Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Habituation to Experimentally Induced Electrical Pain during Voluntary-Breathing Controlled Electrical Stimulation (BreEStim)

  • Shengai Li,

    Affiliations Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America, Neurorehabilitation Research Laboratory TIRR Memorial Hermann Research Center, Houston, Texas, United States of America

  • Tracy Hu,

    Affiliations Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America, Neurorehabilitation Research Laboratory TIRR Memorial Hermann Research Center, Houston, Texas, United States of America

  • Maria A. Beran,

    Affiliations Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America, Neurorehabilitation Research Laboratory TIRR Memorial Hermann Research Center, Houston, Texas, United States of America

  • Sheng Li

    sheng.li@uth.tmc.edu

    Affiliations Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America, Neurorehabilitation Research Laboratory TIRR Memorial Hermann Research Center, Houston, Texas, United States of America

Abstract

Objective

Painful peripheral electrical stimulation to acupuncture points was found to cause sensitization if delivered randomly (EStim), but induced habituation if triggered by voluntary breathing (BreEStim). The objective was to systematically compare the effectiveness of BreEStim and EStim and to investigate the possible mechanisms mediating the habituation effect of BreEStim.

Methods

Eleven pain-free, healthy subjects (6 males, 5 females) participated in the study. Each subject received the BreEStim and EStim treatments in a random order at least three days apart. Both treatments consisted of 120 painful but tolerable stimuli to the ulnar nerve at the elbow on the dominant arm. BreEStim was triggered by voluntary breathing while EStim was delivered randomly. Electrical sensation threshold (EST) and electrical pain threshold (EPT) were measured from the thenar and hypothenar eminences on both hands at pre-intervention and 10-minutes post-intervention.

Results

There was no difference in the pre-intervention baseline measurement of EST and EPT between BreEStim and EStim. BreEStim increased EPT in all tested sites on both hands, while EStim increased EPT in the dominant hypothenar eminence distal to the stimulating site and had no effect on EPT in other sites. There was no difference in the intensity of electrical stimulation between EStim and BreEStim.

Conclusion

Our findings support the important role human voluntary breathing plays in the systemic habituation effect of BreEStim to peripheral painful electrical stimulation.

Introduction

Pain is multi-dimensional and includes distinct sensory and affective (i.e., unpleasantness) components [1]. Memory mechanisms play a significant role in the persistent awareness of chronic neuropathic pain as well as in the reinforcement of the associated distress. Neuropathic pain is very common, difficult to manage, and has increasingly been recognized as a major contributor to suffering, poor rehabilitation outcomes and reduced quality of life of the persons who are suffering from chronic neuropathic pain [2][4]. Various neurostimulation techniques [5], such as transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) [6], electroacupuncture [7], spinal cord stimulation [8], deep brain stimulation [9], and transcranial direct current stimulation [10][13] have been used for management of neuropathic pain.

We recently proposed an innovative treatment – breathing-controlled electrical stimulation (BreEStim) for neuropathic pain management [14], [15]. This technique was developed from our discovery of the systemic effect of human voluntary breathing on motor function and pain perception [14][20]. In the BreEStim treatment (see details in [14]), human voluntary breathing signal triggers an external electrical stimulator. A single-pulse electrical stimulation is then delivered to peripheral acupuncture points. After receiving a week of daily BreEStim treatment to the acupuncture points on the ipsilateral forearm, a patient with constant shooting phantom pain secondary to an above-the-knee amputation reported no more shooting phantom pain, although he was still able to feel the occasional non-painful shooting sensation in the phantom limb [15]. A similar analgesic effect was also reported in a patient with spinal cord injury [14]. To account for this analgesic effect, we hypothesized that BreEStim integrates multiple internal pain coping mechanisms during voluntary breathing [15], such as electroacupuncture effect, habituation to aversive stimuli, analgesia effect from voluntary breathing, anterograde amnesia of aversive stimuli and activation of the reward system. As a result, the BreEStim treatment has a measurable clinical analgesic effect by increasing pain tolerance, i.e., increased pain threshold.

In a recent study [21], we compared the effects of BreEStim and EStim on sensory thresholds in healthy subjects. Electrical pain threshold (EPT) increased after BreEStim, but decreased after EStim. Neither intervention affected other sensory thresholds (mechanical sensation threshold, thermal thresholds). In this study [21], the same protocol (100 painful electrical stimuli at similar intensities delivered to acupuncture points, Neiguan and Weiguan, on the forearm) led to the opposite findings of habituation after BreEStim and sensitization after EStim. This protocol, however, had - a few confounding factors. The location of the acupuncture point (Neiguan) is very close to the path of the median nerve. Thus, electrical stimulation to the acupuncture points would stimulate the median nerve as well. Both repeated aversive electrical stimulation and electro-acupuncture are reported to have analgesic effects [7], [22], [23], but the sensitization effect was only observed after EStim. Following unilateral electrical median nerve stimulation, there was bilateral activation of primary somatosensory cortex [22] and increased pain threshold of the contralateral index finger [23]. In contrast, the sensitization effect was seen in both stimulated and contralateral symmetrical area after EStim in the previous study [21]. In the present study, we aimed to compare BreEStim and EStim by examining these factors (nerve stimulation vs. acupuncture, unilateral vs. contralateral or systemic effects).

Methods

Subjects

Eleven young and healthy subjects (6 male, 5 female, averaged 29.7 years of age, ranging from 25–44) volunteered in this experiment. According to daily use in writing and eating, one subject was left-handed, and the rest were right-headed. All subjects had no known history of neuromuscular diseases and were pain free. All subjects gave written informed consent prior to participation. This study was approved by the Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects at the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston and TIRR Memorial Hermann Hospital.

Experimental procedures and BreEStim/EStim interventions

In the present study, we adopted our recently published protocol [21]. Each subject received two intervention sessions – EStim and BreEStim. Each intervention was given at least 3 days apart and the order of the sessions was randomized and balanced across subjects to minimize the order effect. Each intervention session consisted of 120 stimuli and took about 30–40 min. Details of each intervention are available on the open access methodology video article at: http://www.jove.com/video/50077/.

During each intervention session, subjects were seated comfortably with both arms and hands on the experiment table in approximately symmetrical positions with the elbow joint of the dominant arm flexed at about 90 degrees (Figure 1). A pair of surface electrodes was placed on the medial aspect on the elbow joint. One electrode was at the joint line level and the other was approximately 3 cm above proximally along the path of ulnar nerve. Surface electrodes were trimmed to a size of one inch by one inch prior to placement.

Delivery of electrical stimulation was the key difference between EStim and BreEStim. During EStim, single-pulse electrical stimuli were randomly delivered every 4 to 8 seconds using a computer program. During BreEStim, subjects wore a face mask connected indirectly to the experimental computer via a pneumotach system (Hans Rodulph Inc). A single-pulse electrical stimulus was triggered if a preset threshold was met by voluntary inhalation signals, usually every 4 to 8 seconds. During voluntary inhalation, subjects were instructed to take a deep breath, similar to routine deep breaths, but faster and stronger. To ensure this, subjects were explicitly instructed to expand their chest walls during voluntary, effortful inhalation. Experimentally, the airflow rate was monitored on the computer. When the airflow rate reached 40% of its peak, an electrical pulse was triggered. When wearing a face mask, subjects usually tolerated such breathing well. As in our previous studies [14], [15], [18], no hyperventilation was reported. For both EStim and BreEStim, rest was allowed upon request. Length of rest and number of rest breaks were also upon request.

The intensity of the electrical stimulation started from the pain threshold and increased to the highest level as tolerated. At that level, subjects may find the electrical stimulation annoying, noxious or painful, but still tolerable even if received repetitively. The experimenter(s) verbally encouraged subjects to increase the level of electrical stimulation gradually as tolerated and pointed out that aversion to electrical stimulation was part of the intervention. Subjects were advised that the expected pain level was equivalent to 6∼7 on the 0–10 VAS scale. However, it is important to point out that the intensity of electrical stimulation was adjusted by the subjects themselves according to their subjective feeling of aversiveness. The intensity of electrical stimulation was recorded at the 20th, 40th, 60th, 80th, 100th and 120th trial of each intervention.

Electrical sensation and pain thresholds

Electrical sensation (EST) and pain (EPT) thresholds were the primary quantitative sensory testing measures. To standardize the tests, EST and EPT testing was performed before and 10 minutes after each intervention (EStim and BreEStim) as in our recent publication [21]. Both EST and EPT were assessed on the thenar and hypothenar eminences of both dominant (treatment) and non-dominant (non-treatment) hands to compare the outcomes of the interventions. The order of EST and EPT assessments on the sites (hypothenar and thenar eminences) was randomized and balanced between two hands.

The same trimmed electrodes were used to assess EST and EPT using the same stimulator (electrical stimulator 7SA, Digitimer). A pair of electrodes was spaced next to each other centered on the thenar or hypothenar eminence. The border of each electrode was marked to ensure consistent placement before and after the intervention. For EST, subjects were instructed to close their eyes and to say “yes” when they explicitly felt electrical stimulation. The intensity of electrical stimulation started from zero and gradually increased in increments of 0.1 mA. For EPT, the intensity of electrical stimulation started from the sensation threshold level and increased in increments of 1 mA. The level of electrical stimulation was recorded as EPT when subjects first reported the electrical stimulation as painful. To improve consistency among subjects, they were advised that the pain threshold level was equivalent to 1 on the 0–10 VAS scale. For both EST and EPT assessments, thresholds from three trials were recorded and averaged.

Data analysis and statistical analysis

EST and EPT were measured at the thenar and hypothenar eminences on both treated (dominant) and non-treated (non-dominant) hands before and after each intervention. Paired t-tests were used to compare the baseline thresholds prior to BreEStim and EStim treatment on different days. The thresholds were similar across each hand and data were collapsed for the baseline analysis. The pre-treatment baseline value was obtained by averaging across testing sites (thenar and hypothenar) and hands (dominant and non-dominant) for EST and EPT. To assess the effect of each intervention, a repeated measures three-way ANOVA with factors of TREATMENT (2 levels, pre- and post-intervention) and HAND (2 levels, dominant and non-dominant) and SITE (2 levels, thenar and hypothenar) was performed. The effect of each intervention was first calculated using the following equation: percent change  =  (post-intervention – pre-invention)/pre-interventionx100%. To further compare the effects of intervention on EST and EPT between BreEStim and EStim, a repeated-measures three-way ANOVA with factors of INTERVENTION (2 levels, BreEStim and EStim), SITE and HAND was performed. A repeated measures two-way ANOVA was performed with factors INTERVENTION and TRIAL (7 levels, 0, 20, 40, 60, 80, 100, 120) to compare possible differences in the intensity between the two interventions. Post hoc Tukey's HSD tests were performed when there was a significant effect in ANOVA tests. The alpha level required for all statistical significance was set at .05. Data were reported as means in the text and as means ± standard errors in the figures and in the table.

Results

The pre-intervention baseline EST and EPT values were averaged across sites and hands to compare baseline assessment on different days. EST and EPT are summarized in Table 1. There were no statistically significant differences in these pre-intervention thresholds on different days (paired t-tests, p value: 0.66 for EST and 0.98 for EPT).

thumbnail
Table 1. Electrical sensation threshold and electrical pain threshold from the thenar and hypothenar eminences of the treatment (dominant) and non-treatment (non-dominant) hands before and after BreEStim and EStim.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104729.t001

BreEStim systematically increased EPT across sites and hands (Fig 2, upper panel), without changing EST. For EPT, a repeated measures 3-way ANOVA showed main effects of TREATMENT (F[1, 10] = 10.18, p = .009) and SITE (F[1, 10] = 6.98, p = .025). No main effects of HAND or significant interactions were found. EPT averaged across sites and hands significantly increased after BreEStim (Pre vs. Post: 25.7 mA vs. 29.2 mA). EPT was significantly greater in the hypothenar eminence (29.4 mA) than in the thenar eminence (25.5 mA) both before and after BreEStim. A similar ANOVA for EST only showed a main effect of SITE (F[1, 10] = 11.55, p = .007). EST was 4.17 mA in the thenar eminence and 5.25 mA in the hypothenar eminence. No main effects of TREATMENT and HAND, or significant interactions were found.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Electrical pain thresholds pre- and post-BreEStim (upper panel) and pre- and post-EStim (lower panel) in all four tested sites on both stimulated and non-stimulated side.

Average values and standard errors are shown.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104729.g002

EStim treatment showed a different effect on EPT, (Figure 2). A 3-way ANOVA did not show main effects of TREATMENT and HAND. But there was a main effect of SITE (F[1, 10] = 6.18, p = .032), significant interactions of TREATMENT x HAND (F[1, 10] = 6.42, p = .030) and TREATMENTxHANDxSITE (F[1, 10] = 8.94, p = .014). Post-hoc Tukey HSD tests revealed that EStim significantly increased EPT on the dominant hypothenar eminence distal to the stimulating site (Pre vs. Post: 25.35 mA vs. 28.37 mA in the dominant hypothenar eminence). EStim did not affect EPT on the contralateral site (Pre vs. Post: 27.43 mA vs. 26.39 mA on the thenar eminence of the non-dominant hand) (see Table 1 for details). Like BreEStim, EStim did not significantly affect EST. A similar 3-way ANOVA only revealed a main effect of SITE (F[1, 10] = 12.27, p = .006) (thenar vs. hypothenar: 4.08 mA vs. 5.23 mA). No other main effects or significant interactions were found.

Differences in these pre- and post-intervention thresholds reflected the effect of intervention, since the pre-intervention baseline EST and EPT values on different days were not significantly different. EPT change after BreEStim (15.1%) was significantly different from EPT change after EStim (−1.2%) (Figure 3). A 3-way ANOVA showed a main effect of INTERVENTION (F[1, 10] = 9.11, p = .013). No other main effects or significant interactions were found.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Change of electrical pain threshold as percentage of pre-intervention values after BreEStim and EStim.

Average values and standard errors are presented.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104729.g003

The intensity of electrical stimulation increased progressively during both EStim and BreEStim (Figure 4). According to an INTERVENTION×TRIAL two-way ANOVA, there was a main effect of TRIAL (F[6, 60] = 48.28, p<.00001), indicating progressive increase in the intensity during the course of treatment. Tukey HSD Post-hoc tests revealed that the intensity increased significantly at the end of 20 trials and 40 trials compared to the starting intensity at the pain threshold (p<0.001). There was no statistical significance in the intensity after 40 trials. However, there was no difference in the intensity between two interventions. The ANOVA showed no main effect of INTERVENTION or significant interaction.

thumbnail
Figure 4. The intensity of electrical stimulation from beginning (trial 0) to the end (trial 120) during BreEStim and EStim.

Average values and standard errors are presented.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104729.g004

Discussion

In the present study, pain-free healthy subjects received the same amount of painful, yet tolerable electrical stimuli (120 stimuli) to the ulnar nerve at the elbow level on the dominant side during EStim and BreEStim interventions at least three days apart. Electrical sensation threshold (EST) and electrical pain threshold (EPT) were measured from both thenar and hypothenar eminences of both hands to compare topographic vs. central effects. The main findings were increased EPT in all sites after BreEStim, i.e., habituation, but no effect on EPT after EStim except for the hypothenar eminence on the stimulated side. There was no significant difference in the intensity of electrical stimulation between EStim and BreEStim. Both interventions had no effect on EST.

Overall, the present findings are consistent with the previous study [21]: BreEStim has systemic analgesic effects to painful stimulation after the intervention, i.e., habituation, while there is no such effect after EStim. In the previous study, painful electrical stimuli were delivered to acupuncture points (Neiguan and Weiguan) of the dominant forearm where the acupuncture point – Neiguan is very close to the path of the median nerve Painful electrical stimuli that was delivered to the ulnar nerve at the elbow level (far from the acupuncture points) resulted in the same pattern of analgesic effect across subjects. Variations in individual responses could account for the degrees of difference between two studies. Increase in EPT was about 27% after BreEStim in the previous study. It was 15% in this study. If acupuncture-induced analgesia contributed to the difference, we would have seen further decrease in EPT after EStim in this study as compared to 10% decrease in EPT in the previous study. However, there was no change in EPT in the contralateral non-stimulated hand after EStim.

Central sensitization after short-term painful electrical stimulation

Central sensitization after a brief course of painful stimulation is a common phenomenon [24][26]. In a recent study [24], about 105 stimuli at the intensity of 5 on the visual analogue scale (“10” – worst pain, “0” – no pain) applied to the volar area of the middle forearm caused increased activation in the classic pain processing areas, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insular cortex, as compared to the rest state. The ACC and the insula have been reported to selectively process the aversive quality of noxious stimulation [27], [28]. Using advanced fMRI (7 T), Hahn et al. [25] was able to demonstrate additional activation of periaqueductal gray (PAG) which was often not observed using 3T fMRI techniques. PAG is known as a pivot region of the descending pain control system [29][31]. These studies suggest that painful peripheral electrical stimulation triggers central responses to aversiveness of painful stimuli and internal descending pain coping mechanisms at the same time, i.e., central sensitization.

Our previous findings are consistent with this central sensitization effect of painful EStim [21]. We observed decreased EPT in the thenar eminence in both hands after 100 painful electrical stimuli to the forearm (Neiguan and Weiguan) on the dominant side. However, in the current study we observed increased EPT in the hypothenar eminence distal to the ulnar nerve on the dominant side, but no change in EPT in the thenar eminence of the dominant hand and in both thenar and hypothenar eminences of the non-dominant hand. No change in EPT in the areas that are not innervated by the ulnar nerve may be attributed to the possible habituation effect, since we used the same protocol but a higher number of painful electrical stimuli (120 electrical stimuli) in the present study. However, the result of increased EPT only in the hypothenar eminence distal to the ulnar nerve at the stimulating site may be caused by a different factor. Direct nerve stimulation to the ulnar nerve at the elbow may interfere with orthodromic impulse propagation of electrical stimulation from the distal hypothenar area — the “busy line” effect [32]. Further study is needed to investigate this dose effect and its relation to central sensitization.

The habituation effect of BreEStim

The habituation effect of BreEStim in the present study was consistent with our previous study in which electrical stimulation was delivered to acupuncture points [21]. There was no difference in the intensity of electrical stimulation between EStim and BreEStim in the present and previous [21] studies. Therefore, the habituation effect of BreEStim is likely attributable to the effect associated with voluntary breathing. Distinctly different from autonomic breathing, voluntary breathing requires extensive cortical and subcortical activation and suppression of brainstem respiratory center for autonomic breathing [33], [34]. According to brain imaging studies, these respiratory-related areas include the primary motor cortex, the premotor cortex, the supplementary motor area, the primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, the insula, the ACC and amygdala, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [35][46].

The habituation effect of BreEStim may be explained by multiple internal pain coping mechanisms. Firstly, voluntary breathing-specific cortical activation is likely to make painful stimulation less unpleasant. There are respiratory specific connections between the insula and the ACC and the activity of pulmonary stretch receptors [47], [48]. Secondary to activation of pulmonary stretch receptors from chest wall expansion during voluntary inhalation, the ACC and the insula are thus specifically activated during voluntary inhalation. The ACC and the insula have been reported to selectively process the aversive quality of noxious stimulation [27], [28], but does not influence sensation of the stimulation [49]. The breathing-associated activation in the ACC and the insula has been related to reduction in pain ratings in certain conditions, such as meditation [50]. Secondly, memory of peripheral electrical stimulation is not consolidated. It has been reported that during activation of the insular cortex by localized micro-stimulation, peripheral aversive stimulation leads to item-specific impairment of aversive memory reconsolidation, i.e., anterograde amnesia [51]. In other words, peripheral painful electrical stimulation is likely to be remembered to a lesser degree during BreEStim. In contrast, memory of aversive electrical stimulation is likely to be consolidated during EStim in which electrical stimulation is delivered during normal breathing. Lastly, the descending pain control mechanism is further enhanced by BreEStim. A recent human study supports the important role of the PAG in regulation of both respiration and pain [27]. Activation of PAG is likely further enhanced during voluntary breathing [52], in addition to painful stimulation-induced reflexive activation to peripheral painful electrical stimulation. Thus, the descending pain control role of PAG is further strengthened. Further neuroimaging studies are needed, however, to substantiate the above mechanisms.

Our findings support the important role voluntary breathing plays in habituation to painful peripheral electrical stimulation during BreEStim. The multi-faceted effects on affective and sensation dimensions of pain could explain the effectiveness of BreEStim on pain reduction in amputation and spinal cord injury patients [14], [15]. This is essentially consistent with previous reports of the effect of regulated breathing on reduction in pain perception [50], [53], [54]. After repetitive exposure to thermal pain pulses, pain intensity and unpleasantness were reduced during slow breathing (half of normal rate) as compared to normal breathing in both healthy subjects and subjects with fibromyalgia syndrome. It is worth mentioning that slow breathing also requires voluntary control of breathing. Future imaging studies may be able to detect different activation patterns between effortful and fast vs. slow voluntary breathing.

Conclusion

In summary, BreEStim increased EPT in all tested sites in both hands, while EStim increased EPT in the hypothenar eminence distal to the stimulating nerve and has no effect on EPT in other sites not innervated by stimulating nerve in pain-free healthy human subjects.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported in part by an NIH grant (NIH/NINDS R01NS060774) and a research grant (013-109) from Mission Connect, a program of TIRR Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SL TH MB SL. Performed the experiments: SL TH MB SL. Analyzed the data: SL SL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SL SL. Contributed to the writing of the manuscript: SL SL. Discussed and interpreted the data: SL TH MB SL.

References

  1. 1. Price DD (2000) Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science 288: 1769–1772.
  2. 2. Norrbrink Budh C, Hultling C, Lundeberg T (2005) Quality of sleep in individuals with spinal cord injury: a comparison between patients with and without pain. Spinal Cord 43: 85–95.
  3. 3. Stormer S, Gerner HJ, Gruninger W, Metzmacher K, Follinger S, et al. (1997) Chronic pain/dysaesthesiae in spinal cord injury patients: results of a multicentre study. Spinal Cord 35: 446–455.
  4. 4. Jensen MP, Chodroff MJ, Dworkin RH (2007) The impact of neuropathic pain on health-related quality of life: review and implications. Neurology 68: 1178–1182.
  5. 5. Kotze A, Simpson KH (2008) Stimulation-produced analgesia: acupuncture, TENS and related techniques. Anaesthesia and Intensive Care Medicine 9: 29.
  6. 6. Norrbrink Budh C, Lundeberg T (2004) Non-pharmacological pain-relieving therapies in individuals with spinal cord injury: a patient perspective. Complement Ther Med 12: 189–197.
  7. 7. Ulett GA, Han S, Han JS (1998) Electroacupuncture: Mechanisms and clinical application. Biol Psychi 44: 129.
  8. 8. Finnerup NB, Yezierski RP, Sang CN, Burchiel KJ, Jensen TS (2001) Treatment of spinal cord injury pain. Pain Clin Updates 9: 1–6.
  9. 9. Murphy D, Reid DB (2001) Pain treatment satisfaction in spinal cord injury. Spinal Cord 39: 44–46.
  10. 10. Fregni F, Boggio PS, Lima MC, Ferreira MJ, Wagner T, et al. (2006) A sham-controlled, phase II trial of transcranial direct current stimulation for the treatment of central pain in traumatic spinal cord injury. Pain 122: 197–209.
  11. 11. Boggio PS, Zaghi S, Fregni F (2009) Modulation of emotions associated with images of human pain using anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuropsychologia 47: 212–217.
  12. 12. Boggio PS, Zaghi S, Lopes M, Fregni F (2008) Modulatory effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on perception and pain thresholds in healthy volunteers. Eur J Neurol 15: 1124–1130.
  13. 13. Plow EB, Pascual-Leone A, MacHado A (2012) Brain stimulation in the treatment of chronic neuropathic and non-cancerous pain. J Pain 13: 411–424.
  14. 14. Li S (2013) Breathing-controlled Electrical Stimulation (BreEStim) for management of neuropathic pain and spasticity. J Vis Exp: e50077.
  15. 15. Li S, Melton DH, Berliner JC (2012) Breathing-controlled electrical stimulation (BreEStim) could modify the affective component of neuropathic pain after amputation: a case report. J Pain Res 5: 71–75.
  16. 16. Li S, Laskin JJ (2006) Influences of ventilation on maximal isometric force of the finger flexors. Muscle Nerve 34: 651–655.
  17. 17. Li S, Park WH, Borg A (2012) Phase-dependent respiratory-motor interactions in reaction time tasks during rhythmic voluntary breathing. Motor Control 16: 493–505.
  18. 18. Li S, Rymer WZ (2011) Voluntary breathing influences corticospinal excitability of nonrespiratory finger muscles. J Neurophysiol 105: 512–521.
  19. 19. Li S, Yasuda N (2007) Forced ventilation increases variability of isometric finger forces. Neurosci Lett 412: 243–247.
  20. 20. Ikeda ER, Borg A, Brown D, Malouf J, Showers KM, et al. (2009) The valsalva maneuver revisited: the influence of voluntary breathing on isometric muscle strength. J Strength Cond Res 23: 127–132.
  21. 21. Li S, Berliner JC, Melton DH, Li S (2013) Modification of electrical pain threshold by voluntary breathing-controlled electrical stimulation (BreEStim) in healthy subjects. PLoS One 8: e70282.
  22. 22. Nihashi T, Naganawa S, Sato C, Kawai H, Nakamura T, et al. (2005) Contralateral and ipsilateral responses in primary somatosensory cortex following electrical median nerve stimulation–an fMRI study. Clin Neurophysiol 116: 842.
  23. 23. Kastrup A, Baudewig J, Schnaudigel S, Huonker R, Becker L, et al. (2008) Behavioral correlates of negative BOLD signal changes in the primary somatosensory cortex. NeuroImage 41: 1364–1371.
  24. 24. Nickel FT, Ott S, Möhringer S, Saake M, Dörfler A, et al. (2014) Brain correlates of short-term habituation to repetitive electrical noxious stimulation. Eur J Pain 18: 56–66.
  25. 25. Hahn A, Kranz GS, Seidel EM, Sladky R, Kraus C, et al. (2013) Comparing neural response to painful electrical stimulation with functional MRI at 3 and 7T. NeuroImage 82: 336–343.
  26. 26. Seifert F, Bschorer K, De Col R, Filitz J, Peltz E, et al. (2009) Medial prefrontal cortex activity is predictive for hyperalgesia and pharmacological antihyperalgesia. J Neurosci 29: 6167–6175.
  27. 27. Von Leupoldt A, Sommer T, Kegat S, Eippert F, Baumann HJ, et al. (2009) Down-regulation of insular cortex responses to dyspnea and pain in asthma. Am J Respir Critl Care Med 180: 232.
  28. 28. LaGraize SC, Borzan J, Peng YB, Fuchs PN (2006) Selective regulation of pain affect following activation of the opioid anterior cingulate cortex system. Exp Neurol 197: 22.
  29. 29. Freund W, Wunderlich AP, Stuber G, Mayer F, Steffen P, et al. (2011) The role of periaqueductal gray and cingulate cortex during suppression of pain in complex regional pain syndrome. Clin J Pain 27: 796–804.
  30. 30. Valet M, Sprenger T, Boecker H, Willoch F, Rummeny E, et al. (2004) Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain–an fMRI analysis. Pain 109: 399–408.
  31. 31. Linnman C, Moulton EA, Barmettler G, Becerra L, Borsook D (2012) Neuroimaging of the periaqueductal gray: state of the field. Neuroimage 60: 505–522.
  32. 32. Sluka KA, Walsh D (2003) Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation: Basic science mechanisms and clinical effectiveness. J Pain 4: 109.
  33. 33. Haouzi P, Chenuel B, Barroche G (2006) Interactions between volitional and automatic breathing during respiratory apraxia. Resp Physiol Neurobiol 152: 169–175.
  34. 34. Guz A (1997) Brain, breathing and breathlessness. Respir Physiol 109: 197–204.
  35. 35. Colebatch JG, Adams L, Murphy K, Martin AJ, Lammertsma AA, et al. (1991) Regional cerebral blood flow during volitional breathing in man. J Physiol 443: 91–103.
  36. 36. Maskill D, Murphy K, Mier A, Owen M, Guz A (1991) Motor cortical representation of the diaphragm in man. J Physiol 443: 105–121.
  37. 37. Ramsay SC, Adams L, Murphy K, Corfield DR, Grootoonk S, et al. (1993) Regional cerebral blood flow during volitional expiration in man: a comparison with volitional inspiration. J Physiol 461: 85–101.
  38. 38. Fink GR, Adams L, Watson JD, Innes JA, Wuyam B, et al. (1995) Hyperpnoea during and immediately after exercise in man: evidence of motor cortical involvement. J Physiol 489 (Pt 3): 663–675.
  39. 39. Macey KE, Macey PM, Woo MA, Harper RK, Alger JR, et al. (2004) fMRI signal changes in response to forced expiratory loading in congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. J Appl Physiol 97: 1897–1907.
  40. 40. Macey PM, Macey KE, Henderson LA, Alger JR, Frysinger RC, et al. (2003) Functional magnetic resonance imaging responses to expiratory loading in obstructive sleep apnea. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 138: 275–290.
  41. 41. Evans KC, Shea SA, Saykin AJ (1999) Functional MRI localisation of central nervous system regions associated with volitional inspiration in humans. J Physiol 520 Pt 2: 383–392.
  42. 42. Smejkal V, Druga R, Tintera J (1999) Control of breathing and brain activation in human subjects seen by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Physiol Res 48: 21–25.
  43. 43. Smejkal V, Druga R, Tintera J (2000) Brain activation during volitional control of breathing. Physiol Res 49: 659–663.
  44. 44. Mazzone SB, McLennan L, McGovern AE, Egan GF, Farrell MJ (2007) Representation of Capsaicin-evoked Urge-to-Cough in the Human Brain Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 176: 327–332.
  45. 45. Evans KC (2010) Cortico-limbic circuitry and the airways: Insights from functional neuroimaging of respiratory afferents and efferents. Biological Psychology 84: 13.
  46. 46. Evans KC, Dougherty DD, Schmid AM, Scannell E, McCallister A, et al. (2009) Modulation of spontaneous breathing via limbic/paralimbic-bulbar circuitry: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage 47: 961.
  47. 47. Hanamori T, Kunitake T, Kato K, Kannan H (1998) Neurons in the posterior insular cortex are responsive to gustatory stimulation of the pharyngolarynx, baroreceptor and chemoreceptor stimulation, and tail pinch in rats. Brain Res 785: 97–106.
  48. 48. Gaytan SP, Pasaro R (1998) Connections of the rostral ventral respiratory neuronal cell group: an anterograde and retrograde tracing study in the rat. Brain Res Bull 47: 625–642.
  49. 49. LaBuda CJ, Fuchs PN (2005) Attenuation of negative pain affect produced by unilateral spinal nerve injury in the rat following anterior cingulate cortex activation. Neurosci 136: 311.
  50. 50. Zeidan F, Martucci KT, Kraft RA, Gordon NS, McHaffie JG, et al. (2011) Brain mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain by mindfulness meditation. J Neurosci 31: 5540–5548.
  51. 51. Stehberg J, Levy D, Zangen A (2009) Impairment of aversive memory reconsolidation by localized intracranial electrical stimulation. Eur J Neurosci 29: 964–969.
  52. 52. Green AL, Wang S, Purvis S, Owen SLF, Bain PG, et al. (2007) Identifying cardiorespiratory neurocircuitry involved in central command during exercise in humans. J Physiol 578: 605.
  53. 53. Zautra AJ, Fasman R, Davis MC, Craig AD (2010) The effects of slow breathing on affective responses to pain stimuli: an experimental study. Pain 149: 12–18.
  54. 54. Chalaye P, Goffaux P, Lafrenaye S, Marchand S (2009) Respiratory effects on experimental heat pain and cardiac activity. Pain Med 10: 1334–1340.