Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Formyl Peptide Receptor as a Novel Therapeutic Target for Anxiety-Related Disorders

  • Irene Gallo ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Irene Gallo, Lorenza Rattazzi

    Affiliation William Harvey Research Institute, Barts and the London School of Medicine, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom

  • Lorenza Rattazzi ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Irene Gallo, Lorenza Rattazzi

    Affiliation William Harvey Research Institute, Barts and the London School of Medicine, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom

  • Giuseppa Piras,

    Affiliation William Harvey Research Institute, Barts and the London School of Medicine, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom

  • Thomas Gobbetti,

    Affiliation William Harvey Research Institute, Barts and the London School of Medicine, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom

  • Elisabetta Panza,

    Affiliation Department of Experimental Pharmacology, University of Naples “Federico II”, Naples, Italy

  • Mauro Perretti,

    Affiliation William Harvey Research Institute, Barts and the London School of Medicine, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom

  • Jeffrey W. Dalley ,

    JD and FD are joint senior authors on this work.

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom

  • Fulvio D'Acquisto

    F.Dacquisto@qmul.ac.uk

    JD and FD are joint senior authors on this work.

    Affiliation William Harvey Research Institute, Barts and the London School of Medicine, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom

Abstract

Formyl peptide receptors (FPR) belong to a family of sensors of the immune system that detect microbe-associated molecules and inform various cellular and sensorial mechanisms to the presence of pathogens in the host. Here we demonstrate that Fpr2/3-deficient mice show a distinct profile of behaviour characterised by reduced anxiety in the marble burying and light-dark box paradigms, increased exploratory behaviour in an open-field, together with superior performance on a novel object recognition test. Pharmacological blockade with a formyl peptide receptor antagonist, Boc2, in wild type mice reproduced most of the behavioural changes observed in the Fpr2/3-/- mice, including a significant improvement in novel object discrimination and reduced anxiety in a light/dark shuttle test. These effects were associated with reduced FPR signalling in the gut as shown by the significant reduction in the levels of p-p38. Collectively, these findings suggest that homeostatic FPR signalling exerts a modulatory effect on anxiety-like behaviours. These findings thus suggest that therapies targeting FPRs may be a novel approach to ameliorate behavioural abnormalities present in neuropsychiatric disorders at the cognitive-emotional interface.

Introduction

The immune system is equipped with a vast variety of biological weapons to sense the presence of pathogens via the recognition of pathogen-associated molecular patterns (PAMPs) [1], [2]; these elicit a complex series of events leading to the specialization and differentiation of the immune cells, B and T lymphocytes [3]. Formyl peptide receptors (FPRs) are G protein-coupled receptors whose main function is to sense the presence of harmful or noxious molecules such as formylated peptides and guide cells to the site where pathogen-associated molecules have been released [4]. This sensing function of FPRs is not limited to a particular pathogen and is extended to a wide range of endogenous ligands including classical biomarkers of inflammation and immune activation such as serum amyloid A (SAA) [5], formylated peptides released by mithochondria of damaged cells and tissue [6], the antimicrobial peptide LL-37 [7] and the dual pro- and anti-inflammatory protein Annexin-A1 [8].

There are currently three functional FPRs in humans as well as in mouse - FPR1, FPR2 and FPR3- which all recognise to different degrees a wide range of endogenous and exogenous ligands [6], [9], [10]. Activation of these receptors causes their homo- or hetero-dimerization which in turn depends on the precise ligand they bind to [11], [12]. In this way FPRs are able to exert both pro- and anti-inflammatory effects on immune cells [4], [8], [10].

The expression of FPRs is highest in sentinel innate cells with phagocytic or chemotactic activity such as neutrophils [13], [14], monocytes [13], [15], macrophages[15], [16] and dendritic cells [15], [17]. However, FPR are also expressed in non-phagocytic and “immobile” sentinel cells such as mucosal epithelial cells [18], [19], endothelial cells [20][22] and glia [23][25]. In these cells, FPRs exert a genuine “sentinel role” by sensing pathogens present in the microenvironment as well as by favouring repair upon damage and inflammation. Recent findings show that FPRs are expressed in the vomeronasal system, where they are postulated to detect the presence of infection in the “macro environment” through volatile FPR ligands present in the faeces of pathogen-infected animals [26][29]. Thus, FPRs exert a unique role in the response of the host to pathogens because they signal at two levels; firstly at the level of the central nervous system to alert the host of impeding dangers and secondly at the level of the immune system by initiating a protective inflammatory response.

Recent findings indicate that the centrally regulated behaviours of anxiety and fear-elicited responses are strongly modulated by FPR1 [30]. These data suggest that FPRs may play a permissive role in the pathophysiology of various psychiatric disorders, which increasingly implicate immunological mechanisms in their aetiology [31][33]. In the present study we investigated various anxiety-related behaviours in Fpr2/3-/- mice [34], including responses to novelty and aversive contextual stimuli, and compared the selectivity of these responses with low anxiety-provoking behaviours. We report that Fpr2/3-/- mice show increased explorative behaviour and reduced fear compared with wild type littermates. Notably, the behavioural profile of Fpr2/3-/- mice was partially mimicked by intraperitoneal injection of the pan-FPR inhibitor Boc2 [35], [36], which was accompanied by a decreased activation of downstream FPR signalling pathways in the gut. Together these results support the hypothesis that FPRs may have an important role to play in the regulation of aversive emotional responses. Thus targeting FPRs might provide new avenues of treatment for a range of brain disorders linked to anxiety.

Materials And Methods

Mice

Four to six week old male mice were used for all experiments. Fpr2/3-/- mice have been previously described [34] and were backcrossed onto C57BL/6 for more than 8 generations. Animals were housed in groups of 4–5 under specific-pathogen-free conditions, with free access to food and water and in a room under a 12 h light/dark cycle (light on at 7:00 am). C57BL/6 mice were purchased from Charles River (Margate, UK) and housed for at least 10 days in the same room as the Fpr2/3-/- prior to testing to allow acclimatization. Fpr2/3+/+ littermate controls and C57BL/6 mice were used in equal number and are collectively referred to as wild-type controls since they showed no significant differences in all the preliminary tests. All animal studies were conducted with ethical approval from the Local Ethical Review Committee. This research was carried out in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986 and under the UK Home Office project license number 70/6994.

Behavioural tests and pharmacological treatment

If not otherwise stated, tests were performed double-blind every other day during the light phase of the light-dark cycle, as previously described and recommended [37]. All the efforts were made to minimize mouse discomfort in these behavioral experiments. Mice were brought to the testing room at least 30 minutes before the start of the test session to allow habituation to the testing environment. Unless otherwise specified, standard lighting (about 50 lux) and quiet conditions were maintained throughout each experiment. FPR antagonist studies were performed with male C57BL/6 mice receiving an intraperitoneal injection of the FPR2 antagonist Boc2 (t-Boc-FLFLF; at a previously validated dose of 10 µg/animal [38], [39] or an equal volume of phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) as a control solution (200 µl), 30 minutes before the behavioural tests. This research was carried out in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986.

Open field activity test

The open field test (OFT) is an ethologically based paradigm that provides objective measures of exploratory behaviour as well as a valid initial screen for anxiety-related behaviour in rodents and was carried out as previously described with some modifications [40]. The apparatus consisted of a white PVC arena (50 cm×30 cm×20 cm) divided into 10 cm×10 cm squares (n = 15). The 3 central squares defined the “centre” region (see Fig. 1). Each mouse was placed in a corner square, facing the wall, and observed and recorded for 3 minutes. The total number of squares crossed (all four paws in), total number of rears (defined as both front paws off the ground, but not as a part of grooming) and number of centre crossings was recorded. The walls and floor of the arena were thoroughly cleaned between each trial.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Reduced anxiety-like behaviour of Fpr2/3-/- mice in the open field test.

The bar graphs and images show total number of squares crossed, rears and centre crossings during a 5-minute session. Values are expressed as median ± S.E.M. and representative of four experiments, each involving 6–9 mice per group. * P<0.05 indicate significant values compared with wild-type (WT) control mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g001

Climbing activity test

The climbing test is used to assess vertical activity and exploratory behaviour. The test was performed as previously described but with some modifications [41], [42]. Briefly, mice were placed, one at a time, on a thin layer of fresh wood chip bedding on a laboratory bench and covered with a cylindrical climbing mesh (60 cm×30 cm base diameter) (see Fig. 2). They were each observed and recorded for 5 minutes. The number of climbing events and total duration of climbing activity was assessed. The criterion for climbing was for a mouse to have all 4 feet on the wire mesh while a climb terminated as soon as one foot touched the bench. This test was conducted in the late afternoon, when mice are known to be more active [43].

thumbnail
Figure 2. Increased exploratory behaviour of Fpr2/3-/- mice in the climbing test.

The bar graphs show the number of climbing events and total time (seconds) spent on the climbing mesh during a 5-minute trial. Values are expressed as median ± S.E.M. and representative of three different experiments, each involving 6–9 mice per group. * P<0.05 indicate significant values compared with wild-type (WT) control mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g002

Light-dark shuttle box

In this test exploratory activity reflects the combination of hazard and risk avoidance [44]. The apparatus consisted of a 45 cm×20 cm×21 cm box, divided into two distinct compartments: one third (15 cm long) painted black, with a black lid on top, the remaining two thirds painted white and uncovered (see Fig. 3). A 2.5 cm×2.5 cm opening joined the two compartments. One side of the bright box was transparent to enable behavioural assessment and the averseness of this compartment was increased by additional illumination supplied by a 50 W lamp placed 45 cm above the centre of the box floor. The test was performed in accordance with a previous published protocol [45]. Each mouse was placed in the bright compartment, facing away from the opening and allowed to explore the box for 5 minutes. Dependent variables included the time spent in the light area, latency to cross to the dark area (all four paws in) and the total number of transitions between compartments. The apparatus was cleaned after each trial.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Reduced anxiety-like behaviour of Fpr2/3-/- mice in the light and dark box test.

The bar graphs and images show the total time (seconds) spent in the lit area, latency (seconds) to first cross to the dark chamber and total number of transition during a 5-minute trial. Values are expressed as median ± S.E.M. and representative of four different experiments involving 6–9 mice per group. * P<0.05 and ** P<0.05 indicate significant values compared with wild-type (WT) control mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g003

Marble burying test

The marble-burying test (MBT) is thought to reflect repetitive and perseverative behaviour, possibly related to compulsions and/or anxiety disorders [46]. The test was carried out as described by Deacon and colleagues [47] with some modifications. Briefly, mice were individually placed in a clear plastic box (14 cm×10 cm×11 cm) filled with approximately 5 cm depth of wood chip bedding lightly pressed to give a flat surface (see Fig. 4). Fifteen 1.5 cm diameter glass marbles were placed on the surface, evenly spaced, each about 4 cm apart, so to form 5 rows of 3. The latency to start digging (defined as the mouse digging the bedding with front and hind paws for more than 1 second), the total number of digging bouts and the number of buried marbles (to 2/3 of their depth) were manually recorded during the 10 minute-test.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Reduced digging and marble burying behaviour of Fpr2/3-/- mice in the marble burying test.

The bar graphs and relative pictures show the total number of buried marbles, total duration (seconds) of digging and the latency (seconds) to the first digging bout during a 10-minute trial. Values are expressed as median ± S.E.M. and representative of four different experiments involving 6–9 mice per group. * P<0.05 and ** P<0.05 indicate significant values compared with wild-type (WT) control mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g004

Novel object recognition test (NORT)

The novel object recognition test has been widely used to assess the mouse's ability to discriminate between a previously encountered and a novel object [48]. The test relies on the idea that mice approach more frequently and spend more time exploring a novel object when previously exposed to a familiar one that they recognize as already encountered. The test was carried out as previously described [49], [50]. On day one mice were firstly habituated to the open-field box for 10 minutes. On day two, mice were placed in the same arena for a 10 minute acquisition period, during which time they were exposed to two identical toys (3 cm3 non-toxic red wooden cubes (object A and B). Objects were glued to the floor 10 cm apart from each other, 8 cm away from both box edges. After being returned in their home cage, mice were given a one-hour inter-trial interval. Each subject was then placed back into the arena, where everything was the same as during the acquisition phase except that object A was replaced with a wooden, green, cylinder (4 cm height, 1 cm base diameter) (novel object). During this test phase, mice were allowed to explore both of the objects for 5 minutes. Acquisition and test phases were recorded with a video camera and time spent visiting each object (visit defined as when the animal's nose touched the object or was pointed towards it within 1 cm radius) was manually assessed in both phases. Results were expressed as percentage of object discrimination [(Time spent exploring novel object/total time spent exploring during testing phase) ×100]. After each trial both the arena and the objects were cleaned with 70% ethanol, in order to eliminate olfactory traces.

Y-Maze spontaneous alternation test

The Y maze was made of three enclosed transparent plastic arms (A, B, C) 29 cm×8 cm×19 cm each, set at an angle of 120° to each other in the shape of a Y. It was fixed on a white wooden board and placed on the floor of a room containing several large immovable objects to use as spatial cues. In this test for spatial memory mice tend to enter the maze arm that was explored most recently and remember the order of the arm entry, thanks to their ability to allocate the arm's positions through spatial clues surrounding the testing apparatus. Mice were allowed to freely explore the arena for 5 minutes, during which time the total number of arm entries was recorded, along with the entering sequence, not including the initial arm. A spontaneous alternation occurred when the animal entered into all three arms of the maze on consecutive choices in overlapping triplet sets (e.g. CBABCABCBACB  = 8 alternations) [51]. Spontaneous alternation percentage was calculated as: [Total number of actual alternations/(total arm entries –2)] ×100. The maze was thoroughly cleaned after each test.

Colon whole mount preparation

Colon biopsies were washed with PBS and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Samples were washed 2 times with PBS and then permeabilised with PBS containing 0.1% Triton X-100 for 5 minutes. Thereafter, samples were washed again, then blocked in PBS containing 5% foetal bovine serum (FBS) for 1 hour. Samples were incubated alternatively with mouse monoclonal anti-phospho-p38 (#sc-7973, Santa Cruz Biotechnology) (1∶100 dilutions) for 90 minutes and then all with Alexa Fluor 488 goat polyclonal anti-mouse IgG (H+L) (ab150113, Abcam) 1∶100 for 1 hour. Rinsed samples were finally mounted in Optimum Cutting Temperature (O.C.T.; Tissue-Tek) and frozen at −80°C. Five µm thick sections were mounted on slides and visualized by fluorescence microscopy [52].

Plasma corticosterone and cytokine measurement

Blood was collected from untested mice by intracardiac puncture performed under anaesthesia, and all efforts were made to minimize suffering. Plasma was obtained from the clotted blood by centrifugation (8000 rpm, 5 min) and stored at −80°C before the assay. Corticosterone concentrations were measured in diluted (1∶32) plasma by Enzymatic Immuno Essay (EIA) assay following the manufacturer's instructions (Enzo Life Sciences, Exeter, UK). Cytokine levels in the same samples were measured (dil. 1∶500) using mouse Th1/Th2/Th17/Th22 16 plex Kit FlowCytomix and according to the manufacturer's instructions (eBioscience).

Statistical analysis

Results were analysed as previously described [53][55] using GraphPad. Unpaired Student's t test was performed for experiments where differences between two groups needed to be analysed. For non-parametric data, the Mann–Whitney U-test was applied and results were expressed as medians (interquartile range). Statistical significance was determined at p<0.05. The results were expressed as mean ± S.E.M.

Results

Reduced anxiety in Fpr2/3 null mice

Although we found no statistically significant difference between Fpr2/3-/- mice and wild-type control mice with respect to ambulation and rearing (Fig. 1, left and middle panels, respectively), Fpr2/3-/- mice showed reduced thigmotaxis (walking along the edges) and significantly increased centre crossings (Fig. 1, right panel) indicating a reduced level of anxiety [56], [57].

We further tested anxiety-related behaviour using the climbing test where vertical exploratory behaviour is assessed [41], [42]. As shown in Fig. 2, Fpr2/3-/- mice performed a greater number of climbing acts compared with wild-type (p<0.05) and spent on average more time climbing than control animals (p<0.05).

We next investigated anxiety behaviour using the light/dark shuttle box and the marble-burying test. Consistent with our earlier results, Fpr2/3-/- mice spent significantly more time in the aversive, brightly lit compartment compared with wild-type controls (p<0.05) and waited longer to move to the less aversive, dark side of the box (p<0.01) (Fig. 3, left and middle panels, respectively).

Fpr2/3-/- mice also buried less marbles and spent less time in this activity compared with wild-type (Fig. 4 left and middle panels, respectively). The latency to start this behaviour was also significantly increased in Fpr2/3-/- mice (Fig. 4 right panel) consistent with reduced anxiety.

Improved novel object recognition in Fpr2/3-/- mice

To investigate whether reduced anxiety of Fpr2/3-/- mice was linked to an increased preference for novelty, indicative of low anxiety, we next assessed the performance of animals on a novel object recognition task. This test has been widely used as an explicit test of novel versus familiar object discrimination and relies on the idea that animals tend to preferentially approach novel objects [48]. We found that Fpr2/3-/- mice and controls showed no difference in their exploration of two identical objects (Fig. 5A, left panel). However, following the introduction of the novel object, wild-type mice spent about 40% of their time with the novel object as previously reported [49], [50] while Fpr2/3-/- mice spent a significantly greater proportion (about 60%) (Fig. 5A, right panel).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Increased discriminatory activity of Fpr2/3-/- mice in the novel object recognition test.

The bar graph in A shows the total time (seconds) spent exploring the objects used in the test (shown in the top picture) during the 10-minute acquisition phase (left panel) and the % of time spent on the novel object (shown in the bottom picture) in the subsequent 5-minute test phase (right panel). The bar graphs in B show the total number of arm entries and spontaneous alternation percentage (calculated as described in material and Methods section) in the Y-maze during a 5-minute trial. Values are expressed as median ± S.E.M. and representative of n = 4 different experiments involving 6–9 mice per group. ** P<0.05 indicates significant values compared with wild-type (WT) control mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g005

Fpr2/3-/- mice show no difference in the Y-maze test

We next tested the Fpr2/3-/- mice in the Y maze. In this test mice tend to enter the maze arm that was explored most recently and recall the order of the arm entry. As shown in Fig. 5B, there were no significant difference between wild types and Fpr2/3-/- mice in the number of arm entries or percentages of alternations in this maze. These data show that Fpr2/3-/- mice are not impaired on a spatial memory task and imply that the effects reported earlier pertain mainly to diminished anxiety and fear-related responses in this group of animals.

Higher basal corticosterone levels in Fpr2/3 null mice

To determine whether the apparent differences in behaviour we observed were due to latent infection or inflammation, we performed a number of biochemical tests on serum samples. We found no significant differences in 12 inflammatory cytokines between wild-type and Fpr2/3-/- mice (data not shown). However, levels of circulating corticosterone were markedly higher in Fpr2/3-/- mice compared with controls (Fig. 6). These data are consistent with other findings showing a positive correlation between high responsiveness in a novel environment and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activation [58][60].

thumbnail
Figure 6. Increased level of corticosterone in Fpr2/3-/- mice.

Levels of corticosterone in the plasma of WT and Fpr2/3-/-. Values are expressed as ngml−1 and are representative of three experiments with 6 mice.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g006

Administration of an FPR antagonist reduces some anxiety behaviours

We next investigated whether the reduced anxiety of Fpr2/3-/- mice could be mimicked by administering the FPR inhibitor Boc2 in wild type animals. As shown in Fig. 7, Boc2 had no significant effect on general locomotion or explorative behaviour in the open field test (A) but did increase both the time in the brightly lit aversive compartment and the latency to cross to the ‘safe’ dark compartment (B). Moreover, Boc2-treated mice showed an increased preference for the novel object on the object recognition task compared with vehicle-treated wild-type animals (Fig. 8). These findings suggest FPR blockers may reduce some anxiety-related behaviours, including neophobia

thumbnail
Figure 7. Boc2-treatment reduces anxiety-like behaviour in C57BL/6 mice.

The bar graphs in A show the total number of squares crossed, rears and centre crossings of Boc2-treated mice compared to PBS vehicle-treated during a 5-minute trial in the open field test. The bar graph in B shows the total time (seconds) spent in the lit area and the latency (seconds) to first cross to the dark chamber of Boc2-treated mice compared to PBS vehicle-treated mice during a 5-minute trial. Values are expressed as median ± S.E.M. and representative of four different experiments involving 6 mice per group. * P<0.05 and ** P<0.01 indicate significant values compared to PBS-vehicle treated mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g007

thumbnail
Figure 8. Boc2-treatment increases the recognition of a novel object in C57BL/6 mice.

The bar graphs show the total time (seconds) spent exploring the objects used in the test during the 10-minute acquisition phase (left panel) and the % of time spent on the novel object in the subsequent 5-minute test phase (right panel) of Boc2-treated mice compared to PBS vehicle-treated mice. Values are expressed as median ± S.E.M. and representative of four different experiments involving 6 mice per group. ** P<0.01 indicates significant values compared to PBS-vehicle treated mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g008

Reduced FPR signalling in the gut of Fpr2/3-/- and Boc2-treated mice

Since the behavioural phenotype of Fpr2/3-/- mice could be partly reproduced by FPR antagonism we next investigated whether these effects were related to changes in the local (peritoneal) microenvironment. Given previous findings showing a key role of FPRs in regulating gut microbiota homeostasis we measured p-p38, a widely recognised intracellular readout of FPR activation [61]. Fluorescence microscopy of colonic tissue confirmed our prediction of increased staining for p-p38 in wild-type mice compared with Fpr2/3-/- and Boc2-treated mice (Fig. 9).

thumbnail
Figure 9. Reduced p-p38 staining in the colon of Boc2-treated C57BL/6 and Fpr2/3-/- mice.

Immunofluorescence (top panel) of phospho-p38 of intestinal whole mount preparations (as described in Material and Methods) in either PBS-vehicle treated, Boc2 treated or Fpr2/3-/- intestinal mucosa. The middle and bottom panel show the bright field and the overlay pictures of the same samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g009

Discussion

The results of this study suggest that genetic deletion of Fpr2/3 in mice causes significant changes in anxiety-related behaviour. Our experiments expand on previous observations made by Gao et al. on the behaviour of Fpr1-/- mice [30] in terms of exploratory activity, anxiety, and fear-associated memory. The present study confirms and extends these findings by revealing reduced anxiety of Fpr2/3-/- mice on a range of tests of anxiety, including open-field and climbing exploratory behaviour, choice preference for aversive versus non-aversive contexts, and novel versus familiar objects. However, loss of Fpr2/3function did not affect species-specific activities such as burrowing or nest construction (data not shown). We also found a significant increase in the level of corticosterone in Fpr2/3-/- mice compared with controls. This is consistent with previous studies suggesting that altered baseline concentrations of cortisol in blood plasma is one of the features of anxiety disorders [62]. Often considered a biomarker of stress [63][65], the level of corticosterone does not always correlate with the level of anxiety. Indeed, studies for instance on the anxiolytic effects of enriched environment or voluntary exercise in experimental animals have reported conflicting data including no changes in corticosterone [66], [67] or an initial increase followed by a decrease to basal levels [68] or, as in our case, a significant increase [69][71]. We were very intrigued by the results of these studies since, as for the enriched or ‘exercised’ mice, the Fpr2/3-/-mice show both increased level of corticosterone and an overall increase in exploratory and locomotory activity as shown by the open field (Fig. 1) and climbing test (Fig. 2). In light of these findings it is tempting to speculate that the increased corticosterone levels in Fpr2/3-/- mice might be the results of their increased ‘engagement’ with the external and social environment. Interestingly, these data contrast with those observed in Fpr1-/- mice [30] suggesting orthogonal regulation of corticosterone levels by FPR2/3 and FPR1 receptors.

Further exploration of the inquisitive and fearless nature of Fpr2/3-/- mice using the novel object test showed an almost 50% increase their discriminatory activity and no difference in spatial memory. These results suggest that the absence of homeostatic FPR2/3 signalling might induce a state of behavioural disinhibition and reduced anxiety. This conclusion is consistent with the widely recognised sensing/alerting function of FPRs in the olfactory system [29] and thus provides a further example of behavioural modulation by FPR signalling.

To support this hypothesis and to explore the therapeutic potential of our findings we investigated the effects of a FPR blocker on behaviour. Our findings reveal that administration of the pan-FPR antagonist Boc2 induced a behavioural profile that resembled, at least in part, what we observed in the Fpr2/3-/- mice. We think that this is most likely due to the metabolism of this inhibitor and hence to its transient effect as previously shown [72], [73]. Thus, we observed a significant increase in the number of center crossings in the open field and a significant increase in the time spent in the lit area of the light and dark box, both observations that are indicative of reduced anxiety. We also observed a marked improvement in the ability of wild-type animals to discriminate the novel object. We found that these differences (readily detectable after as little as 2 hour post treatment) were present only after intraperitoneal but not intravenous administration of Boc2 (data not shown). The lack of effect of intravenous administration of Boc2 led us to test whether Boc2 inhibited FPR signalling in the gut or intestinal mucosa.

A number of studies have shown that the intestinal mucosa expresses receptors for formylated peptides produced by the gut microbiota [18]. These commensal bacteria are known to play important and non-detrimental roles for the host [74][76] and have provided a perfect example of consensual interaction between microbes and immune sentinels present throughout the gut. These immune-microbiome interactions are known to be an important part of a dual circuit that controls behaviour and overall emotional wellbeing [77][79]. Indeed, one of the best examples of this system are the germ-free mice that are known to show signs of increased anxiety and reduced neurogenesis [80][83].

Our findings also show that both Fpr2/3-/- mice and Boc2-treated mice have a reduced immunostaining for p-p38 – a key FPR signalling pathway [4], [11], [84], [85]. Similar findings have been previously reported in other studies where it has been shown that commensal bacteria such as the Lactobacillus species stimulated these pathways in gut epithelial cells [19], [52], [74], [76]. It was recently suggested that the expression of FPR2 on the apical and lateral membrane of mouse colonic epithelial cells may have important biological significance, as it enables the epithelial cells to respond to both locally and systemically available ligands under various pathophysiological conditions [86]. Although we have not systematically explored this idea using a wider range of doses and other FPR antagonists our results show that the effects of Boc2 on behaviour occurs in parallel with a modulation of microbiota-induced FPR signalling in the gut. More specifically, the homeostatic and protective inflammatory state of the gut sustained by the commensal microbiota might contribute to a “homeostatic” status of focus and alertness that feature what we know as physical and mental wellbeing. Conversely, in the absence of this physiological loop a state of alertness and reduced anxiety might help the host to “focus” on the possible origin of “internal conflicts and dangers” (Fig. 10).

thumbnail
Figure 10. Hypothetical schema of the role of Fpr2/3 at the interface of the gut-brain axis.

Non-pathogenic gut microbiota releases physiological levels of formylated peptides that activate FPR signalling in the gut epithelium. This homeostatic level of protective inflammation influences brain function maintaining a physiological level of focus and attention. The blockage of FPR signalling by an antagonist or the absence of gut microbiota causes a reduction in FPR activation and a parallel increase in the state of alertness, as observed in Fpr2/3-/- and Boc2-treated mice.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114626.g010

The validation of this model would have a significant translational impact for a variety of disorders that express impaired levels of attention and focus and a strong anxiety component, including obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). Indeed, a number of recent studies have shown that dysfunctions of the gastrointestinal and immune systems are common comorbidities of anxiety related disorders [87][90]. Therefore, modulation of the microbiota through administration of FPR antagonists or genetically-engineered probiotic bacteria releasing Boc2-like peptides might represent a novel strategy for the treatment of a number of cognitive and anxiety-related brain disorders.

Author Contributions

None. Conceived and designed the experiments: FD JD. Performed the experiments: IG LR GP TG EP. Analyzed the data: IG LR FD. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MP. Wrote the paper: FD JD.

References

  1. 1. Janeway CA Jr, Medzhitov R (2002) Innate immune recognition. Annu Rev Immunol 20:197–216.
  2. 2. Medzhitov R, Janeway C Jr (2000) Innate immune recognition: mechanisms and pathways. Immunol Rev 173:89–97.
  3. 3. Sallusto F, Lanzavecchia A, Araki K, Ahmed R (2010) From vaccines to memory and back. Immunity 33:451–463.
  4. 4. Ye RD, Boulay F, Wang JM, Dahlgren C, Gerard C, et al. (2009) International Union of Basic and Clinical Pharmacology. LXXIII. Nomenclature for the formyl peptide receptor (FPR) family. Pharmacol Rev 61:119–161.
  5. 5. Su SB, Gong W, Gao JL, Shen W, Murphy PM, et al. (1999) A seven-transmembrane, G protein-coupled receptor, FPRL1, mediates the chemotactic activity of serum amyloid A for human phagocytic cells. J Exp Med 189:395–402.
  6. 6. Zhang Q, Raoof M, Chen Y, Sumi Y, Sursal T, et al. (2010) Circulating mitochondrial DAMPs cause inflammatory responses to injury. Nature 464:104–107.
  7. 7. De Y, Chen Q, Schmidt AP, Anderson GM, Wang JM, et al. (2000) LL-37, the neutrophil granule- and epithelial cell-derived cathelicidin, utilizes formyl peptide receptor-like 1 (FPRL1) as a receptor to chemoattract human peripheral blood neutrophils, monocytes, and T cells. J Exp Med 192:1069–1074.
  8. 8. Perretti M, D'Acquisto F (2009) Annexin A1 and glucocorticoids as effectors of the resolution of inflammation. Nat Rev Immunol 9:62–70.
  9. 9. Panaro MA, Acquafredda A, Sisto M, Lisi S, Maffione AB, et al. (2006) Biological role of the N-formyl peptide receptors. Immunopharmacol Immunotoxicol 28:103–127.
  10. 10. Dufton N, Perretti M (2010) Therapeutic anti-inflammatory potential of formyl-peptide receptor agonists. Pharmacol Ther 127:175–188.
  11. 11. Cooray SN, Gobbetti T, Montero-Melendez T, McArthur S, Thompson D, et al. (2013) Ligand-specific conformational change of the G-protein-coupled receptor ALX/FPR2 determines proresolving functional responses. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:18232–18237.
  12. 12. Kasai RS, Suzuki KG, Prossnitz ER, Koyama-Honda I, Nakada C, et al. (2011) Full characterization of GPCR monomer-dimer dynamic equilibrium by single molecule imaging. J Cell Biol 192:463–480.
  13. 13. Spurr L, Nadkarni S, Pederzoli-Ribeil M, Goulding NJ, Perretti M, et al. (2011) Comparative analysis of Annexin A1-formyl peptide receptor 2/ALX expression in human leukocyte subsets. Int Immunopharmacol 11:55–66.
  14. 14. El Kebir D, Jozsef L, Filep JG (2008) Opposing regulation of neutrophil apoptosis through the formyl peptide receptor-like 1/lipoxin A4 receptor: implications for resolution of inflammation. J Leukoc Biol 84:600–606.
  15. 15. Yang D, Chen Q, Le Y, Wang JM, Oppenheim JJ (2001) Differential regulation of formyl peptide receptor-like 1 expression during the differentiation of monocytes to dendritic cells and macrophages. J Immunol 166:4092–4098.
  16. 16. Gemperle C, Schmid M, Herova M, Marti-Jaun J, Wuest SJ, et al. (2012) Regulation of the formyl peptide receptor 1 (FPR1) gene in primary human macrophages. PLoS One 7:e50195.
  17. 17. Yang D, Chen Q, Gertz B, He R, Phulsuksombati M, et al. (2002) Human dendritic cells express functional formyl peptide receptor-like-2 (FPRL2) throughout maturation. J Leukoc Biol 72:598–607.
  18. 18. Anton P, O'Connell J, O'Connell D, Whitaker L, O'Sullivan GC, et al. (1998) Mucosal subepithelial binding sites for the bacterial chemotactic peptide, formyl-methionyl-leucyl-phenylalanine (FMLP). Gut 42:374–379.
  19. 19. Babbin BA, Jesaitis AJ, Ivanov AI, Kelly D, Laukoetter M, et al. (2007) Formyl peptide receptor-1 activation enhances intestinal epithelial cell restitution through phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase-dependent activation of Rac1 and Cdc42. J Immunol 179:8112–8121.
  20. 20. Keitoku M, Kohzuki M, Katoh H, Funakoshi M, Suzuki S, et al. (1997) FMLP actions and its binding sites in isolated human coronary arteries. J Mol Cell Cardiol 29:881–894.
  21. 21. Heo SC, Kwon YW, Jang IH, Jeong GO, Yoon JW, et al. (2014) WKYMVm-induced activation of formyl peptide receptor 2 stimulates ischemic neovasculogenesis by promoting homing of endothelial colony-forming cells. Stem Cells 32:779–790.
  22. 22. Mou H, Li Z, Kong Y, Deng B, Qian L, et al. (2012) Proinflammatory stimulants promote the expression of a promiscuous G protein-coupled receptor, mFPR2, in microvascular endothelial cells. Inflammation 35:656–664.
  23. 23. Chen K, Iribarren P, Huang J, Zhang L, Gong W, et al. (2007) Induction of the formyl peptide receptor 2 in microglia by IFN-gamma and synergy with CD40 ligand. J Immunol 178:1759–1766.
  24. 24. Cui YH, Le Y, Zhang X, Gong W, Abe K, et al. (2002) Up-regulation of FPR2, a chemotactic receptor for amyloid beta 1–42 (A beta 42), in murine microglial cells by TNF alpha. Neurobiol Dis 10:366–377.
  25. 25. Cui YH, Le Y, Gong W, Proost P, Van Damme J, et al. (2002) Bacterial lipopolysaccharide selectively up-regulates the function of the chemotactic peptide receptor formyl peptide receptor 2 in murine microglial cells. J Immunol 168:434–442.
  26. 26. Ihara S, Yoshikawa K, Touhara K (2013) Chemosensory signals and their receptors in the olfactory neural system. Neuroscience 254:45–60.
  27. 27. Liberles SD, Horowitz LF, Kuang D, Contos JJ, Wilson KL, et al. (2009) Formyl peptide receptors are candidate chemosensory receptors in the vomeronasal organ. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:9842–9847.
  28. 28. Munger SD (2009) Olfaction: Noses within noses. Nature 459:521–522.
  29. 29. Riviere S, Challet L, Fluegge D, Spehr M, Rodriguez I (2009) Formyl peptide receptor-like proteins are a novel family of vomeronasal chemosensors. Nature 459:574–577.
  30. 30. Gao JL, Schneider EH, Dimitrov EL, Haun F, Pham TM, et al. (2011) Reduced fear memory and anxiety-like behavior in mice lacking formylpeptide receptor 1. Behav Genet 41:724–733.
  31. 31. Sato T, Nishio H, Iwata M, Kentotsuboi, Tamura A, et al. (2010) Postmortem molecular screening for mutations in ryanodine receptor type 1 (RYR1) gene in psychiatric patients suspected of having died of neuroleptic malignant syndrome. Forensic Sci Int 194:77–79.
  32. 32. Saigo M, Abe K, Abe K, Aihara H, Akatsu M, et al. (2005) Study of the suppressed decays B- —>[K+pi-](D)K- and B- —>[K+pi-]Dpi. Phys Rev Lett 94:091601.
  33. 33. Condo GT, Handler T, Shimony J, Abe K, Armenteros R, et al. (1991) Photoproduction of an isovector rho pi state at 1775 MeV. Phys Rev D Part Fields 43:2787–2791.
  34. 34. Dufton N, Hannon R, Brancaleone V, Dalli J, Patel HB, et al. (2010) Anti-inflammatory role of the murine formyl-peptide receptor 2: ligand-specific effects on leukocyte responses and experimental inflammation. J Immunol 184:2611–2619.
  35. 35. Stenfeldt AL, Karlsson J, Wenneras C, Bylund J, Fu H, et al. (2007) Cyclosporin H, Boc-MLF and Boc-FLFLF are antagonists that preferentially inhibit activity triggered through the formyl peptide receptor. Inflammation 30:224–229.
  36. 36. Derian CK, Solomon HF, Higgins JD 3rd, Beblavy MJ, Santulli RJ, et al. (1996) Selective inhibition of N-formylpeptide-induced neutrophil activation by carbamate-modified peptide analogues. Biochemistry 35:1265–1269.
  37. 37. McIlwain KL, Merriweather MY, Yuva-Paylor LA, Paylor R (2001) The use of behavioral test batteries: effects of training history. Physiol Behav 73:705–717.
  38. 38. Gavins FN, Hughes EL, Buss NA, Holloway PM, Getting SJ, et al. (2012) Leukocyte recruitment in the brain in sepsis: involvement of the annexin 1-FPR2/ALX anti-inflammatory system. FASEB J 26:4977–4989.
  39. 39. Gavins FN, Yona S, Kamal AM, Flower RJ, Perretti M (2003) Leukocyte antiadhesive actions of annexin 1: ALXR- and FPR-related anti-inflammatory mechanisms. Blood 101:4140–4147.
  40. 40. Deacon RM, Croucher A, Rawlins JN (2002) Hippocampal cytotoxic lesion effects on species-typical behaviours in mice. Behav Brain Res 132:203–213.
  41. 41. Deacon RM, Rawlins JN (2005) Hippocampal lesions, species-typical behaviours and anxiety in mice. Behav Brain Res 156:241–249.
  42. 42. Sutton LM, Sanders SS, Butland SL, Singaraja RR, Franciosi S, et al. (2013) Hip14l-deficient mice develop neuropathological and behavioural features of Huntington disease. Hum Mol Genet 22:452–465.
  43. 43. Onnela JP, Christakis NA (2012) Spreading paths in partially observed social networks. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 85:036106.
  44. 44. Barr JL, Forster GL (2011) Serotonergic neurotransmission in the ventral hippocampus is enhanced by corticosterone and altered by chronic amphetamine treatment. Neuroscience 182:105–114.
  45. 45. Bourin M, Hascoet M (2003) The mouse light/dark box test. Eur J Pharmacol 463:55–65.
  46. 46. Kedia S, Chattarji S (2014) Marble burying as a test of the delayed anxiogenic effects of acute immobilisation stress in mice. J Neurosci Methods.
  47. 47. Deacon RM (2006) Digging and marble burying in mice: simple methods for in vivo identification of biological impacts. Nat Protoc 1:122–124.
  48. 48. Antunes M, Biala G (2012) The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cogn Process 13:93–110.
  49. 49. Christakis DA, Ramirez JS, Ramirez JM (2012) Overstimulation of newborn mice leads to behavioral differences and deficits in cognitive performance. Sci Rep 2:546.
  50. 50. Taglialatela G, Hogan D, Zhang WR, Dineley KT (2009) Intermediate- and long-term recognition memory deficits in Tg2576 mice are reversed with acute calcineurin inhibition. Behav Brain Res 200:95–99.
  51. 51. Carpenter AC, Saborido TP, Stanwood GD (2012) Development of hyperactivity and anxiety responses in dopamine transporter-deficient mice. Dev Neurosci 34:250–257.
  52. 52. Wentworth CC, Jones RM, Kwon YM, Nusrat A, Neish AS (2010) Commensal-epithelial signaling mediated via formyl peptide receptors. Am J Pathol 177:2782–2790.
  53. 53. van Gaalen MM, Reul JH, Gesing A, Stenzel-Poore MP, Holsboer F, et al. (2002) Mice overexpressing CRH show reduced responsiveness in plasma corticosterone after a5-HT1A receptor challenge. Genes Brain Behav 1:174–177.
  54. 54. Pellow S, File SE (1985) The effects of putative anxiogenic compounds (FG 7142, CGS 8216 and Ro 15-1788) on the rat corticosterone response. Physiol Behav 35:587–590.
  55. 55. Jahng JW (2011) An animal model of eating disorders associated with stressful experience in early life. Horm Behav 59:213–220.
  56. 56. Bourin M, Petit-Demouliere B, Dhonnchadha BN, Hascoet M (2007) Animal models of anxiety in mice. Fundam Clin Pharmacol 21:567–574.
  57. 57. Walsh RN, Cummins RA (1976) The Open-Field Test: a critical review. Psychol Bull 83:482–504.
  58. 58. Aoki M, Shimozuru M, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (2010) Sex differences in behavioral and corticosterone responses to mild stressors in ICR mice are altered by ovariectomy in peripubertal period. Zoolog Sci 27:783–789.
  59. 59. Marquez C, Nadal R, Armario A (2006) Influence of reactivity to novelty and anxiety on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal and prolactin responses to two different novel environments in adult male rats. Behav Brain Res 168:13–22.
  60. 60. Touma C, Bunck M, Glasl L, Nussbaumer M, Palme R, et al. (2008) Mice selected for high versus low stress reactivity: a new animal model for affective disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33:839–862.
  61. 61. Cattaneo F, Guerra G, Ammendola R (2010) Expression and signaling of formyl-peptide receptors in the brain. Neurochem Res 35:2018–2026.
  62. 62. Strohle A, Holsboer F (2003) Stress responsive neurohormones in depression and anxiety. Pharmacopsychiatry 36 Suppl 3: S207–214.
  63. 63. Ganella DE, Kim JH (2014) Developmental rodent models of fear and anxiety: from neurobiology to pharmacology. Br J Pharmacol 171:4556–4574.
  64. 64. Schoenfeld TJ, Gould E (2012) Stress, stress hormones, and adult neurogenesis. Exp Neurol 233:12–21.
  65. 65. Costa-Pinto FA, Palermo-Neto J (2010) Neuroimmune interactions in stress. Neuroimmunomodulation 17:196–199.
  66. 66. Leasure JL, Jones M (2008) Forced and voluntary exercise differentially affect brain and behavior. Neuroscience 156:456–465.
  67. 67. Santos-Soto IJ, Chorna N, Carballeira NM, Velez-Bartolomei JG, Mendez-Merced AT, et al. (2013) Voluntary running in young adult mice reduces anxiety-like behavior and increases the accumulation of bioactive lipids in the cerebral cortex. PLoS One 8:e81459.
  68. 68. Fediuc S, Campbell JE, Riddell MC (2006) Effect of voluntary wheel running on circadian corticosterone release and on HPA axis responsiveness to restraint stress in Sprague-Dawley rats. J Appl Physiol (1985) 100:1867–1875.
  69. 69. Benaroya-Milshtein N, Hollander N, Apter A, Kukulansky T, Raz N, et al. (2004) Environmental enrichment in mice decreases anxiety, attenuates stress responses and enhances natural killer cell activity. Eur J Neurosci 20:1341–1347.
  70. 70. Marashi V, Barnekow A, Ossendorf E, Sachser N (2003) Effects of different forms of environmental enrichment on behavioral, endocrinological, and immunological parameters in male mice. Horm Behav 43:281–292.
  71. 71. Olsson IA, Dahlborn K (2002) Improving housing conditions for laboratory mice: a review of "environmental enrichment". Lab Anim 36:243–270.
  72. 72. Sato AK, Viswanathan M, Kent RB, Wood CR (2006) Therapeutic peptides: technological advances driving peptides into development. Curr Opin Biotechnol 17:638–642.
  73. 73. Park S, Kim SD, Lee HY, Hwang D, Park JS, et al. (2014) A novel delivery platform for therapeutic peptides. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 450:13–18.
  74. 74. Alam A, Leoni G, Wentworth CC, Kwal JM, Wu H, et al. (2014) Redox signaling regulates commensal-mediated mucosal homeostasis and restitution and requires formyl peptide receptor 1. Mucosal Immunol 7:645–655.
  75. 75. Molloy MJ, Grainger JR, Bouladoux N, Hand TW, Koo LY, et al. (2013) Intraluminal containment of commensal outgrowth in the gut during infection-induced dysbiosis. Cell Host Microbe 14:318–328.
  76. 76. Wentworth CC, Alam A, Jones RM, Nusrat A, Neish AS (2011) Enteric commensal bacteria induce extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway signaling via formyl peptide receptor-dependent redox modulation of dual specific phosphatase 3. J Biol Chem 286:38448–38455.
  77. 77. De Palma G, Collins SM, Bercik P, Verdu EF (2014) The Microbiota-Gut-Brain axis in gastrointestinal disorders: Stressed bugs, stressed brain or both? J Physiol.
  78. 78. Foster JA, McVey Neufeld KA (2013) Gut-brain axis: how the microbiome influences anxiety and depression. Trends Neurosci 36:305–312.
  79. 79. Cryan JF, Dinan TG (2012) Mind-altering microorganisms: the impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:701–712.
  80. 80. Cryan JF, O'Mahony SM (2011) The microbiome-gut-brain axis: from bowel to behavior. Neurogastroenterol Motil 23:187–192.
  81. 81. Diaz Heijtz R, Wang S, Anuar F, Qian Y, Bjorkholm B, et al. (2011) Normal gut microbiota modulates brain development and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:3047–3052.
  82. 82. Neufeld KM, Kang N, Bienenstock J, Foster JA (2011) Reduced anxiety-like behavior and central neurochemical change in germ-free mice. Neurogastroenterol Motil 23:255–264, e119.
  83. 83. Gareau MG, Wine E, Rodrigues DM, Cho JH, Whary MT, et al. (2011) Bacterial infection causes stress-induced memory dysfunction in mice. Gut 60:307–317.
  84. 84. Krump E, Sanghera JS, Pelech SL, Furuya W, Grinstein S (1997) Chemotactic peptide N-formyl-met-leu-phe activation of p38 mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) and MAPK-activated protein kinase-2 in human neutrophils. J Biol Chem 272:937–944.
  85. 85. Rane MJ, Carrithers SL, Arthur JM, Klein JB, McLeish KR (1997) Formyl peptide receptors are coupled to multiple mitogen-activated protein kinase cascades by distinct signal transduction pathways: role in activation of reduced nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide oxidase. J Immunol 159:5070–5078.
  86. 86. Chen K, Liu M, Liu Y, Yoshimura T, Shen W, et al. (2013) Formylpeptide receptor-2 contributes to colonic epithelial homeostasis, inflammation, and tumorigenesis. J Clin Invest 123:1694–1704.
  87. 87. Miceli F, Soldovieri MV, Lugli L, Bellini G, Ambrosino P, et al. (2009) Neutralization of a unique, negatively-charged residue in the voltage sensor of K V 7.2 subunits in a sporadic case of benign familial neonatal seizures. Neurobiol Dis 34:501–510.
  88. 88. Luisi R, Panza E, Barrese V, Iannotti FA, Viggiano D, et al. (2009) Activation of pre-synaptic M-type K+ channels inhibits [3H]D-aspartate release by reducing Ca2+ entry through P/Q-type voltage-gated Ca2+ channels. J Neurochem 109:168–181.
  89. 89. Lim H, Jang S, Lee Y, Moon S, Kim J, et al. (2012) Enhancement of Anxiety and Modulation of TH and pERK Expressions in Amygdala by Repeated Injections of Corticosterone. Biomol Ther (Seoul) 20:418–424.
  90. 90. Wang Q, Yu K, Wang J, Lin H, Wu Y, et al. (2012) Predator stress-induced persistent emotional arousal is associated with alterations of plasma corticosterone and hippocampal steroid receptors in rat. Behav Brain Res 230:167–174.