Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The Increased Risk of Road Crashes in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Adult Drivers: Driven by Distraction? Results from a Responsibility Case-Control Study

  • Kamal El Farouki ,

    kamalef@gmail.com

    Affiliations ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France, INSERM, Equipe Prévention et prise en charge des traumatismes, ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, Bordeaux, France

  • Emmanuel Lagarde,

    Affiliations ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France, INSERM, Equipe Prévention et prise en charge des traumatismes, ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, Bordeaux, France

  • Ludivine Orriols,

    Affiliations ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France, INSERM, Equipe Prévention et prise en charge des traumatismes, ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, Bordeaux, France

  • Manuel-Pierre Bouvard,

    Affiliation Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Charles Perrens Hospital, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France

  • Benjamin Contrand,

    Affiliations ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France, INSERM, Equipe Prévention et prise en charge des traumatismes, ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, Bordeaux, France

  • Cédric Galéra

    Affiliations ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France, INSERM, Equipe Prévention et prise en charge des traumatismes, ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, Bordeaux, France, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Charles Perrens Hospital, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France

Abstract

Background and Objective

Both distractions (external and internal) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are serious risk factors for traffic crashes and injuries. However, it is still unknown if ADHD (a chronic condition) modifies the effect of distractions (irregular hazards) on traffic crashes. The objective of this study was to assess the effects of distractions and ADHD on traffic crash responsibility.

Methods

A responsibility case-control study was conducted in the adult emergency department of Bordeaux University Hospital, France. Subjects were recruited among drivers injured in a motor vehicle crash between April 2010 and August 2011. Responsibility levels were estimated using a standardized method. Frequencies of exposures were compared between drivers responsible and drivers not responsible for the crash. Independent risk factors were identified using a multivariate logistic regression including test interactions between distractions and ADHD.

Results

A total of 777 subjects were included in the analysis. Factors associated with responsibility were distraction induced by an external event (adjusted OR (aOR)  = 1.47; 95% confidence interval (CI) [1.06–2.05]), distraction induced by an internal thought (aOR = 2.38; CI: [1.50–3.77]) and ADHD (aOR = 2.18 CI: [1.22–3.88]). The combined effect of ADHD and external distractions was strongly associated with responsibility for the crash (aOR = 5.79 CI: [2.06–16.32]). Interaction assessment showed that the attributable proportion due to the interaction among participants with both exposures was 68%.

Discussion

Adults with ADHD are a population at higher risk of being responsible for a road traffic crash when exposed to external distractions. This result reinforces the need to diagnose adult ADHD and to include road safety awareness messages delivered by the physician. Developing advanced driver assistance systems devoted to the management of attention lapses is also increasingly relevant for these drivers.

Introduction

Injuries due to traffic accidents will become the fifth leading cause of death by 2030 if preventive interventions are not implemented quickly (World Health Organization). Whereas rising motorization in newly industrialized countries increases the worldwide population at risk for traffic crash, high income countries have faced a plateauing of the number of lives saved [1].

Among human factors related to traffic crashes, attention issues have been largely ignored by road safety research. Yet, while driving, the driver is submitted to several stimuli causing distractions to deal with. This may degrade the driver's performance in his main task and increase the risk of accidents (e.g. look diversion, slower reaction time…). Interestingly, distractions (both external and internal) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have been associated with traffic crashes [2][4]. External distractions (defined as the diversion of attention away from activities critical for safe driving, towards a competing activity) may result in insufficient attention to secure driving [5], [6]. Of note, it is suggested that external distractions could increase due to the growing use of information technologies while driving (i.e. cell phones, GPS devices). It is supported by experimental studies observing participants' behaviors when driving an instrumented vehicle with induced distracting tasks which showed poorer driving performance [7][9]. Recently a naturalistic study concluded that secondary-task distraction was a contributing factor in over 22% of all crashes and near crashes [10].

Internal distraction includes all signals from the body which divert the driver's attention from the driving task. It comprises mind wandering (i.e. thoughts unrelated to the task at hand) [11][14] which tends to occur when drivers are pre-occupied by their thoughts and can be responsible for attention lapses that might dangerously distract them from the road. In a previous analysis of the same data, we focused on mind wandering and showed that participants who declared they had experienced mind wandering with highly distracting content just before the crash had twice the risk of being responsible for their accident compared with participants who did not experience mind wandering [3].

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a lifelong disorder which has also been associated with traffic crashes [15]. It is a developmental disorder characterized by poor sustained attention, distractibility, impaired impulse control and hyperactivity [16]. Its prevalence in adulthood is relatively high, at around 2.5% [17][19]. ADHD is associated with a large range of negative outcomes comprising psychiatric disorders, socio-adaptive problems and difficulties related to daily attention failures [20]. A meta-analysis of observational studies suggests that ADHD patients receive more tickets for traffic violations, have worse driving habits and are more involved in road accidents in comparison with control groups [4]. This was described as related to the fact that they are more prone to anger on the road (road rage), aggressiveness, impulsivity, risk-taking and higher consumption of alcohol and drugs while driving. Reimer and colleagues (2010) have explored the impact of secondary tasks on ADHD drivers using a driving simulator. Their results suggested an impact on ADHD drivers ability to allocate their attention resources since they had greater difficulty performing in situations where the driving task did not demand or engender a high level of attention (such as on highway sections) [21]. Nevertheless, although impaired ability to concentrate and to sustain attention when dealing with several cognitive activities is well known, few observational studies have explored the impact of distractions on drivers with ADHD [22].

Overall, both distractions and ADHD impact driving. Whereas distractions occur irregularly and randomly, ADHD is a chronic condition characterizing the individual. We hypothesized that ADHD would modify the effect of distractions on traffic crashes. The objective of the study was to assess the effect of distractions (external and internal) and ADHD on responsibility for traffic crash and test the interactions between these two risk factors.

Methods

Setting

We performed a comparative study of responsibility in a population of patients involved in injurious road traffic crashes. Its principle was to compare the frequency of exposures (ADHD, external distraction and internal distraction) and confounders between drivers responsible for the crash (cases) and drivers not responsible for the crash (controls). The study was conducted in the adult emergency department of the Bordeaux University Hospital (France), which serves urban and rural populations of an area comprising more than 1.4 million people. Patients were recruited from April 2010 to August 2011. Data were collected by trained research interviewers through direct interviews using a standardized questionnaire including questions about the crash, the patient's characteristics and the potentially distracting tasks or events at the time of the crash. To preserve intimacy and improve report reliability, the part of the questionnaire with psychiatric screening tools was self-reported. All participants were informed and signed a written consent form. This study was approved by the French data protection authority (Commission Nationale Informatique et Libertés) and the regional ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud-Ouest et Outre Mer III).

Participants

Patients were eligible for study inclusion if they had been admitted to the emergency department in the previous 72 hours for injury linked to a road traffic crash, were aged 18 or older, were drivers in the crash and were able to answer the interviewer (Glasgow coma score 15 at the time of interview, as determined by the attending physician). We assessed 1,436 patients for eligibility. Of these, 368 were excluded for ineligibility (93 were not the driver, 29 were admitted for more than 72 hours and 246 were unable to answer). This resulted in 1068 eligible patients. Of these, 57 refused to participate and a further 56 were excluded from the analysis because of insufficient data. Of the 955 drivers included, 178 did not answer the screening tools for psychiatric disorders. The final sample for analysis comprised 777 participants (73% of the eligible drivers). There was no significant difference between the respondent group and the non-respondent group with respect to responsibility in the crash (respectively 46.1% and 53.4% were responsible for their crash, p = 0.08).

Measures

Outcome variable: responsibility for the crash.

Responsibility levels in the crash were determined by a standardized method adapted from the quantitative Robertson and Drummer crash responsibility instrument [23]. Robertson and Drummer's method was validated in several studies assessing the association between responsibility and exposure to drugs. Adaptation of the method to the French context has been validated and described in previous research [24][27].

Notably, this method of determining the driver's crash responsibility was compared with an independent expert responsibility evaluation, achieving fair agreement with a kappa of 0.71. The method takes into consideration 6 different mitigating factors considered to reduce driver responsibility: road environment, vehicle-related factors, traffic conditions, type of accident, traffic rule obedience and difficulty of the driving task. The adapted method we used has been described elsewhere [3]. Drivers who were assigned any degree of crash responsibility were considered to be cases; drivers who were judged not responsible served as controls. The interviewers were blind to the participant responsibility status and to the psychiatric screening results when using questionnaire sections related to potential distractions, because responsibility score and psychiatric screening scores were computed during the analysis. Furthermore, compliance with traffic rules was reported after the distraction section and the psychiatric screening tools part of the questionnaire was self-administered.

Exposure to distractions.

When interviewed, patients were asked to describe distracting events and activities that occurred just before the crash (i.e. were going on at the time of the driving mistake (inappropriate maneuver, failure to detect a threat, …) that led to the crash), from a list of potential distracting events and activities including: listening to the radio, cell phone use, navigation system use, having a conversation with passengers, eating, drinking, smoking, picking up an object, being distracted by an event outside the vehicle,…. The external distraction variable was then coded as a dichotomous variable: presence of external distraction (when there is at least one distraction reported) vs absence of external distraction.

To assess mind wandering, patients were asked to describe their thought content just before the crash. To reduce memory bias and halo effect, they were given two opportunities to report their thoughts during the interview. Two researchers (CG and EL) examined and recoded each verbal reporting of thoughts until a consensus was found. Each thought was classified in one of the following categories: thought unrelated to the driving task or to the immediate sensory input, thought related to the driving task, no thought or no memory of any thought. To capture the intensity of the thought, the participant filled in a numerical scale (0–10) answering the question: “How much did the thought disrupt/distract you?” We then dichotomized the score (slightly disrupting/distracting 0–4 vs highly disrupting/distracting 5–10). Mind wandering was then coded as a dichotomous variable: internal distraction (ID) (mind wandering with highly distracting content unrelated to the driving task or to the immediate sensory input) vs no ID (mind wandering with little disrupting/distracting content unrelated to the driving task or to the immediate sensory input or no mind wandering reported).

Potential confounders included patient's characteristics (age, sex, socioeconomic category), crash characteristics (season, time of day, location, vehicle type), and self-reported psychotropic medicine use in the preceding week Patients were also asked how many hours they had slept during the previous 24 hours. They were considered as sleep deprived if they reported sleeping less than six hours. Emotional valence was evaluated with the self-assessment manikin (SAM) tool to characterize the driver's emotional valence state just before the crash [28]. It was recoded in a dichotomous variable (negative affect v positive or neutral affect). Finally, the participants were questioned about alcohol consumption within the six preceding hours.

Psychiatric disorders.

We investigated three psychiatric dimensions using screening tools included in a self-administered questionnaire: ADHD symptoms, depressive disorders and anxiety disorders. These two last disorders were assessed as potential cofounders since ADHD is frequently associated with anxiety disorders, mood disorders and earlier onset of major depression [20]. The Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale V1.1 (ASRS-V1.1) from the WHO Composite International Diagnosis Interview was used for ADHD screening. It examines symptoms described by the DSM-IV as being commonly seen in adult ADHD [29].

The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) was used to screen for depressive disorders and anxiety disorders [30]. This questionnaire is used to explore several dimensions of psychiatric disorders, referring to the classification of psychiatric disorders as defined in the DSM. We used 13 items for depressive disorders and 7 items for anxiety disorders.

Participants were asked to report any current use of psychotropic medications from a list of psychiatric disorders, impairments and symptoms (anxiety, depression, ADHD, epilepsy, insomnia, other disorder, other medication).

Statistical analyses

Participants of the study sample were compared with the group of those excluded because they did not respond to the psychiatric disorders screening part of the questionnaire. Univariate and multivariate analyses (based on logistic regression using SAS statistical software package version 9.3–SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) were conducted to determine associations between exposures, potential confounders and the driver's responsibility for a road traffic crash (responsible versus not responsible). Significant variables (p<0.2, univariate logistic regression) were included in a multivariate logistic regression model. We then performed a manual stepwise backward logistic regression to select the variables in the final model used below to assess interaction between distraction and ADHD. To determine the extent to which ADHD modified associations between distractions and responsibility, we evaluated interaction on an additive scale. We assessed departures from additive effects as described by Rothman and colleagues when considering interaction, particularly in the public health field [31]. It examines the extent to which the effect of both risk factors when present jointly departs from the sum of their independent effects. Interaction was assessed separately for ADHD and external distraction on the one hand and for ADHD and mind wandering on the other hand. Two separate models were fitted for each distraction with four dummy variables representing groups of exposure: no ADHD and no distraction (reference group), ADHD only, distraction only, both ADHD and distraction. For each model, joint effects of ADHD and distraction were estimated using relative excess risk due to interaction (RERI) and attributable risk proportion due to interaction (AP) and their respective 95% confidence intervals. RERI is an estimate of excess risk that is directly attributable to the interaction between two exposures. These measures of interaction were calculated using the following formulas:

RERI  =  OR++−OR+ −−OR− ++1 and AP =  RERI/OR++ (where OR designates odds ratio and the subscripts represent the presence or absence of each of the two risk factors). Bootstrapped confidence intervals were derived via 1,000 bootstrap replications of the original data. Confidence intervals were used to interpret interactions: RERI = 0 or AP = 0 means no interaction (exact additive affect); RERI>0 or AP>0 means positive interaction (beyond additive effect); RERI<0 or AP<0 means negative interaction (antagonism).

Results

Of the 777 subjects with complete data, 419 (53.9%) were classified as controls and 358 (46.1%) as cases. Table 1 shows the participants' characteristics and univariate associations with responsibility. The ASRS screening tool defined a group of 67 participants (8.6%) with ADHD symptoms. Depressive disorder and anxiety disorder scores were grouped into quartiles (first quartile includes the lowest scores and fourth quartile the highest). An external distraction was reported by 270 (34.8%) subjects and internal distraction by 104 (13.4%). Although depressive and anxious disorders were not significantly associated with responsibility, we forced these two variables in our final model as potential confounders. Multivariate modeling showed the following factors as significantly associated with an increased risk for being responsible for the crash (Table 2): ADHD (aOR = 2.18; 95%CI [1.22–3.88]), external distraction (aOR = 1.47; 95%CI [1.06–2.05]) and internal distraction (aOR = 2.38; 95%CI [1.50–3.77]). Alcohol consumption and psychotropic drug use were also associated with an increased risk of being responsible (respectively aOR = 3.19; 95%CI [1.70–6.01] and aOR = 2.10; 95%CI [1.23–3.60]) as well as sleep deprivation (aOR = 1.88; 95%CI [1.09–3.25]). Being a professional driver was associated with a decreased risk of being responsible (aOR = 0.58; 95%CI [0.38–0.90]). The fit of the model was good (Hosmer-Lemeshow test p = 0.46). Figs. 1 and 2 show adjusted odds ratios used to assess interactions between ADHD and external and internal distraction, respectively. There was a higher risk of being responsible for participants who had both ADHD and ED (aOR = 5.79; 95%CI [2.06–16.32]) compared with the reference group. The ADHDxED interaction was significant (RERI = 3.92; 95%CI [0.36–34.2]; AP = 0.68; 95%CI [0.13–0.95]). Regarding the ADHDxID interaction, results showed higher risks of being responsible for participants who had only ADHD (aOR = 2.26 [1.20–4.23]), for those who were exposed to ID only (aOR = 2.43 [1.50–3.96]) and also for those who were exposed to both ADHD and ID (aOR = 4.37 [1.11–17.28]). There was no significant interaction (RERI = 0.68; 95%CI [−3.68–5.4×106], AP = 0.16; 95%CI [−6.88–0.99]) (Fig. 2).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Association of ADHD and external distraction with driver responsibility, multivariate logistic regression (n = 777).

Adjusted for internal distraction, alcohol use, psychotropic drug use, sleep deprivation, professional driver, gender, age, depressive disorders and anxiety disorders. (+) and (−) represent presence or absence of the risk factor.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115002.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Association of ADHD and internal distraction with driver responsibility, multivariate logistic regression (n = 777).

Adjusted for external distraction, alcohol use, psychotropic drug use, sleep deprivation, professional driver, gender, age, depressive disorders and anxiety disorders. (+) and (−) represent presence or absence of the risk factor.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115002.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Sample characteristics and univariate analysis of driver responsibility for road traffic crashes, univariate logistic regression (n = 777).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115002.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Factors associated with driver responsibility for road traffic crashes, multivariate logistic regression (n = 777).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115002.t002

Among the 777 subjects included in the analysis, only 1 participant declared to use ADHD medication. Additional sensitivity analysis was performed and the results remained consistent when this participant was not included.

Discussion

Principal findings and interpretation

Participants who had symptoms of ADHD, and those exposed to external/internal distractions were significantly more likely to be responsible for a road crash after adjustment for a range of potential confounders. Assessment of interaction showed a higher risk of being responsible when exposed to both external distraction and ADHD. The proportion of risk that is attributable to the interaction for those who were exposed to both ADHD and external distraction was 68%. These results are consistent with the increased risk of road crashes described in patients with ADHD. We can hypothesize that the effect of external distraction on the risk of being responsible for a road accident was higher in participants with ADHD compared with controls because of greater difficulty in managing the dual task situation. The process of managing the dual task involves the subject's ability to allocate attention flexibly between the tasks to be performed, an ability which can be affected by working memory impairment. Hence, the interaction between ADHD and external distraction could be related to the frequent working memory impairments in ADHD [32], [33]. Working memory is defined as a cognitive system that provides temporary storage and manipulation of the information necessary for complex cognitive tasks [34]. Working memory impairments may be responsible for difficulties completing two tasks simultaneously (dual task) or resisting an external distraction. External distractions may compete with the main task of driving and involve the driver in a dual task situation. This multitasking overload can affect the driver's performance through a large range of inattention-related impairments, such as failing to detect stimuli in the periphery of the visual field, fewer rear-view mirror and speedometer checks, increased time reaction [35]. The more the secondary task calls upon cognitive processing resources, the more it alters the driver's behavior. Moreover, attention ensures consistency of behavior towards a purpose (top-down) and allows a quick response to changes in the environment (bottom-up). It is therefore important for a driver distracted by secondary information (indicating a situation of danger) to process it quickly and to return to the main task. The interaction between cognitive flexibility and resistance to distraction allows this process to move back and forth between main and secondary task stimuli. These cognitive abilities involve the working memory, which has to stick to the purpose of the main task. Thus, because of a higher susceptibility to being distracted by surrounding stimuli, the same external distraction can capture the attention of a driver with ADHD and cause greater attention failures than in a control driver, resulting in a greater increase in the risk of being responsible for an accident.

In our sample, only 1 participant declared to use ADHD medication. This is consistent with the fact that ADHD is currently underdiagnosed and treated in France and many European countries [36]. Given that the use of psychotropic stimulants improves ADHD impairments, additional sensitivity analysis was performed. The results remained consistent when this participant was not included.

Strengths and weaknesses

To our knowledge, there is no study of the interactions between distractions and ADHD, except for driving simulator studies [22]. The large sample size of the present observational study provided the first approach to real driving conditions. The standardized method using the occurrence conditions, the environment and vehicle-related factors to define responsibility for each driver is particularly useful when studying the impact of human factors by looking for an over-representation of these factors in the responsible group. Assessment of interaction using an additive scale allows the proposal of recommendations whose impact is measurable.

This study has limitations which should be taken into account when interpreting its results. It includes subjective evaluation of both impairments and distraction exposures. The assessment of psychiatric disorders was not based on clinical diagnosis. Reassuringly, the ASRS-V1.1 has performed well in studies, shows good internal consistency, and good concordance with clinical diagnosis (area under the receiver operator curve 0.90) [29], [37]. Distractions were assessed retrospectively and can be affected by a possible memory bias. We can, however, assume this potential bias to be non-differential among cases and controls. For external distractions, it is possible that responsible drivers tended to under-report their distractive activities while driving since some of them are prohibited (namely cell phone use). This possible differential misclassification bias would then have resulted in an underestimation of the association measure. Another limitation with respect to external validity is that the participants included were not representative of all traffic crash victims. The drivers whose condition did not require emergency care, because of low crash severity, could not be recruited. Similarly, patients who were unable to answer the questions because of the severity of their condition were not included (17% of eligible patients). The non-inclusion of these two groups, corresponding to the extremes in crash severity, made it difficult to interpret the selection bias effect potentially induced by an association between responsibility and severity. It is noteworthy that no objective measures of cognitive impairments were performed in this observational study. Hence, it would be of great interest to explore our findings using computerized neuropsychological testing and driving simulators

Implications and future research

ADHD and external distraction are known risk factors for traffic crashes. The interaction assessed on an additive scale highlighted the impact of the combination of these two factors, a result with potential consequences as regard to prevention. This suggests potential public health benefits of ADHD screening. It is estimated that 2.5% of adults have ADHD, though many remain undiagnosed [19]. Improved screening and diagnosis of ADHD in adults would enable adoption of care strategies that focus on reducing attention impairment and improving cognitive performance. These strategies, which may also include road safety awareness messages, are likely to reduce the risk of road crashes in patients with ADHD. Some authors have reported improved driving performance in ADHD patients treated pharmacologically. However, these studies were all performed on driving simulators in small numbers of participant, which limits their generalizability to real driving conditions [4], [15], [22], [38]. A recent epidemiological Swedish study has shown that the risk of serious transport crashes was significantly reduced in ADHD patients while they were treated [39]. Other approaches are also being developed based on cognitive or emotional therapy. Cognitive remediation strategies to improve executive functions seem to decrease attention impairments and to improve attention flexibility in adult patients with ADHD [40]. Among emotional therapies, meditation techniques such as "mindfulness" may also improve sustained attention [41]. These strategies were also applied to small samples and further research seems necessary. Interestingly, there is a growing literature on advanced driver assistance systems, which have become an active area of traffic safety research [42]. Such systems support information assimilation and help to avoid distraction (e.g. if the driver is visually distracted, a warning halo refocuses his or her attention). It is now considered that there is a safety potential for advanced driver assistance systems, which may also be of great value in ADHD patients.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CG EL. Analyzed the data: CG BC MB LO EL KE. Wrote the paper: KE. Revising data critically for important intellectual content, final approval of the version to be published, agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work: KE EL LO MB BC CG. Acquisition of data and interpretation of data: EL CG.

References

  1. 1. World Health Organization (2013) Global status report on road safety. Available: http://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/road_safety_status/2013/en/index.html. Accessed 19 May 2013.
  2. 2. Bakiri S, Galéra C, Lagarde E, Laborey M, Contrand B, et al. (2013) Distraction and driving: results from a case-control responsibility study of traffic crash injured drivers interviewed at the emergency room. Accid Anal Prev 59:588–92.
  3. 3. Galera C, Orriols L, M′Bailara K, Laborey M, Contrand B, et al. (2012) Mind wandering and driving: responsibility case-control study. BMJ 345:e8105.
  4. 4. Jerome L, Segal A, Habinski L (2006) What we know about ADHD and driving risk: a literature review, meta-analysis and critique. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 15(3):105–25.
  5. 5. Lee JD, Young KL, Regan MA (2008) Defining driver distraction. In: Lee JD, Young KL, Regan MA, editors. Driver distraction: Theory, effects, and mitigation. CRC Press, Taylor & Francis Group. pp.31–40.
  6. 6. Ranney TA (2008) Driver Distraction: A Review of the Current State-of-Knowledge. Report No. DOT HS 810 787. USDOT, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Available: http://trid.trb.org/view.aspx?id=868221. Accessed 19 May 2013.
  7. 7. Young KL, Salmon PM, Cornelissen M (2013) Distraction-induced driving error: An on-road examination of the errors made by distracted and undistracted drivers. Accid Anal Prev 58:218–25.
  8. 8. Young KL, Mitsopoulos-Rubens E, Rudin-Brown CM, Lenné MG (2012) The effects of using a portable music player on simulated driving performance and task-sharing strategies. Appl Ergon 43(4):738–46.
  9. 9. Horberry T, Anderson J, Regan MA, Triggs TJ, Brown J (2006) Driver distraction: the effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment complexity and age on driving performance. Accid Anal Prev 38(1):185–91.
  10. 10. Klauer SG, Dingus TA, Neale VL, Sudweeks JD, Ramsey DJ (2006) The impact of driver inattention on near-crash/crash risk: an analysis using the 100-car naturalistic driving study data. Report No. DOT HS 810 594. USDOT, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Available: http://trid.trb.org/view.aspx?id=786825. Accessed 19 May 2013.
  11. 11. Killingsworth MA, Gilbert DT (2010) A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330(6006):932.
  12. 12. Schooler JW, Smallwood J, Christoff K, Handy TC, Reichle ED, et al. (2011) Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends Cogn Sci 15(7):319–26.
  13. 13. Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, et al. (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315(5810):393–5.
  14. 14. Christoff K (2012) Undirected thought: neural determinants and correlates. Brain Res 1428:51–9.
  15. 15. Jerome L, Habinski L, Segal A (2006) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and driving risk: a review of the literature and a methodological critique. Curr Psychiatry Rep 8(5):416–26.
  16. 16. Riccio CA, Wolfe M, Davis B, Romine C, George C, et al. (2005) Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: manifestation in adulthood. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 20(2):249–69.
  17. 17. Polanczyk MD, de Lima MD, Horta MD, Biederman MD, Rohde MD (2007) The Worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry 164(6):942–8.
  18. 18. Lara C, Fayyad J, de Graaf R, Kessler RC, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, et al. (2009) Childhood predictors of adult Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: results from the World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Biol Psychiatry 65(1):46–54.
  19. 19. Simon V, Czobor P, Bálint S, Mészáros A, Bitter I (2009) Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 194(3):204–11.
  20. 20. Sobanski E (2006) Psychiatric comorbidity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 256 Suppl 1: 26–31.
  21. 21. Reimer B, Mehler B, D′Ambrosio LA, Fried R (2010) The impact of distractions on young adult drivers with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Accid Anal Prev 42(3):842–51.
  22. 22. Barkley RA, Cox D (2007) A review of driving risks and impairments associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the effects of stimulant medication on driving performance. J Safety Res 38(1):113–28.
  23. 23. Robertson MD, Drummer OH (1994) Responsibility analysis: a methodology to study the effects of drugs in driving. Accid Anal Prev 26(2):243–7.
  24. 24. Laumon B, Gadegbeku B, Martin J-L, Biecheler M-B (2005) Cannabis intoxication and fatal road crashes in France: population based case-control study. BMJ 331:1371.
  25. 25. Orriols L, Delorme B, Gadegbeku B, Tricotel A, Contrand B, et al. (2010) Prescription medicines and the risk of road traffic crashes: a French registry-based study. PLoS Med 7(11):e1000366.
  26. 26. Lowenstein SR, Koziol-McLain J (2001) Drugs and traffic crash responsibility: a study of injured motorists in Colorado. J Trauma 50(2):313–20.
  27. 27. Salmi LR, Orriols L, Lagarde E (2014) Comparing responsible and non-responsible drivers to assess determinants of road traffic collisions: time to standardise and revisit. Inj Prev 20(6):380–6.
  28. 28. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (2001) International Affective Picture System (IAPS): instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical report A-5, Center for Research in Psychophysiology. University of Florida.
  29. 29. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, et al. (2005) The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med 35(2):245–56.
  30. 30. Achenbach TM, Newhouse PA, Rescorla LA (2004) Manual for the ASEBA older adult forms and profiles. Research center for children, youth and families. University of Vermont.
  31. 31. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL (2008) Modern Epidemiology, 3rd Edition. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins.
  32. 32. Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, Tannock R (2005) A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44(4):377–84.
  33. 33. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF (2005) Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry 57(11):1336–46.
  34. 34. Baddeley A (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 4(11):417–23.
  35. 35. Hurts K, Angell LS, Perez MA (2011) The Distracted Driver Mechanisms, Models, and Measurement. Rev Hum Factors Ergon 7(1):3–57.
  36. 36. Kooij SJJ, Bejerot S, Blackwell A, Caci H, Casas-Brugué M, et al. (2010) European consensus statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD: The European Network Adult ADHD. BMC Psychiatry 10:67.
  37. 37. Adler LA, Spencer T, Faraone SV, Kessler RC, Howes MJ, et al. (2006) Validity of pilot Adult ADHD Self- Report Scale (ASRS) to rate adult ADHD symptoms. Ann Clin Psychiatry 18(3):145–8.
  38. 38. Barkley RA (2004) Driving impairments in teens and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 27(2):233–60.
  39. 39. Chang Z, Lichtenstein P, D'Onofrio BM, Sjölander A, Larsson H (2014) Serious transport accidents in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the effect of medication: a population-based study. JAMA Psychiatry 71(3):319–25.
  40. 40. Solanto MV, Marks DJ, Wasserstein J, Mitchell K, Abikoff H, et al. (2010) Efficacy of meta-cognitive therapy for adult ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 167(8):958–68.
  41. 41. Zylowska L, Ackerman DL, Yang MH, Futrell JL, Horton NL, et al. (2008) Mindfulness meditation training in adults and adolescents with ADHD: a feasibility study. J Atten Disord 11(6):737–46.
  42. 42. Staubach M (2009) Factors correlated with traffic accidents as a basis for evaluating Advanced Driver Assistance Systems. Accid Anal Prev 41(5):1025–33.