Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Are Normal Decision-Makers Sensitive to Changes in Value Contrast under Uncertainty? Evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task

Abstract

The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) developed by Bechara et al. in 1994 is used to diagnose patients with Ventromedial Medial Prefrontal Cortex (VMPFC) lesions, and it has become a landmark in research on decision making. According to Bechara et al., the manipulation of progressive increments of monetary value can normalize the performance of patients with VMPFC lesions; thus, they developed a computerized version of the IGT. However, the empirical results showed that patients' performances did not improve as a result of this manipulation, which suggested that patients with VMPFC lesions performed myopically for future consequences. Using the original version of the IGT, some IGT studies have demonstrated that increments of monetary value significantly influence the performance of normal subjects in the IGT. However, other research has resulted in inconsistent findings. In this study, we used the computerized IGT (1X-IGT) and manipulated the value contrast of progressive increments (i.e., by designing the 10X-IGT, which contained 10 times of progressive increment) to investigate the influence of value contrast on the performance of normal subjects. The resulting empirical observations indicated that the value contrast (1X- vs. 10X-IGT) of the progressive increment had no effect on the performance of normal subjects. This study also provides a discussion of the issue of value in IGT-related studies. Moreover, we found the “prominent deck B phenomenon” in both versions of the IGT, which indicated that the normal subjects were guided mostly by the gain-loss frequency, rather than by the monetary value contrast. In sum, the behavioral performance of normal subjects demonstrated a low correlation with changes in monetary value, even in the 10X-IGT.

Background

The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) was developed by Bechara et al. [1] to examine the differences in decision making between patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) lesions and normal subjects and to prove the somatic marker hypothesis (SMH), according to which people may be guided by their emotions to make fast and rational decisions under complex and uncertain situations [2]. IGT serial studies [1], [3], [4], [5], [6] specifically indicated that because of the effects of brain lesions on emotional processing, patients with VMPFC lesions are unable to form somatic markers (indexed by skin conductance response (SCR)) that can guide people in making rational decisions, and consequently, perform myopically in decision-making tasks [3], [4], [5], [6].

In the original IGT, participants were presented with four decks of cards (i.e., decks A, B, C, and D) and required to choose cards manually. The gain-loss structure of the ten selections in each deck is designed as a section and is repeated four times. The expected value in each section of the original IGT is steady and unchanged. For each selection, participants are informed of the outcome of wins and losses. Although each selection in disadvantageous decks A and B carries an immediate reward of $100, some trials also contain an uncertain punishment (losses of $150 to $1250). Over the long run, selecting from decks A and B causes negative outcomes. In contrast, every selection in advantageous decks C and D carries an immediate reward of $50, but in some trials also contains an uncertain punishment (losses of $50 to $250). Over the long run, selections from decks C and D cause positive outcomes. Furthermore, decks A and C contain 5 losses in every section, but decks B and D have only 1 loss (see Table 1). According to the prediction of the SMH, normal participants will avoid the bad decks and select more from the good decks due to the formation of somatic markers. The results reported by Bechara et al. [1], [4] proved the SMH; however, the performance of patients with VMPFC lesions showed a reversed outcome (i.e., they preferred decks A and B over decks C and D).

The IGT researchers demonstrated that the decision-making deficit in patients with VMPFC lesions may be due to insensitivity to future consequences (see details in [5]). Furthermore, in their attempt to normalize the performance of patients with VMPFC lesions by enhancing the patients' sensitivity to future consequences, Bechara et al. increased the prospective rewards and penalties gradually in each section of the original IGT and developed a computerized version of the IGT, thereby increasing the expected value in each section of the computerized IGT dynamically. The gains in decks A and B and decks C and D were programmed at an average of $100 or $50, respectively, in the first section of 10 cards and progressively increased by $10 or $5 in consecutive sections in decks A and B and decks C and D, respectively. Additionally, in each section of decks A and C, an additional loss trial was added, but the value remains the same (with losses ranging from $150 to $350 in deck A and $25 to $75 in deck C, which is identical to the original IGT). Therefore, 5 losses are contained in the first section, 6 in the second section, and so on. In each section of decks B and D, the value increases, but the frequency remains the same. The net difference (the expected value of a section) of decks A and B increases by $150 in a negative direction, whereas decks C and D increase by $25 in a positive direction from the second to the sixth section [5], [7] (see Table 2). Although Bechara et al. thought that the progressive increment would improve sensitivity to future consequences in patients with VMPFC lesions and normalize their performances, this manipulation did not succeed. Despite the changes made to the original IGT, the empirical results showed that patients with VMPFC lesions continued to perform myopically, preferring the bad decks A and B [5]. This result further showed the decision-making deficit of future-consequence insensitivity resulting from VMPFC lesions.

thumbnail
Table 2. The gain-loss structure of 1X-IGT (Bechara et al., 2000).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101878.t002

To our knowledge, only a few studies on the influence of value magnitude on selections in IGT exist. Notably, Tomb et al. [8] noted two factors, monetary value and long-term outcome, that were confounded in guiding decision making in the IGT. Thus, Tomb et al. retested the original findings of the IGT [1], [4] and further changed the schedule in which advantageous decks correlated with large gain-loss values, whereas the bad decks correlated with small gain-loss values. The results of their study showed the value-modification of the gain-loss structure to be critical in driving the anticipatory SCR of the IGT, although the behavioral performance still showed the dominance of the final outcome. On the other hand, by using the original version of the IGT, van den Bos et al. [9] suggested that if the ratio of gain value between the good and bad decks increased from 50:100 to 50:200 and 50:300, the selection of bad decks would also increase accordingly (see also [10]). However, Li [11] retested the study by van den Bos et al. and showed normal subjects to be insensitive to the changes in gain value. The value-related IGT studies of Tomb et al., van den Bos et al., and Li used the original IGT as the research instrument, which contains stable expected values in each section.

In this study, we used the computerized IGT [5] (1X-IGT), which contains dynamic expected values and further manipulates value contrast. We also designed the 10X-IGT, which contains 10 times the monetary value of progressive changes. Our study revised both gain and loss structures, rather than the gain structure only [9], and it also renders the differences between the net gains in decks C and D and the net losses in decks A and B even larger to test the influence of value contrast on the performance of normal subjects and whether this manipulation facilitates their performances. If the monetary value contrast were a critical factor in guiding choice behavior, the normal subjects' performance should be largely influenced by the huge amount of progressive increments contained in the 10X-IGT rather than the one in the 1X-IGT. Although the EV difference between disadvantageous and advantageous decks are even greater, normal subjects will choose more cards from the advantageous decks (C and D) in the 10X-IGT, compared to the 1X-IGT. Conversely, if the value contrast does not play an important role in guiding decision making, then there is no significant difference between the two versions.

Materials and Methods

A total of 104 undergraduate students (age: M  =  21.42, SD  =  3.75) at Soochow University were recruited in this study. Students who participated in this study were given course credit in return. This behavioral study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Psychology Department at Soochow University (REC no: 101-1-2) and was conducted in accordance with the unwritten rule of the Taiwan Psychological Society. All subjects gave written informed consent prior to participating. Moreover, the data were analyzed in group level and reported anonymously. A debriefing was conducted after the experiment was completed. The between-subject design was used here to prevent the practice effect during the game.

1X-IGT

52 subjects (26 males) (age: M  =  21.39, SD  =  3.64) participated in the 1X-IGT experiment. The detailed gain-loss structure of the 1X-IGT was retrieved from the IGT software published by Bechara [7]. In the 1X-IGT, the average gain in decks A and B increases by $10 and the total loss also increases by $250 in each section. Thus, each section in decks A and B has a net loss outcome (i.e., expected value of deck A and B corresponding to Table 2). For example, the net loss in the first section is $250 and in each following section an increment of $150 is added until the net loss reaches $1000 in the sixth section. On the other hand, the average gain in decks C and D has an increment of $5 and the total loss increases by $25 in each section. Therefore, decks C and D have a net gain outcome (i.e., expected value of deck C and D corresponding to Table 2) in each section. For example, the net gain in the first 10 selections is $250, and in the subsequent sections the net gain increases by $25 until it reaches $375 in the sixth section. Furthermore, the losses in decks A and C change in frequency, whereas those in decks B and D change in value (Table 2).

10X-IGT

Another 52 subjects (26 males) (age: M  =  21.46, SD  =  3.88) participated in the 10X-IGT experiment. In this version of the IGT, the average gain in decks A and B increases by $100, and the total loss also increases to $2500 in each section. Therefore, each section of decks A and B has a net loss outcome (i.e., expected value of deck A and B corresponding to Table 3). For example, the net loss in the first section is $250 and increases at an increment of $1500 in each subsequent section until the net loss reaches $7750 in the sixth section. On the other hand, the average gain in decks C and D has a $50 increment, and the total loss increases by $250 in each section. Therefore, decks C and D have a net gain outcome (i.e., expected value of deck C and D corresponding to Table 3) in each section. For example, the net gain in the first ten selections is $250, and in the subsequent section the net gain increases by $250 until it reaches $1500 in the sixth section. Briefly, the difference between the 10X-IGT and the 1X-IGT is the increment of 10 times within the average gain and average loss in each section of every deck. Furthermore, the 10X-manipulation consequently makes the original net gain (decks C and D) or loss (decks A and B) in each section of the 1X-IGT increase by 10 times (see detailed in footnotes in Table 3). Notably, in the process of adjustment, we rounded the gain-loss values into whole numbers but without influencing the gain-loss outcome in each of the sections mentioned above. Two versions of the IGT were administered in semi-groups. The subjects were required to earn money or avoid losing money as much as possible in these two tasks, and the tasks ended after 100 selections were made, which was unknown to the subjects. Both versions of the IGT were programmed using Matlab 7.0 for data collection.

Analysis

The data analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS 20.0. Here, we provided the four-factor ANOVA (version: 1X vs. 10X; sex: male vs. female; decks: ABCD and blocks: 1-5) by using univariate ANOVA analysis to evaluate the main effect of each variable and the interactions among factors. Furthermore, we conducted post-hoc analysis (simple main effects) to understand the influences that contribute to the significant interactions.

Results

Global analysis of all factors (version, sex, decks, and blocks)

Two subjects of the 1X-IGT group were excluded from the analysis due to 100 selections of deck C or D. The results of the four factors of the ANOVA (versions [1X vs 10X] × sex [male vs female] × decks [ABCD] × blocks [1]-[5]) indicated no main effects for version, sex, and blocks, but it did for decks (F (3, 1960)  =  113.914, p < .01) (Fig. 1, 2). Two significant interactions were observed (version * decks: F (3, 1960)  =  2.66, p  =  .047; decks * blocks: F (12, 1960)  =  4.793, p < .01). However, interactions of gender * decks (F (3, 1960)  =  1.914, p  =  .125), version * gender * decks (F (3, 1960)  =  1.320, p  =  .266), version * decks * blocks (F (12, 1960)  =  1.059, p  =  .392), gender * decks * blocks (F (12, 1960)  =  1.270, p  =  .230) and version * gender * decks * blocks (F (12, 1960)  =  .663, p  =  .788) are non-significant.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Mean number of deck selection in the two versions of the IGT.

Blue bars represent the mean number of cards chosen in the 1X-IGT and red bars represent the mean number of cards chosen in the10X-IGT. In the 1X-IGT, the average number of selections (SD) for decks A, B, C, and D were 15.28 (5.75), 31.98 (8.32), 25.54 (11.32), and 27.20 (9.25), respectively. In the 10X-IGT, the average number of selections were 15.64 (6.15), 34.87 (13.97), 23.12 (10.10), and 26.39 (14.00), respectively. It can be observed that the number of selections from deck B was significantly higher than the averages for the other decks. Normal subjects demonstrated the “prominent deck B phenomenon” in both versions of the IGT. There were no significant differences between the two versions of the IGT. The behavioral performances in the two versions of the IGT were similar, indicating that the manipulation of value contrast in the 10X-IGT did not influence the performance of normal subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101878.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Card selection tendency in 1X-IGT and 10X-IGT.

The selection tendencies for (a) deck A, (b) deck B, (c) deck C, and (d) deck D were similar in the 1X-IGT (blue line) and 10X-IGT (red line). The empirical results showed that the manipulation of value contrast in progressive changes was unable to influence the performance of normal subjects. Selection curves in both IGT versions also demonstrated that bad deck B and good decks C and D were preferred by the normal decision makers throughout the game. However, the bad deck A was avoided in most of the blocks (i.e., 20 consecutive selections). Clearly, the selection curve of deck B was unexpectedly higher than that of deck A from the beginning to the end. This phenomenon clearly violates the basic assumption of the IGT.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101878.g002

Post-hoc analysis (simple main effects)

We further conducted post-hoc analysis (simple main effects) by using one way ANOVA to understand the influences that contribute to the significant interactions of version * decks and decks * blocks respectively [12]. The post-hoc analysis showed that, in the interaction of version * decks, there are significant between the decks within the 1X-IGT (F (3, 996)  =  50.828, p < .01) and 10X-IGT (F (3, 1036)  =  64.071, p < .01), respectively, but no significant differences between the decks across the two versions (A: F (1, 508)  =  .149, p  =  .700; B: F (1, 508)  =  3.378, p  =  .067; C: F (1, 508)  =  3.076, p  =  .080; D: F (1, 508)  =  .251, p  =  .616). Detailed paired comparison (corrected by Bonferroni) of decks in each version was listed in Table S1. In both 1X- and 10X-IGT, the preference for deck A was significantly lower than the other decks (p < .01), whereas the number of selections for deck B was significantly higher than the selections for decks C (p < .01) and D (p < .01)(see Fig. 1).

On the other hand, post-hoc analysis of decks * blocks interactions showed that there are significant differences within the blocks in deck A (F (4, 505)  =  20.878, p < .01) and also within the blocks in deck D (F (4, 505)  =  3.270, p < .05), but no significant differences within the blocks in decks B (F (4, 505)  =  .482, p  =  .749) and deck C (F (4, 505)  =  1.285, p  =  .275). Analysis of decks in fixed block (1–5) also demonstrated difference between decks is significant in each block (block 1: F (3, 404)  =  23.337, p < .01); block 2: F (3, 404)  =  23.622, p < .01; block 3: F (3, 404)  =  21.836, p < .01); block 4: F (3, 404)  =  29.884, p < .01); block 5: F (3, 404)  =  30.971, p < .01)).

1X- and 10X- IGT learning curve

Notably, the results showed that the interaction of version * decks * blocks (F (12, 1960)  =  1.059, p  =  .392) was non-significant. The deck by deck comparison between 1X- and 10X-IGT is shown in Figure 2. Based on the traditional IGT analysis [5], we further detailed the deck preference with learning curve format in each version. 1X- IGT learning curve: The analysis of blocks in each deck of the two versions showed that selections from deck A gradually decreased, whereas selections from deck D gradually increased over blocks in the 1X-IGT (A: F (4, 196)  =  13.438, p < .01; B: F (4, 196)  =  1.843, p  =  .122; C: F (4, 196)  =  1.402, p  =  .235; D: F(4, 196)  =  4.193, p < .01) (Fig. 2).10X-IGT learning curve: Only selections from deck A gradually decreased over decks (A: F (4, 204)  =  13.911, p < .01; B: F (4, 204)  =  .441, p  =  .779; C: F (4, 204)  =  .809, p  =  .521; D: F(4, 204)  =  1.270, p  =  .283) (Fig. 2).

Discussion

The empirical results of the four-factor ANOVA indicated no significant differences between the two versions of the IGT (1X vs. 10X). Clearly, the main manipulation was unfounded, and the present observation is inconsistent with the fundamental assumption based on expected value [1], [3], [4], [5], [6]. Also, we found no statistical significance for differences in sex and the block effect. Nevertheless, significant results were observed for decks. Furthermore, the effects of interaction were observed not only in the versions and decks, but also in decks and blocks. Table S1 shows that cards from the bad deck A were chosen less frequently than those from the other three decks in both 1X- and 10X-IGT. Notably, the bad deck B was selected at a significantly higher frequency than the other three decks, which is consistent with the “prominent deck B phenomenon” discussed in some previous studies [13][17].

This study used the 1X-IGT (i.e., computerized IGT) [5], [7] and the 10X-IGT to observe the performance of normal subjects. It demonstrated that the manipulation of monetary value contrast did not critically influence the performance of normal decision makers. In this study, we designed 10X-IGT that contains 10 times the monetary increment in progressive changes to test the influence of value contrast on the performance of normal subjects. The empirical results indicate that despite manipulating the increment of value contrast in progressive changes, normal subjects still performed myopically for future outcome and preferred the bad deck B (with frequent gains). Bechara et al. [5] found patients with VMPFC lesions to be insensitive to the manipulation of progressive increments in the computerized IGT, and this result further proved that patients with VMPFC lesions are myopic toward future results. Although patients with VMPFC dysfunction were not included in our study, the persistence of the normal subjects' preference for deck B in both tasks showed that the normal subjects were also insensitive to progressive increments even when the value contrast had been increased 10 times.

Unlike past IGT studies [8], [9], [11] related to monetary value which used the original IGT that did not contain dynamic increment as a research instrument, our study used a computerized IGT (1X-IGT) to test the effect of value contrast on decision making. van den Bos et al. [9] suggested that widening the ratio of gain value between advantageous and disadvantageous decks (changing 50:100 to 50:200, 50:300) would increase selections from the disadvantageous deck. On the other hand, if the ratio was decreased to 50:50, selections from the disadvantageous deck would also decrease [9]. However, Li [11] suggested that the design of 50:50 proposed by van den Bos et al. oversimplified the task and eliminated the conflict between advantageous and disadvantageous decks. Therefore, Li designed the manipulation of 50:75 and reexamined the manipulation (50:100, 50:200) proposed by van den Bos et al. The empirical result showed there is indifference between the groups. Li (sample size: 20 subjects in each condition, total 20 males and 40 females) further suggested that the phenomenon that was proposed by van den Bos et al. (sample size: 50 vs.100 condition: 9 subjects; 50 vs. 200 condition: 12 subjects; 50 vs. 300 condition: 6 subjects) may be accounted by a small sample size. Recently, Penolazzi et al. [10] tested 165 subjects by using the original IGT and the modified IGT, which contains the gain ratio of 300:50 (designed by van den Bos et al. [9]; sample size: 40 males, 41 females) to investigate personality differences that might affect decision making. Comparing the selection of each deck between the original IGT and the modified IGT, only a marginal significant effect in deck B (p  =  .055) was noted. These empirical results further supported the explanation proposed by Li [11]. On the other hand, the findings in this study are generally consistent with those of Li [11].

Furthermore, the empirical results in both versions of the IGT (1X- and 10X-IGT) verified previous findings, wherein normal subjects preferred the frequent-gain decks [13], [18][21], and reexamined the “prominent deck B phenomenon” (i.e., the normal subjects preferred the disadvantageous deck B which has the feature of frequent gains) as suggested by Lin et al. [13] (see also [14][17]). Despite the fact that the computerized IGT revised the structure through progressive changes, each cycle in decks B and D still retained 10 gains and 1 loss and changed only in the loss value. This fact shows the “prominent deck B phenomenon” found in the original IGT [13]. Moreover, the results indicated that the manipulation of progressive increment neither improved the performance of patients with VMPFC lesions [5], nor influenced the performance of normal subjects.

Recently, Steingroever et al. [22] analyzed data from eight groups of normal subjects and found that normal subjects preferred the decks that resulted in infrequent losses. These results were used to challenge the basic assumptions of the IGT. The results in our study support this argument: despite having revised the 1X-IGT and 10X-IGT with progressive increments, normal subjects still preferred decks with frequent gains, or rather, those with infrequent losses. Therefore, both the previous findings and the present study contradicted the basic assumption of the IGT regarding normal subjects.

Based on our empirical results, although monetary value contrast was increased and made the net losses in decks A and B even larger in the negative direction and the net gains in decks C and D even larger in positive direction, no differences in behavioral performance were found in the two versions of the IGT. This suggests that performance in the IGT is dominated by a gain-loss frequency, rather than by value and long-term outcomes [13][16], [23][27].

Recently, researchers have also illustrated that the primary factor in the performance of normal subjects in the IGT was gain-loss frequency and that expected value was secondary. Moreover, the study also suggested that the results were consistent with those of another study using animal subjects, which showed that the influence of reward frequency was larger than that of reward magnitude on the behavior of animal subjects [27], a statement that is confirmed by our study.

In this study, we examined the influence of sex on performance in both versions of the IGT. The results of our study are inconsistent with those of previous studies which suggested that women preferred decks yielding frequent gains [28][31]. Furthermore, in contrast to the original IGT [1] that contains 40 cards in each deck, the computerized IGT [5] contains 60 cards in each deck. Therefore, if the subjects wanted to choose more than 40 cards from each deck of the original IGT (or more than 60 cards from each deck of the computerized IGT), they would be prevented from doing so. However, unlike this setting, the structures of both IGT used in our study were repeated after 60 selections of each deck. This limitation should be noted in future researches.

Conclusion

This research shows that even normal decision-makers were mostly uninfluenced by the manipulation of value contrast under uncertainty. This observation is partially inconsistent with the finding of van den Bos et al. [9], but consistent with those produced in several animal studies and behavioral-analysis literatures. Our study showed the manipulation of progressive increment suggested by Bechara et al. [5] to be not only invalid for improving the performance of patients with VMPFC lesions but also for determining the behavior of normal subjects. Moreover, even when the monetary value contrast of the gain-loss structure increased by 10 times, the subjects continued to prefer the decks with frequent gains. Furthermore, the “prominent deck B phenomenon” was demonstrated in both versions of the IGT.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Paired comparison (corrected by Bonferroni) for two decks in each version (1X- and 10X-IGT).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101878.s001

(doc)

Acknowledgments

The authors would also like to thank Yu-Kai Lin, Shih-Syu Yen, Po-Chin Lee, and Li-Hua Liu for their assistance in test administration and data collection and for providing some helpful discussion. We are grateful to Dr. Raelyn Lee for her help in recruiting subjects. We are also grateful for the help and expert editorial services provided by NOVA Journal Experts (NJE). In addition, we deeply appreciate the two anonymous reviewers who kindly provided us with so many careful and valuable comments related to the concepts and writing of the paper, as well as to the statistical methods used and proper grammar. The authors also sincerely appreciate the editor's patience in coordinating so many rounds of review processing for this article.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: YCC YAS WKL. Performed the experiments: WKL TJS. Analyzed the data: WKL TJS CHL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: TJS CHL YAS. Wrote the paper: WKL YCC CHL. Led the study: YCC CHL.

References

  1. 1. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50: 7–15.
  2. 2. Damasio A (1994) Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: G. P. Putnam's Sons. 205–222 p.
  3. 3. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP (1999) Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci 19: 5473–5481.
  4. 4. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR (1997) Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275: 1293–1295.
  5. 5. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H (2000) Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 123: 2189–2202.
  6. 6. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1996) Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 6: 215–225.
  7. 7. Bechara A (2007) Iowa Gambling Task Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
  8. 8. Tomb I, Hauser M, Deldin P, Caramazza A (2002) Do somatic markers mediate decisions on the gambling task? Nat Neurosci 5: 1103–1104.
  9. 9. van den Bos R, Houx BB, Spruijt BM (2006) The effect of reward magnitude differences on choosing disadvantageous decks in the Iowa Gambling Task. Biol Psychol 71: 155–161.
  10. 10. Penolazzi B, Leone L, Russo PM (2013) Individual Differences and Decision Making: When the Lure Effect of Gain Is a Matter of Size. PLoS ONE 8: e58946
  11. 11. Li XL (2009) The sensitivity to changes of contrast in reward magnitude: the influence to performance in Iowa Gambling Task. Modern Business Trade Industry 12. [Translated from Chinese article]
  12. 12. Clark L, Manes F, Antoun N, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (2003) The contributions of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment following frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 41: 1474–1483.
  13. 13. Lin CH, Chiu YC, Lee PL, Hsieh JC (2007) Is deck B a disadvantageous deck in the Iowa Gambling Task. Behav Brain Funct 3: 16.
  14. 14. Chiu YC, Lin CH, Huang JT (2012) Ch 7: Prominent deck B phenomenon: Are decision-makers sensitive to long-term outcome in the Iowa Gambling Task? In A. Cavanna (Ed.), Psychology of gambling: New research: Nova, 93–118.
  15. 15. Lin CH, Song TJ, Lin YK, Chiu YC (2012) Mirrored Prominent Deck B Phenomenon: Frequent Small Losses Override Infrequent Large Gains in the Inverted Iowa Gambling Task. PloS ONE 7: e47202.
  16. 16. Lin CH, Song TJ, Chen YY, Lee WK, Chiu YC (2013) Reexamining the validity and reliability of the clinical version of the Iowa gambling task: Evidence from a normal subject group. Front Psychol 4: 220.
  17. 17. Takano Y, Takahashi N, Tanaka D, Hironaka N (2010) Big losses lead to irrational decision-making in gambling situations: relationship between deliberation and impulsivity. PLoS ONE 5: e9368.
  18. 18. Fernie G, Tunney RJ (2006) Some decks are better than others: the effect of reinforcer type and task instructions on learning in the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain Cogn 60: 94–102.
  19. 19. Fum D, Napoli A, Stocco A (2008) Somatic Markers and Frequency Effects: Does Emotion Really Play a Role on Decision Making in the Iowa Gambling Task? In: V. Sloutsky BLKM, editor.
  20. 20. Upton DJ, Kerestes R, Stout JC (2012) Comparing the iowa and soochow gambling tasks in opiate users. Front Neurosci 6: 34.
  21. 21. Wilder KE, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE (1998) Operant conditioning and the orbitofrontal cortex in schizophrenic patients: unexpected evidence for intact functioning. Schizophr Res 30: 169–174.
  22. 22. Steingroever H, Wetzels R, Horstmann A, Neumann J, Wagenmakers EJ (2013) Performance of healthy participants on the Iowa Gambling Task. Psychol Assess 25: 180–193.
  23. 23. Chiu YC, Lin CH (2007) Is deck C an advantageous deck in the Iowa Gambling Task? Behav Brain Funct 3: 37.
  24. 24. Chiu YC, Lin CH, Huang JT, Lin S, Lee PL, et al. (2008) Immediate gain is long-term loss: Are there foresighted decision makers in the Iowa Gambling Task. Behav Brain Funct 4: 13.
  25. 25. Lin CH, Chiu YC, Cheng CM, Hsieh JC (2008) Brain maps of Iowa gambling task. BMC Neurosci 9: 72.
  26. 26. Lin CH, Chiu YC, Huang JT (2009) Gain-loss frequency and final outcome in the Soochow gambling task: a reassessment. Behav Brain Funct 5: 45.
  27. 27. Horstmann A, Villringer A, Neumann J (2012) Iowa Gambling Task: there is more to consider than long-term outcome. Using a linear equation model to disentangle the impact of outcome and frequency of gains and losses. Front Neurosci 6.
  28. 28. Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL (2004) Sex-related differences in a gambling task and its neurological correlates. Cereb Cortex 14: 1226–1232.
  29. 29. Overman WH (2004) Sex differences in early childhood, adolescence, and adulthood on cognitive tasks that rely on orbital prefrontal cortex. Brain Cogn 55: 134–147.
  30. 30. Overman WH, Frassrand K, Ansel S, Trawalter S, Bies B, et al. (2004) Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia 42: 1838–1851.
  31. 31. van den Bos R, Homberg J, de Visser L (2013) A critical review of sex differences in decision-making tasks: Focus on the Iowa Gambling Task. Behav Brain Res 238: 95–108.