. Mills H, Wraqq R, Hapiak V, Castelletto M, Zahratka J, et al. (2012) Monoamines and neuropeptides interact to inhibit aversive behaviour in Caenorhabditis elegans. EMBO J 31:667–678.
  33. 33. Wragg RT, Hapiak V, Miller SB, Harris GP, Gray J, et al. (2007) Tyramine and octopamine independently inhibit serotonin-stimulated aversive behaviors in Caenorhabditis elegans through two novel amine receptors. J Neurosci 27:13402–13412.
  34. 34. White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN, Brenner S (1986) The structure of the nervous system of the nematodes C. elegans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 314:1–340.
  35. 35. Esposito G, Amoroso MR, Bergamasco C, Di Schiavi E, Bazzicalupo P (2010) The G protein regulators EGL-10 and EAT-16, the Giα GOA-1 and the Gqα EGL-30 modulate the response of the C. elegans ASH polymodal nociceptive sensory neurons to repellents. BMC Biol 8:138.
  36. 36. Ezak MJ, Ferkey DM (2010) The C. elegans D2-like dopamine receptor DOP-3 decreases behavioral sensitivity to the olfactory stimulus 1-octanol. PLoS ONE 5:e9487.
  37. 37. Comings DE, Comings BG, Muhleman D, Dietz G, Shahbahrami B, et al. (1991) The dopamine D2 receptor locus as a modifying gene in neuropsychiatric disorders. JAMA 266:1793–1800.
  38. 38. Knable MB, Weinberger DR (1997) Dopamine, the prefrontal cortex and schizophrenia. J Psychopharmacol 11:123–131.
  39. 39. De Krom M, Staal WG, Ophoff RA, Hendriks J, Buitelaar J, et al. (2009) A common variant in DRD3 receptor is associated with autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry 65:625–630.
  40. 40. Brenner S (1974) The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 77:71–94.
  41. 41. Mello C, Fire A (1995) DNA transformation. Methods Cell Biol 48:451–482.