Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Disruptions in Brain Networks of Older Fallers Are Associated with Subsequent Cognitive Decline: A 12-Month Prospective Exploratory Study

  • Chun Liang Hsu,

    Affiliations Aging, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Brain Research Center, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Center for Hip Health and Mobility, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Michelle W. Voss,

    Affiliations Health, Brain, & Cognition Lab, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, United States of America, Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, United States of America

  • Todd C. Handy,

    Affiliations Centre for Clinical Epidemiology and Evaluation, University of British Columbia & Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute (VCHRI), Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Jennifer C. Davis,

    Affiliations Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Brain Research Center, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Centre for Clinical Epidemiology and Evaluation, University of British Columbia & Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute (VCHRI), Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Lindsay S. Nagamatsu,

    Affiliations Aging, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Brain Research Center, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Center for Hip Health and Mobility, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Alison Chan,

    Affiliations Aging, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Brain Research Center, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Center for Hip Health and Mobility, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Niousha Bolandzadeh,

    Affiliations Aging, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Brain Research Center, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Center for Hip Health and Mobility, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Teresa Liu-Ambrose

    tlambrose@exchange.ubc.ca

    Affiliations Aging, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Brain Research Center, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Center for Hip Health and Mobility, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

Abstract

Cognitive impairment and impaired mobility are major public health concerns. There is growing recognition that impaired mobility is an early biomarker of cognitive impairment and dementia. The neural basis for this association is currently unclear. We propose disrupted functional connectivity as a potential mechanism. In this 12-month prospective exploratory study, we compared functional connectivity of four brain networks– the default mode network (DMN), fronto-executive network (FEN), fronto-parietal network (FPN), and the primary motor sensory network (SMN) – between community-dwelling older adults with ≥ two falls in the last 12 months and their non-falling counterparts (≤ one fall in the last 12 months). Functional connectivity was examined both at rest and during a simple motor tapping task. Compared with non-fallers, fallers showed more connectivity between the DMN and FPN during right finger tapping (p = 0.04), and significantly less functional connectivity between the SMN and FPN during rest (p≤0.05). Less connectivity between the SMN and FPN during rest was significantly associated with greater decline in both cognitive function and mobility over the12-month period (r = −0.32 and 0.33 respectively; p≤0.04). Thus, a recent history of multiple falls among older adults without a diagnosis of dementia may indicate sub-clinical changes in brain function and increased risk for subsequent decline.

Introduction

Cognitive impairment and impaired mobility among older adults are major public health concerns. Both are associated with increased risk for disability, institutionalization, and death [1]. Critically, there is growing recognition that clinical gait abnormalities and falls are early biomarkers of cognitive impairment and dementia [2]. For example, in the Health, Aging and Body Composition Study [3], slower gait speed at baseline was predictive of subsequent cognitive decline. Gait speed was also reported to slow at 0.023 meters/second/year approximately one decade before the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment [4]. Conversely, baseline lower executive functions predicted subsequent decline in gait speed [5]. These results suggest cognitive decline and impaired mobility share common neurobiological mechanisms.

Neuroimaging studies highlight the role of white matter integrity in the association between cognitive function and mobility [6][9]. Both leukoaraiosis, or white matter lesions, and degeneration in white matter microstructure (i.e., myelodegeneration) are associated with cognitive decline and impaired mobility [10][15]. Functionally, white matter deterioration results in alterations in the coordination of brain networks that span multiple association cortices [16][18]. Functional connectivity analysis examines such disruptions; it aims to quantify the temporal coherence between spatially remote brain regions [19]. Regions with a positive correlation in blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal over time are said to have high functional connectivity, and regions uncorrelated or negatively correlated are thought to be in separate, or possibly competing, brain networks [20]. Current evidence suggests that a comprehensive examination of brain networks should include both task-free (“resting state”) and task-based conditions [21]. Mennes and colleagues [21] recently showed resting state connectivity patterns only correspond partially with on-task patterns. The authors found this is particularly true for sub-cortical regions, limbic regions, primary sensory cortex, and primary motor cortex.

A comprehensive examination of large-scale brain networks should also include both within-network and between-network functional connectivity [21]. To date, most studies have focused exclusively on within-network connectivity, particularly within the default mode network (DMN) [22][24]. Using neuroimaging data acquired from the “1000 Functional Connectomes Project” (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/fcon_1000/), Tomasi and colleagues [23] investigated the effect of aging on functional connectivity patterns within resting-state networks. They found that long range connections in the DMN and dorsal attention network were susceptible to aging-related deterioration. In a similar attempt to examine age-associated changes to intrinsic resting-state networks, Spreng and colleagues [25] demonstrated that older adults showed inability to suppress the DMN during cognitively active state, which was subsequently explained as reduced cognitive flexibility. However, recent evidence demonstrates that disruptions in both within-network and between-network connectivity can be observed during resting state [26], [27]. Specifically, using resting-state magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), Brier and colleagues [27] demonstrated that Alzheimer’s disease is associated with widespread loss of both within-network and between-network connectivity. Specifically, compared with non-demented older adults, individuals with very mild or mild Alzheimer’s disease exhibited less connectivity within the DMN and between five functionally defined networks.

Aging and neurodegeneration are characterized by disruptions in the coordination of brain networks that support cognitive function and motor control [16], [28][31] [32][34]. These networks include the DMN, fronto-executive network (FEN), fronto-parietal network (FPN), and the primary motor sensory network (SMN) [16], [35]. The DMN is highly metabolically active when there is a lack of external stimulus (i.e., during rest) and deactivates during goal-oriented activity [36], [37]. Broadly, the DMN is involved in self-referential thoughts (i.e., accessing and processing of past events for the purpose of problem solving or future planning), memory consolidation, and autobiographical memory [16], [37]. The FEN is primarily involved in executive functions, error monitoring of top-down control, and maintaining an extended task-dependent cognitive state [38], [39]. The FPN is primarily involved in attentional control and contributes to cognitive abilities such as response anticipation and conflict processing [39][41]. The FPN and the SMN overlap in their anatomy (Figure 1) and both are involved in top-down control of motor planning and execution [32], [42], [43]. Critically, several studies revealed the FPN actively participate in the parcellation of long motor sequences into smaller motor segments [44], [45]– a process that improves motor performance accuracy and efficiency by reducing required overall cognitive load [46]. Older adults are less capable of performing motor sequence segmentation and researchers proposed this may be due to aging-related structural alteration in the FPN among the elderly [45]. Thus, examining the functional connectivity between these two networks may be of particular relevance to understanding the neural mechanisms underlying the association between cognitive function and mobility.

Given that impaired mobility precedes cognitive impairment and dementia [2], [4], and is significantly associated with white matter lesions [10], [11], [14], [47], we hypothesize that disrupted functional connectivity may be observed among older adults with impaired mobility without dementia and may be a neural mechanism underlying the association between reduced cognitive function and impaired mobility. Thus, the objectives of this exploratory study were: 1) to compare functional connectivity (within-network and between-network) of four brain networks, DMN, FEN, FPN, and SMN, using task-free and task-based conditions between community-dwelling older adults with a history of ≥2 falls in the last 12 months [48] (i.e., fallers) and their non-falling counterparts; and 2) to determine if differences in functional connectivity between fallers and non-fallers are associated with changes in cognitive function and mobility over a 12-month period. Specifically, we hypothesize that compared with non-fallers, fallers will demonstrate disrupted functional connectivity: 1) between the SMN and FPN during both task-free and task-based conditions; 2) between the SMN and FEN during task-based condition; and 3) within the DMN during task-free condition. Furthermore, we hypothesize that these differences in functional connectivity will be significantly associated with changes in cognitive function and mobility over 12 months. If our hypothesis is supported, a recent history of multiple falls among older adults without a diagnosis of dementia may be a biomarker of sub-clinical changes in brain function and increased risk for subsequent decline.

Materials and Methods

2.1 Study Design and Participants

We conducted a 12-month prospective exploratory study with 44 older adults. Participants were recruited from metropolitan Vancouver via newspaper advertisements. Individuals were eligible if they: 1) were aged 70 to 80 years; 2) scored ≥24/30 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [49]; 3) were right hand dominant as measured by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [50]; 4) were living independently in their own homes; 5) had visual acuity of at least 20/40, with or without corrective lenses; and 6) provided informed consent. We excluded those who: 1) had a neurodegenerative disease, stroke, dementia (of any type), or psychiatric condition; 2) had clinically significant peripheral neuropathy or severe musculoskeletal or joint disease; 3) were taking psychotropic medication; 4) had a history indicative of carotid sinus sensitivity; 5) were living in a nursing home, extended care facility, or assisted-care facility; or 6) did not meet MRI scanning requirements.

Based on their falls history in the 12 months prior to study entry, participants were classified as a faller or non-faller (see 2.1.1 and 2.1.2). Ethics approval was obtained from the Vancouver Coastal Research Health Institute and University of British Columbia’s Clinical Research Ethics Board. All participants provided written consent.

2.1.1 Specific inclusion criterion for fallers.

An individual must have experienced ≥2 minimal displacement non-syncopal falls in the previous 12 months, with one of the falls occurring in the last 6 months [51]. This was determined from two sources: 1) participant recall; and 2) participant’s immediate family member or friend recall. Falls were defined as “unintentionally coming to rest on the ground, floor, or lower level[52].

2.1.2 Specific inclusion criterion for non-fallers.

An individual must not have experienced >1 displacement falls (with or without syncope) in the previous 12 months. This was determined based on two sources: 1) participant recall; and 2) participant’s immediate family member or friend recall. Individuals with one fall (non-injurious) in the previous 12 months resemble the physiological profile of non-fallers [48], [53]. Specifically, a prospective study found that while multiple falls (i.e., ≥2 falls) over 12 months were significantly associated with musculoskeletal and neurological deficits, single falls were not [48]. Importantly, older adults with a single fall were very similar to non-fallers in their physical and mental status.

2.2 Measurement

All measures, with the exception of neuroimaging, were assessed at baseline and 12 months. All assessors were trained and standardized protocols were used.

2.2.1 Global cognition and current physical activity level.

Global cognition was assessed using the MMSE [49] and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [54]. The MoCA is a 30-point test that covers multiple cognitive domains. The MoCA has been found to have good internal consistency and test-retest reliability and was able to correctly identify 90% of a large sample of individuals with mild cognitive impairment from two different clinics with a cut-off scores of <26/30 [54]. Current level of physical activity (i.e., last 7 days) was determined by the Physical Activities Scale for the Elderly (PASE) self-report questionnaire [55].

2.2.2 Comorbidity and depression.

Comorbidities were assessed with the Functional Comorbidity Index (FCI) [56], a 21-item questionnaire that calculates the total number of comorbidities associated with physical functioning [56]. We used the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [57], [58] to indicate the presence of depression; a score of ≥5 indicates depression [59].

2.2.3 Falls-related self-efficacy.

Self-efficacy is associated with cognitive function [60], [61], mobility [62][64], and brain volume [65]. In this study, we assessed falls-related self-efficacy on mobility-related tasks using the 16-item Activities-specific Balance Confidence (ABC) Scale [57]. Each item is rated from 0% (no confidence) to 100% (complete confidence) and a score out of 100 is calculated.

2.2.4 Physiological falls risk.

Physiological falls risk was assessed using the short form of the Physiological Profile Assessment (PPA). The PPA is a valid [58], [59] and reliable [60] measure of falls risk. Based on a participant’s performance in five physiological domains – postural sway, reaction time, strength, proprioception, and vision – the PPA computes a falls risk score (standardized score) that has a 75% predictive accuracy for falls among older people [53], [66]. A PPA Z-score of ≥0.60 indicates high physiological falls risk [67].

2.2.5 Mobility and balance.

Mobility and balance were assessed using the Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB) [68] and the Timed-Up-and-Go Test (TUG) [69]. For the Short Physical Performance Battery, participants were assessed on performances of standing balance, walking, and sit-to-stand. Each component is rated out of four points, for a maximum of 12 points; a score <9/12 predicts subsequent disability [70]. For the TUG, participants rose from a standard chair, walked a distance of three meters, turned, walked back to the chair and sat down [69]. We recorded the time (s) to complete the TUG, based on the average of two separate trials.

2.2.6 Executive functions.

We used: 1) the Stroop Test [71] to assess selective attention and conflict resolution; 2) the Trail Making Tests (Part A & B) to assess set shifting [72]; and 3) the verbal digits forward and backward tests to index working memory [73]. For the Stroop Test [71], participants first read out words printed in black ink (e.g., BLUE). Second, they named the display colour of coloured-X’s. Finally, they were shown a page with colour-words printed in incongruent coloured inks (e.g., the word “BLUE” printed in red ink). Participants were asked to name the ink colour in which the words were printed (while ignoring the word itself). We recorded the time participants took to read the items in each condition and calculated the time difference between the third condition (Stroop 3) and the second condition (Stroop 2). Smaller time differences indicate better selective attention and conflict resolution performance.

For the Trail Making Tests (Part A & B) [74], participants were required to draw lines connecting encircled numbers sequentially (Part A) or alternating between numbers and letters (Part B). The difference in time to complete Part B and Part A was calculated, with smaller difference scores indicating better set shifting performance.

For the Verbal Digits Forward and Backward Tests [75], participants repeated progressively longer random number sequences in the same order as presented (forward) and the reversed order (backward). Successful performance on the verbal digits span backward test represents a measure of central executive function due to the additional requirement of manipulation of information within temporary storage [76]. Thus, we subtracted the verbal digits backward test score from the verbal digits forward test score to provide an index of working memory with smaller difference scores indicating better working memory.

2.3 Functional MRI (fMRI)

All fMRI was performed at the UBC MRI Research Center located at the UBC Hospital on a 3.0 Tesla Intera Achieva MRI Scanner (Phillips Medical Systems Canada, Markham, Ontario) using an 8-channel SENSE neurovascular coil. The fMRI consisted of 166 dynamic images of 36 slices (3 mm thick) with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) of 2000 milliseconds (ms), echo time (TE) of 30 ms, flip angle (FA) of 90 degrees, field of view (FoV) of 240 mm, acquisition matrix 80×80. The high resolution T1 images were acquired using the following parameters: 170 slices (1 mm thick), TR of 7.7 ms, TE of 3.6 ms, FA of 8 degrees, FoV of 256 mm, acquisition matrix of 256×200.

2.3.1 Motor task.

During the fMRI scan, participants performed a simple finger tapping motor test that allows the examination of functional connectivity of networks both during rest and on task. This type of motor task is sensitive in differentiating older adults with Alzheimer’s disease from those with other types of dementia [77].

Our motor task consisted of three conditions: left finger tapping, right finger tapping, and rest. The participants performed finger tapping with the respective hands as indicated, starting with index finger and progressing outward to the little (pinky) finger. This finger tapping motion was continuously performed until the next condition was displayed. For the rest condition, the participants were asked to rest with their eyes open.

The specific order of the motor task blocks, which was not disclosed to the study participants, was counter-balanced over three runs as followed:

Run 1: Rest, Left, Rest, Right, Rest, Left, Rest, Right, Rest

Run 2: Rest, Right, Rest, Left, Rest, Right, Rest, Left, Rest

Run 3: Rest, Left, Rest, Left, Rest, Right, Rest, Right, Rest

Each run contained nine short blocks of 34 seconds, and the duration of each run was 330.897 seconds.

2.4 Data Analysis

2.4.1 Functional MRI data preprocessing.

Image preprocessing was carried out using tools from FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library) [78], MATLAB (Matrix Laboratory), and toolboxes from SPM (Statistical Parametric Mapping). Excess unwanted structures (i.e., bones, skull, etc.) in high resolution T1 images were removed via Brain Extraction Tool (BET); rigid body motion correction was completed using MCFLIRT (absolute and relative mean displacement were subsequently extracted and included in the statistical analysis as covariates); spatial smoothing was carried out using Gaussian kernel of Full-Width-Half-Maximum (FWHM) 6.0 mm; temporal filtering was applied with high pass frequency cut-off of 120 seconds. In addition, a low pass temporal filtering was also included to ensure the fMRI signal fluctuated between 0.008<f<0.080 Hz, the ideal bandwidth to examine functional connectivity. Furthermore, the application of a low pass filter eliminated high frequency signals that could be confounds. Participants’ low-resolution functional data were registered to personal high resolution T1 anatomical images, which were subsequently registered to standardized 152 T1 Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.

Noise generated from both physiological and non-physiological sources were removed through regression of the cerebral-spinal fluid (CSF) signal, white matter signal, and global brain signal. In addition, excess movement of the study participant was corrected by regression of motion parameters.

2.4.2 Functional connectivity analysis.

Previous studies guided our choice of seeds in the whole brain analysis of the DMN, FEN, FPN, and SMN [16], [32], [35]. The DMN included the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), ventral and superior frontal medial cortices (FMC), middle temporal gyrus (MTG), parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), middle frontal gyrus (MFG), and lateral occipital cortex (LOC) [20], [37]. The FEN included the anterior lateral prefrontal cortex (RALPFC), insular sulcus (INS), prefrontal cortex (PFC), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and anterior cingulate gyrus (CING) [38]. The FPN included the inferior parietal sulcus (IPS), ventral visual cortex (VV), supramarginal gyrus (SMG), superior lateral occipital cortex (SLOC), frontal eye field (FEF), as well as overlapping areas in the temporal-parietal junction [38]. The SMN included the primary motor cortex (PCG), cerebellum (CB), premotor area (PM), and supplementary motor area (SMA) [32]. We examined the left and right SMN individually because the use of dominant or non-dominant hand evokes different neural activity between the hemispheres [79]. Thus, examining each hemispheric SMN separately provides a better understanding of their temporal coherence with other networks during finger tapping. The respective MNI space coordinates for each region of interest (ROI) are provided in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Neural Networks and Regions of Interests Included in the Analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093673.t001

From each ROI, preprocessed time-series data were extracted with 14 mm spherical regions of interest drawn around their respective MNI coordinates in standard space. The different conditions (i.e., left, right, and rest) within each block of the motor task were extracted and compiled together [80]. To concatenate the time-series data, the stimulus onset time for each task condition was acquired from the task program. Each volume of the data was then sorted according to their respective condition. Once the data were properly categorized, the task-specific volumes (e.g. all the “left” volumes) were merged using a bash script provided in the FSL program. The first three volumes of any condition were discarded to account for delay of the hemodynamic response. To ensure our results were not affected by small number of time points in the fMRI data, we performed an addition analysis in which all task conditions were combined (please see Text S1 and Table S1 in Supplementary S1).

Region of interest time-series data were subsequently cross-correlated with every voxel within the brain to establish functional connectivity maps of their associated neural networks. Individual-level within-subject results were generated via ordinary least squares (OLS) in FSL by congregating the voxel-wise functional connectivity maps from each condition. Similarly, for group results, a mixed-level OLS analysis was conducted. The statistical map thresholding was set at Z = 2.33, with cluster correction of p<0.05. Pearson’s correlation coefficients were subsequently calculated between the ROI listed in Table 1.

2.4.3 Statistical analysis.

To normalize our data, the Pearson’s correlation coefficients (between the time-series of the seeded region and other voxels in the brain) were converted into Fisher’s z correlation coefficients via Fisher’s r-to-z transformation [81] in MATLAB. Fisher’s transformation generates normally distributed sample distribution and ensures the variance of the correlation coefficient remain constant for all values in the sample population correlation [81].

To reduce Type I error and potentially produce a more robust signal, we reduced the number of comparisons by averaging all the ROI-pairs within each of the four networks as well as all ROI-pairs between the networks. For example, the DMN contains the following 6 ROIs: PCC, FMC, RMTG, RPHG, LMFG, and RLOC. Six ROIs result in 15 ROI-pairs. To calculate the mean network correlation for the DMN, we totalled the 15 Fisher’s z correlation coefficients and then divided the total value by 15.

Data were analyzed using the IBM SPSS Statistic 19 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). Descriptive data are reported for variables of interest. Comparisons of group characteristics at baseline were undertaken using a Chi Square test for differences in proportions and ANOVAs for differences in means. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was performed to statistically test for significant between-group differences in mean network functional connectivity. In the model, baseline FCI, baseline mean ABC Scale score [62][64], and mean relative head motion (extracted from McFLIRT) [82], [83] were included as covariates. The overall alpha value was set at p≤0.05. Overall, 18 within-network and 27 between-network group comparisons in functional connectivity were performed.

Finally, Pearson correlations were computed to determine whether significant differences in mean network functional connectivity observed at baseline between fallers and non-fallers were significantly associated with changes in cognitive function and mobility over a 12-month period. Change for all measures of cognitive function and mobility was calculated as: 12-month value minus baseline value. For example, change in Stroop Test performance was calculated as: (12-month Stroop 3 completion time –12-month Stroop 2 completion time) – (baseline Stroop 3 completion time – baseline Stroop 2 completion time). Hence, for the Stroop Test, negative change values reflect improvement. Conversely, positive change values for SPPB reflect improvement. Overall, 36 correlation comparisons were conducted.

Results

3.1 Participant

Of the 44 participants, 23 were classified as fallers and 21 were classified as non-fallers. Among the 21 non-fallers, six participants had 1 falls in the prior 12 months. Number of falls 12-month prior to the study ranged from 2–20 in the fallers group; 0–1 in the non-fallers group. Other than the number of falls reported in the previous 12 months, fallers and non-fallers were not significantly different at baseline, including falls risk as measured by the PPA (Table 2). Based on the mean MoCA scores, our participants had mild cognitive impairment. Notably, the proportion of individuals with MoCA scores <26/30 was not significantly different between the two groups. No differences between the two groups were detected 12 months later (Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Twelve-Month Follow-Up Description of Participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093673.t003

3.2 Functional Connectivity of Fallers versus Non-fallers

3.2.1 Mean within-network connectivity.

There were no significant differences in within-network connectivity between fallers and non-fallers (Table 4). Overall, compared with non-fallers, fallers exhibit a non-significant trend for less functional connectivity within each network across all conditions (p≥0.12).

3.2.2 Mean between-network connectivity.

Figure 2 reports the between-network connectivity correlation coefficients. Compared with non-fallers, fallers showed greater connectivity between the DMN and FPN during right hand finger tapping (p = 0.04) (Figure 2a; Table 5). Furthermore, Compared with non-fallers, fallers demonstrated significantly less connectivity between the left hemispheric SMN and FPN (p = 0.02) during rest (Figure 2b; Table 5), and between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during rest (p = 0.05) and left hand finger tapping (p = 0.03) (Figure 2c; Table 5).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Graphical Representations of Between-Network Connectivity Significantly Different Between Fallers and Non-Fallers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093673.g002

thumbnail
Table 5. Between-Network Functional Connectivity Results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093673.t005

3.2.3 Correlation results.

We found connectivity between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during rest was significantly associated with change in Stroop Test performance (Pearson’s r = −0.32, p = 0.04; Spearman’s r =  −0.21, p = 0.18) Table 6; Figure 3a) and SPPB performance (Pearson’s r = 0.33, p = 0.03; Spearman’s r =  −0.27, p = 0.09; Table 6; Figure 3b). Specifically, less connectivity between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during rest, as demonstrated by fallers at baseline, was associated with greater reductions in Stroop Test and SPPB performance over the 12-month study period. As indicated in Figure 3a, the association between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during rest and change in Stroop Test performance may be influenced by an extreme (positive) score. Hence, we also calculated the Spearman correlation, which was r = −0.21 (p = 0.18).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Correlations of Behavioural Performances and Functional Connectivity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093673.g003

thumbnail
Table 6. Correlations Between Change in Performance Measures and Baseline Functional Connectivity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093673.t006

Discussion

Findings from our exploratory study suggest that community-dwelling older fallers without dementia may have disrupted functional connectivity between large neural networks. Specifically, compared with non-fallers, fallers demonstrated greater connectivity between the DMN and FPN during right hand finger tapping and less connectivity between the SMN and FPN during rest and left hand finger tapping. Importantly, less connectivity between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during rest was significantly associated with greater decline in mobility over the 12-month period. There was also suggestion that less connectivity between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during rest was associated with greater decline in cognitive function as measured by the Stroop Test. Thus, a recent history of multiple falls among older adults without a diagnosis of dementia may indicate sub-clinical changes in brain function and increased risk for subsequent decline.

The finding of increased connectivity between the DMN and FPN among older fallers during right hand finger tapping condition provides novel insight into the neural risk factors for falls. It is widely recognized that the DMN deactivates during goal-oriented activity [36], [37]. In accord with this concept, evidence in the literature found an association between reduced cognitive performance and aging-related increase in functional connectivity between the DMN and FPN [84]. Thus, our results suggest that fallers may have reduced ability to disengage from internally-generated thoughts when performing a task. In particular, our participants were right hand dominant and thus, right finger tapping was less cognitively demanding than left finger tapping [85]. According to the capacity model of attention, during a less cognitively-demanding task, more resources are available to distribute to task-irrelevant stimuli or thoughts [86]. Hence, we speculate that the reduced ability to attend externally by disengaging from thoughts focused internally, or increased propensity for mind-wandering, may contribute to falls risk among older adults. Certainly there is emerging evidence to suggest that mind-wandering negatively impacts motor control [87], [88].

Majority of the differences we found in functional connectivity were between the SMN and FPN. This supports our original hypothesis that these two functionally- and anatomically-overlapping networks (Figure 1) are of specific interest in understanding the neural basis for the association between cognitive function and mobility. Our observation that connectivity between the SMN and FPN during rest was less among fallers than non-fallers concurs and extends previous findings. Inman and colleagues [89] also found less connectivity between the SMN and FPN during rest in stroke survivors – a population that is also at significant risk for falls and dementia [90][92]. Less connectivity between these two networks during rest may suggest reduced motor preparatory inputs, in anticipation of motor performance, from FPN to the SMN. This, in turn, may increase falls risk. Our fMRI design may have been particularly sensitive to evoking anticipation because the rest periods were interspersed among the finger tapping blocks. Using event related potentials, Berchicci and colleagues [93] recently demonstrated that older adults require greater motor preparation (as indexed by earlier onset latency onset and greater prefrontal cortex activation) than young adults to obtain the same level of motor performance.

Compared with non-fallers, fallers also demonstrated less connectivity between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during left finger tapping. Evidence suggests that compared with dominant hand use, non-dominant hand use requires greater neural resources (bilateral recruitment of the brain) to maintain stable motion [79], [85]. Given our participants were right hand dominant, one would expect greater connectivity between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during left finger tapping than right finger tapping. Importantly, we did observe the mean network connectivity between right hemispheric SMN and FPN during left finger tapping (i.e., 0.19) was greater than the mean network connectivity left hemispheric SMN and FPN right finger tapping (i.e., 0.17). However, these connectivity values were not significantly different (p = 0.78). Thus, this finding suggests that reduced temporal coherence between the SMN and FPN during novel or challenging motor tasks may increase falls risk among older adults.

Furthermore, we found that less functional connectivity between the right hemispheric SMN and FPN during rest was significantly associated with greater decline in both cognitive function and mobility over the 12-month period. This finding further supports the hypothesis that both cognitive decline and impaired mobility may share common neurobiological mechanisms.

We did not observe any significant group differences in the functional connectivity between the SMN and FEN. This null finding could be because the motor task does not specifically engage executive processes, such as working memory or maintenance of a complex task set. We also did not observe any significant group differences within the DMN. Changes within the DMN have been observed in healthy aging [22], [94], [95], Alzheimer’s disease [34], [95], [96], individuals with amnestic mild cognitive impairment [97], and in APOE-ε4 carriers [98], [99]. Hence, the lack of significant differences between fallers and non-fallers in age, MoCA score, MMSE score, and executive functions, may explain our null finding within the DMN. Our findings may also suggest that between-network functional connectivity disruptions may precede within-network connectivity disruptions in older adults without a diagnosis of dementia. However, future studies are needed to confirm the temporal order of functional connectivity disruptions with both healthy aging and neurodegeneration.

It is noteworthy that despite observing significant differences in functional connectivity between fallers and non-fallers, we found no differences in measures of falls risk, mobility and balance, and cognitive function. Importantly, based on the performance on the MoCA, the number of mild cognitively impaired individuals was equally distributed between fallers and non-fallers (Table 2). The only clinical characteristic that significantly distinguished the two groups was the number of falls in the 12 months prior to enrollment in the study. A previous prospective study also demonstrated that falls occur prior to cognitive changes among community-dwelling older adults [100]. This prior observation and our current findings support our hypothesis that a recent history of multiple falls may be a biomarker of sub-clinical changes in brain function and increased risk for subsequent decline among community-dwelling older adults without dementia.

We recognize the limitations of our study. The validity of our findings depends on accurate identification of recurrent fallers and non-fallers and previous research has demonstrated that falls recall in older adults is subject to retrospective recall bias [101]. However, we corroborated falls history with immediate family members or close friends. Moreover, fallers had a mean baseline PPA score of 0.60, indicating high physiological falls risk [67]. Our classification scheme separated fallers and non-fallers by 1 fall in the last 12 months (i.e., ≥2 falls versus ≤1 fall) and thus, we may have underestimated the association between falls and disrupted functional connectivity. Future research may wish to classify fallers as those with 3 or more falls in the last 12 months.

Differences in functional connectivity between fallers and non-fallers may be dependent on the specific task imposed. Thus, the generalizability of our findings to other task states will be an important direction of future research. Our functional connectivity analysis was completed with a relatively low number of data points (i.e., 51 per condition) and therefore, may not reflect an ideal signal-to-noise ratio. Potential confounders not accounted for in this study include APOE ε4 genotype; recent evidence report that functional connectivity of large neural networks is influenced by the presence of APOE ε4 allele. We also did not quantify white matter lesions and this should be an important consideration for future functional connectivity research. While we substantially reduced the number of comparison by averaging the ROI-pairs, multiple comparisons were still performed, rendering the results susceptible to Type I error. Notably, results of this exploratory study would no longer be statistically significant if Bonferroni correction (i.e., divide the alpha by the total number of comparisons being made) was applied to our data. Finally, seed-based functional connectivity analysis restricted our results within the scope of the identified regions of interest. Therefore, to extend our results beyond the list of regions and networks presented in this paper, future research should consider data-driven methods of analysis (e.g., Independent Component Analysis).

Our study sample consisted exclusively of independent community-dwelling older adults specifically between the age-range of 70–80 who were without significant physical or cognitive impairment. Thus, our results may not generalize beyond this population of older adults.

In summary, the results of our exploratory study suggest that community-dwelling older adults with a recent history of multiple falls may have disrupted functional connectivity between large neural networks. Importantly, these disruptions may be associated with greater decline in both cognitive function and mobility over a 12-month period. Thus, health care professionals working with older adults should consider falls history in their assessment to potentially identify those who are at greater risk for subsequent decline. Lastly, future studies with larger samples will need to be conducted in order to confirm our exploratory findings.

Supporting Information

Supplementary S1.

Supporting information. Text S1. Combined Analysis. Table S1. Results from Combined Analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093673.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

Teresa Liu-Ambrose is a Canada Research Chair in Physical Activity, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience, a Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research (MSFHR) Scholar, a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) New Investigator, and a Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada’s (HSFC) Henry JM Barnett’s Scholarship recipient. Jennifer Davis is a MSFHR and a CIHR Post-Doctoral Fellow. Lindsay Nagamatsu is a MSFHR Senior Graduate trainee and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Doctoral trainee. Niousha Bolandzadeh is a HSFC Doctoral trainee. Chun Liang Hsu is an Alzheimer’s Society Research Program Doctoral trainee.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CLH MWV TCH JCD LSN AC NB TLA. Performed the experiments: CLH MWV TCH JCD LSN AC NB TLA. Analyzed the data: CLH MWV TLA. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MWV TLA. Wrote the paper: CLH MWV TCH JCD LSN AC NB TLA. Seed-based functional connectivity analysis technique: MWV.

References

  1. 1. Rosano C, Newman AB, Katz R, Hirsch CH, Kuller LH (2008) Association Between Lower Digit Symbol Substitution Test Score and Slower Gait and Greater Risk of Mortality and of Developing Incident Disability in Well-Functioning Older Adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 56: 1618–1625.
  2. 2. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Hall CB, Kuslansky G, Katz MJ, et al. (2002) Abnormality of gait as a predictor of non-Alzheimer’s dementia. N Engl J Med 347: 1761–1768.
  3. 3. Inzitari M, Newman AB, Yaffe K, Boudreau R, de Rekeneire N, et al. (2007) Gait speed predicts decline in attention and psychomotor speed in older adults: the health aging and body composition study. Neuroepidemiology 29: 156–162.
  4. 4. Buracchio T, Dodge HH, Howieson D, Wasserman D, Kaye J (2010) The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 67: 980–986.
  5. 5. Atkinson HH, Rosano C, Simonsick EM, Williamson JD, Davis C, et al. (2007) Cognitive function, gait speed decline, and comorbidities: the health, aging and body composition study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 62: 844–850.
  6. 6. Madden DJ, Bennett IJ, Burzynska A, Potter GG, Chen NK, et al. (2012) Diffusion tensor imaging of cerebral white matter integrity in cognitive aging. Biochim Biophys Acta 1822: 386–400.
  7. 7. Kantarci K, Senjem ML, Avula R, Zhang B, Samikoglu AR, et al. (2011) Diffusion tensor imaging and cognitive function in older adults with no dementia. Neurology 77: 26–34.
  8. 8. Van Impe A, Coxon JP, Goble DJ, Doumas M, Swinnen SP (2012) White matter fractional anisotropy predicts balance performance in older adults. Neurobiol Aging 33: 1900–1912.
  9. 9. Kafri M, Sasson E, Assaf Y, Balash Y, Aiznstein O, et al.. (2012) High-Level Gait Disorder: Associations with Specific White Matter Changes Observed on Advanced Diffusion Imaging. J Neuroimaging.
  10. 10. Rosano C, Kuller LH, Chung H, Arnold AM, Longstreth WT Jr, et al. (2005) Subclinical brain magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities predict physical functional decline in high-functioning older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 53: 649–654.
  11. 11. Kuo HK, Lipsitz LA (2004) Cerebral white matter changes and geriatric syndromes: is there a link? J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 59: 818–826.
  12. 12. Davis SW, Dennis NA, Buchler NG, White LE, Madden DJ, et al. (2009) Assessing the effects of age on long white matter tracts using diffusion tensor tractography. Neuroimage 46: 530–541.
  13. 13. Della Nave R, Foresti S, Pratesi A, Ginestroni A, Inzitari M, et al. (2007) Whole-brain histogram and voxel-based analyses of diffusion tensor imaging in patients with leukoaraiosis: correlation with motor and cognitive impairment. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 28: 1313–1319.
  14. 14. de Laat KF, Tuladhar AM, van Norden AG, Norris DG, Zwiers MP, et al. (2011) Loss of white matter integrity is associated with gait disorders in cerebral small vessel disease. Brain 134: 73–83.
  15. 15. Madden DJ, Spaniol J, Costello MC, Bucur B, White LE, et al. (2009) Cerebral white matter integrity mediates adult age differences in cognitive performance. J Cogn Neurosci 21: 289–302.
  16. 16. Andrews-Hanna JR, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Lustig C, Head D, et al. (2007) Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56: 924–935.
  17. 17. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P (2005) The implications of cortical recruitment and brain morphology for individual differences in inhibitory function in aging humans. Psychol Aging 20: 363–375.
  18. 18. Marquez de la Plata CD, Garces J, Shokri Kojori E, Grinnan J, Krishnan K, et al. (2011) Deficits in functional connectivity of hippocampal and frontal lobe circuits after traumatic axonal injury. Arch Neurol 68: 74–84.
  19. 19. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS (1993) Functional connectivity: the principal-component analysis of large (PET) data sets. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 13: 5–14.
  20. 20. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, et al. (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 9673–9678.
  21. 21. Mennes M, Kelly C, Colcombe S, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2013) The extrinsic and intrinsic functional architectures of the human brain are not equivalent. Cereb Cortex 23: 223–229.
  22. 22. Damoiseaux JS, Beckmann CF, Arigita EJ, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, et al. (2008) Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb Cortex 18: 1856–1864.
  23. 23. Tomasi D, Volkow ND (2012) Aging and functional brain networks. Mol Psychiatry 17: 471, 549–458.
  24. 24. Esposito F, Aragri A, Pesaresi I, Cirillo S, Tedeschi G, et al. (2008) Independent component model of the default-mode brain function: combining individual-level and population-level analyses in resting-state fMRI. Magn Reson Imaging 26: 905–913.
  25. 25. Spreng RN, Schacter DL (2012) Default network modulation and large-scale network interactivity in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex 22: 2610–2621.
  26. 26. Onoda K, Ishihara M, Yamaguchi S. Aging effects on functional connectivity within and between resting-state networks; 2012 1–4 July. 2012: 119–124.
  27. 27. Brier MR, Thomas JB, Snyder AZ, Benzinger TL, Zhang D, et al. (2012) Loss of intranetwork and internetwork resting state functional connections with Alzheimer’s disease progression. J Neurosci 32: 8890–8899.
  28. 28. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Lustig C (2005) Brain aging: reorganizing discoveries about the aging mind. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15: 245–251.
  29. 29. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P (2009) The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol 60: 173–196.
  30. 30. Persson J, Nyberg L, Lind J, Larsson A, Nilsson LG, et al. (2006) Structure-function correlates of cognitive decline in aging. Cereb Cortex 16: 907–915.
  31. 31. Vidoni ED, Thomas GP, Honea RA, Loskutova N, Burns JM (2012) Evidence of altered corticomotor system connectivity in early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Phys Ther 36: 8–16.
  32. 32. Wu T, Wang L, Chen Y, Zhao C, Li K, et al. (2009) Changes of functional connectivity of the motor network in the resting state in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett 460: 6–10.
  33. 33. Greicius M (2008) Resting-state functional connectivity in neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr Opin Neurol 21: 424–430.
  34. 34. Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V (2004) Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101: 4637–4642.
  35. 35. Voss MW, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Basak C, Chaddock L, et al.. (2010) Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2.
  36. 36. Greicius MD, Menon V (2004) Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J Cogn Neurosci 16: 1484–1492.
  37. 37. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL (2008) The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124: 1–38.
  38. 38. Dosenbach NU, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, et al. (2006) A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50: 799–812.
  39. 39. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, et al. (2007) Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 27: 2349–2356.
  40. 40. Fogassi L, Luppino G (2005) Motor functions of the parietal lobe. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15: 626–631.
  41. 41. Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V (2008) A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105: 12569–12574.
  42. 42. Wise SP, Boussaoud D, Johnson PB, Caminiti R (1997) Premotor and parietal cortex: corticocortical connectivity and combinatorial computations. Annu Rev Neurosci 20: 25–42.
  43. 43. Wymbs NF, Bassett DS, Mucha PJ, Porter MA, Grafton ST (2012) Differential recruitment of the sensorimotor putamen and frontoparietal cortex during motor chunking in humans. Neuron 74: 936–946.
  44. 44. Pammi VS, Miyapuram KP, Ahmed, Samejima K, Bapi RS, et al. (2012) Changing the structure of complex visuo-motor sequences selectively activates the fronto-parietal network. Neuroimage 59: 1180–1189.
  45. 45. Verwey WB (2010) Diminished motor skill development in elderly: indications for limited motor chunk use. Acta Psychol (Amst) 134: 206–214.
  46. 46. Verwey WB (2001) Concatenating familiar movement sequences: the versatile cognitive processor. Acta Psychol (Amst) 106: 69–95.
  47. 47. Starr JM, Leaper SA, Murray AD, Lemmon HA, Staff RT, et al. (2003) Brain white matter lesions detected by magnetic resonance [correction of resosnance] imaging are associated with balance and gait speed. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 74: 94–98.
  48. 48. Nevitt MC, Cummings SR, Kidd S, Black D (1989) Risk factors for recurrent nonsyncopal falls. A prospective study. JAMA 261: 2663–2668.
  49. 49. Cockrell JR, Folstein MF (1988) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Psychopharmacol Bull 24: 689–692.
  50. 50. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113.
  51. 51. Delbaere K, Close JC, Heim J, Sachdev PS, Brodaty H, et al. (2010) A multifactorial approach to understanding fall risk in older people. J Am Geriatr Soc 58: 1679–1685.
  52. 52. Hauer K, Lamb SE, Jorstad EC, Todd C, Becker C (2006) Systematic review of definitions and methods of measuring falls in randomised controlled fall prevention trials. Age Ageing 35: 5–10.
  53. 53. Lord S, Clark R, Webster I (1991) Physiological factors associated with falls in an elderly population. Journal of American Geriatrics Society 39: 1194–1200.
  54. 54. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, et al. (2005) The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 53: 695–699.
  55. 55. Washburn RA, McAuley E, Katula J, Mihalko SL, Boileau RA (1999) The physical activity scale for the elderly (PASE): Evidence for validity. J Clin Epidemiol 52: 643–651.
  56. 56. Groll DL, To T, Bombardier C, Wright JG (2005) The development of a comorbidity index with physical function as the outcome. J Clin Epidemiol 58: 595–602.
  57. 57. Yesavage JA (1988) Geriatric Depression Scale. Psychopharmacol Bull 24: 709–711.
  58. 58. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, et al. (1982) Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 17: 37–49.
  59. 59. van Marwijk HW, Wallace P, de Bock GH, Hermans J, Kaptein AA, et al. (1995) Evaluation of the feasibility, reliability and diagnostic value of shortened versions of the geriatric depression scale. Br J Gen Pract 45: 195–199.
  60. 60. Seeman T, McAvay G, Merrill S, Albert M, Rodin J (1996) Self-efficacy beliefs and change in cognitive performance: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Psychol Aging 11: 538–551.
  61. 61. Seeman TE, Unger JB, McAvay G, Mendes de Leon CF (1999) Self-efficacy beliefs and perceived declines in functional ability: MacArthur studies of successful aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 54: P214–222.
  62. 62. Davis JC, Nagamatsu LS, Hsu CL, Beattie BL, Liu-Ambrose T (2012) Self-efficacy is independently associated with brain volume in older women. Age Ageing 41: 495–501.
  63. 63. Liu-Ambrose T, Khan KM, Donaldson MG, Eng JJ, Lord SR, et al. (2006) Falls-related self-efficacy is independently associated with balance and mobility in older women with low bone mass. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 61: 832–838.
  64. 64. Liu-Ambrose T, Katarynych LA, Ashe MC, Nagamatsu LS, Hsu CL (2009) Dual-task gait performance among community-dwelling senior women: the role of balance confidence and executive functions. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 64: 975–982.
  65. 65. Davis JC, Nagamatsu LS, Hsu CL, Beattie BL, Liu-Ambrose T (2012) Self-efficacy is independently associated with brain volume in older women. Age Ageing.
  66. 66. Lord S, Ward J, Williams P, Anstey K (1994) Physiological factors associated with falls in older community-dwelling women. Journal of American Geriatrics Society 42: 1110–1117.
  67. 67. Delbaere K, Close JCT, Brodaty H, Sachdev P, Lord SR (2010) Determinants of disparities between perceived and physiological risk o falling among elderly people: cohort study. British Medical Journal.
  68. 68. Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L, Simonsick EM, Salive ME, Wallace RB (1995) Lower-extremity function in persons over the age of 70 years as a predictor of subsequent disability. N Engl J Med 332: 556–561.
  69. 69. Shumway-Cook A, Brauer S, Woollacott M (2000) Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults using the Timed Up & Go Test. Phys Ther 80: 896–903.
  70. 70. Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L, Simonsick EM, Salive ME, Wallace RB (1995) Lower-Extremity Function in Persons over the Age of 70 Years as a Predictor of Subsequent Disability 10.1056/NEJM199503023320902. N Engl J Med 332: 556–562.
  71. 71. Graf P, Uttl B, Tuokko H (1995) Color- and picture-word Stroop tests: performance changes in old age. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 17: 390–415.
  72. 72. Spreen O, Strauss E (1998) A compendium of neurological tests. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc. 736 p.
  73. 73. Wechsler D (1981) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised.: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
  74. 74. Spreen O SE (1998) A Compendium of Neurological Tests. ed 2nd Edition, New York, Oxford University Press, Inc.
  75. 75. Wechsler D (1981) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised. In, The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
  76. 76. Baddeley A (1992) Working memory. Science 255: 556–559.
  77. 77. Arnold G, Boone KB, Lu P, Dean A, Wen J, et al. (2005) Sensitivity and specificity of finger tapping test scores for the detection of suspect effort. Clin Neuropsychol 19: 105–120.
  78. 78. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, et al. (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23 Suppl 1S208–219.
  79. 79. Grabowska A, Gut M, Binder M, Forsberg L, Rymarczyk K, et al. (2012) Switching handedness: fMRI study of hand motor control in right-handers, left-handers and converted left-handers. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 72: 439–451.
  80. 80. Fair DA, Schlaggar BL, Cohen AL, Miezin FM, Dosenbach NU, et al. (2007) A method for using blocked and event-related fMRI data to study “resting state” functional connectivity. Neuroimage 35: 396–405.
  81. 81. Konishi S (1981) Normalizing transformations of some statistics in multivariate analysis. Biometrika 68: 647–651.
  82. 82. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2012) Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59: 2142–2154.
  83. 83. Van Dijk KR, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL (2012) The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 59: 431–438.
  84. 84. Geerligs L, Maurits NM, Renken RJ, Lorist MM (2012) Reduced specificity of functional connectivity in the aging brain during task performance. Hum Brain Mapp.
  85. 85. Aramaki Y, Osu R, Sadato N (2010) Resource-demanding versus cost-effective bimanual interaction in the brain. Exp Brain Res 203: 407–418.
  86. 86. Kahneman D (1973) Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall Inc.
  87. 87. Kam JW, Dao E, Blinn P, Krigolson OE, Boyd LA, et al. (2012) Mind wandering and motor control: off-task thinking disrupts the online adjustment of behavior. Front Hum Neurosci 6: 329.
  88. 88. Nagamatsu LS, Kam JW, Liu-Ambrose T, Chan A, Handy TC (2013) Mind-wandering and falls risk in older adults. Psychol Aging 28: 685–691.
  89. 89. Inman CS, James GA, Hamann S, Rajendra JK, Pagnoni G, et al. (2012) Altered resting-state effective connectivity of fronto-parietal motor control systems on the primary motor network following stroke. Neuroimage 59: 227–237.
  90. 90. Divani AA, Majidi S, Barrett AM, Noorbaloochi S, Luft AR (2011) Consequences of stroke in community-dwelling elderly: the health and retirement study, 1998 to 2008. Stroke 42: 1821–1825.
  91. 91. Weerdesteyn V, de Niet M, van Duijnhoven HJ, Geurts AC (2008) Falls in individuals with stroke. J Rehabil Res Dev 45: 1195–1213.
  92. 92. Kokmen E, Whisnant JP, O’Fallon WM, Chu CP, Beard CM (1996) Dementia after ischemic stroke: a population-based study in Rochester, Minnesota (1960–1984). Neurology 46: 154–159.
  93. 93. Berchicci M, Lucci G, Pesce C, Spinelli D, Di Russo F (2012) Prefrontal hyperactivity in older people during motor planning. Neuroimage 62: 1750–1760.
  94. 94. Persson J, Lustig C, Nelson JK, Reuter-Lorenz PA (2007) Age differences in deactivation: a link to cognitive control? J Cogn Neurosci 19: 1021–1032.
  95. 95. Lustig C, Snyder AZ, Bhakta M, O’Brien KC, McAvoy M, et al. (2003) Functional deactivations: change with age and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100: 14504–14509.
  96. 96. Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, Goekoop R, Stam CJ, Scheltens P (2005) Altered resting state networks in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 26: 231–239.
  97. 97. Qi Z, Wu X, Wang Z, Zhang N, Dong H, et al. (2010) Impairment and compensation coexist in amnestic MCI default mode network. Neuroimage 50: 48–55.
  98. 98. Persson J, Lind J, Larsson A, Ingvar M, Sleegers K, et al. (2008) Altered deactivation in individuals with genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 46: 1679–1687.
  99. 99. Fleisher AS, Sherzai A, Taylor C, Langbaum JB, Chen K, et al. (2009) Resting-state BOLD networks versus task-associated functional MRI for distinguishing Alzheimer’s disease risk groups. Neuroimage 47: 1678–1690.
  100. 100. Stark S, Roe C, Grant E, Morris J (2011) Risk of Falls Among Older Adults with Preclinical Alzheimer’s Disease. Alzheimers Dement 7: S176.
  101. 101. Cummings S, Nevitt M, Kidd S (1988) Forgetting falls. The limited accuracy of recall of falls in the elderly. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 36: 613–616.