Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Female Preference for Sympatric vs. Allopatric Male Throat Color Morphs in the Mesquite Lizard (Sceloporus grammicus) Species Complex

  • Elizabeth Bastiaans ,

    ejbastiaans@gmail.com

    Current address: Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Behavior, University of Minnesota, Falcon Heights, Minnesota, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, California, United States of America

  • Mary Jane Bastiaans,

    Affiliation Nanooptical Materials, Incorporated, Carson, California, United States of America

  • Gen Morinaga,

    Current address: Department of Biology, Clark University, Worcester, Massachusetts, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, California, United States of America

  • José Gamaliel Castañeda Gaytán,

    Affiliation Facultad en Ciencias Biológicas, Universidad Juárez del Estado de Durango, Gómez Palacio, Durango, México

  • Jonathon C. Marshall,

    Affiliation Department of Zoology, Weber State University, Ogden, Utah, United States of America

  • Brendan Bane,

    Affiliation Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, California, United States of America

  • Fausto Méndez de la Cruz,

    Affiliation Laboratorio de Herpetología, Departamento de Zoología, Instituto de Biología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Distrito Federal, México

  • Barry Sinervo

    Affiliation Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, California, United States of America

Abstract

Color polymorphic sexual signals are often associated with alternative reproductive behaviors within populations, and the number, frequency, or type of morphs present often vary among populations. When these differences lead to assortative mating by population, the study of such polymorphic taxa may shed light on speciation mechanisms. We studied two populations of a lizard with polymorphic throat color, an important sexual signal. Males in one population exhibit orange, yellow, or blue throats; whereas males in the other exhibit orange, yellow, or white throats. We assessed female behavior when choosing between allopatric and sympatric males. We asked whether females discriminated more when the allopatric male was of an unfamiliar morph than when the allopatric male was similar in coloration to the sympatric male. We found that female rejection of allopatric males relative to sympatric males was more pronounced when males in a pair were more different in throat color. Our findings may help illuminate how behavioral responses to color morph differences between populations with polymorphic sexual signals contribute to reproductive isolation.

Introduction

Discrete phenotypic polymorphisms in colorful sexual signals are associated with alternative reproductive tactics in a variety of taxa [1][9], including several lizards [3], [10], [11]. Taxa in which color polymorphisms or alternative reproductive tactics are common may diversify at higher rates than taxa that do not exhibit these characteristics [12][17], a hypothesis supported by the observation that closely related species or divergent populations of the same species often differ in their sexual signals [18], [19]. These differences may initially arise in allopatry [20][23], in parapatry [24], [25], after secondary contact [26][28], or in sympatry [29]31. Regardless of which evolutionary mechanism gave rise to divergent sexual signals in a particular system, there are numerous cases in which individuals mate preferentially with members of their own populations based on sexual signals that differ between populations or closely related species [31][37].

In several lizards in which color morphs vary in behavior within populations, there is also variation among populations in morph frequencies, or in which combinations of color morphs are present in particular populations. This pattern may represent an intermediate stage in the transition between polymorphic species in which all populations are linked by gene flow and dimorphic or monomorphic species which are reproductively isolated [16], [22], [38], [39]. A recent study assessed reproductive isolation between two parapatric populations of the color polymorphic lizard Uta stansburiana that differed in habitat, dorsal coloration, and number of throat color morphs present. Color morphs varied in the degree of postzygotic reproductive isolation they displayed in interpopulation crosses, indicating that which color morphs are present, and the frequencies at which they occur, may influence the degree of gene flow between color polymorphic populations [40].

Polymorphic coloration in lizards may be important in intrasexual signaling among both males and females [1], [3], [41][43] as well as intersexual signaling and/or mate choice [2], [44][48]. Lizards in which sexual color signals vary at both the intra- and interpopulation levels may therefore be ideal systems for understanding the importance of divergence in sexually selected traits before, during, and after speciation [14], [15], [19], [24].

The mesquite lizard (Sceloporus grammicus) species complex is an excellent system in which to investigate intrapopulation color and behavior polymorphisms, variation among populations in sexual signals, and the roles both kinds of variation may play in speciation. The species complex occurs in desert and montane habitats in Mexico. It consists of eight chromosome races, among which the diploid number varies from 32 to 46 [49][51]. This karyotypic variation may indicate that the S. grammicus complex is an example of incipient speciation [49], [51][53]. Several hybrid zones between different chromosomal races exist [54][58], with reduced hybrid fitness occurring in at least one case [59]. Populations of the S. grammicus complex also vary in life history, habitat use, and morphology [53], [60][64].

Intrapopulation throat color polymorphisms occur in both males and females of the S. grammicus species complex [65]. In some populations, males exhibit orange/yellow/blue color polymorphisms (Fig. 1A–F) that are phenotypically similar to the polymorphisms described in two confamilial lizards, Uta stansburiana [3] and Urosaurus ornatus [66]. In other populations, males exhibit orange/yellow/white throat color polymorphisms (Fig. 2A–F) that appear more similar to the throat color polymorphism in the congeneric Sceloporus consobrinus (formerly Sceloporus undulatus erythrocheilus) [67], [68]. Blue-throated and white-throated male S. grammicus have never been observed within the same population [69]. Female S. grammicus in both types of populations exhibit orange/yellow/white throat color polymorphisms (Fig. 1G–L, Fig. 2G–L).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Male and female throat color morphs present in S. grammicus at CPN.

A–F: Male morphs. A. Pure orange. B. Pure blue. C. Pure yellow. D. Yellow-orange. E. Blue-orange. F. Blue-yellow. G–L: Female morphs. G. Pure orange. H. Pure white. I. Pure yellow. J. Yellow-orange. K. White-orange. L. White-yellow. M. Frequencies of male and female throat color morphs among all lizards captured at CPN in 2011. Sample sizes above bars.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Male and female throat color morphs present in S. grammicus at SAA.

A–F: Male morphs. A. Pure orange. B. Pure White. C. Pure yellow. D. Yellow-orange. E. White-orange. F. White-yellow. G–L: Female morphs. G. Pure orange. H. Pure white. I. Pure yellow. J. Yellow-orange. K. White-orange. L. White-yellow. M. Frequencies of male and female throat color morphs among all lizards captured at SAA in 2011. Sample sizes above bars.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.g002

The presence of blue-throated males in some populations and white-throated males in others is the most striking feature of interpopulation sexual signal variation in the S. grammicus species complex, and there are cases in which populations with blue males occur in close proximity to populations with white males but retain their morph differences [69]. The circumstances under which the divergence in throat color originally occurred are not known. If the divergence in sexual signals initially occurred in sympatry or parapatry, or if the divergence began in allopatry but was completed in parapatry, blue vs. white variation might represent reproductive character displacement due to reinforcement [26], [70]. However, this hypothesis does not explain why the yellow and orange male morphs have not diverged. If the substitution of one morph for another was completed in allopatry, it may have occurred because some environments favor blue-throated males, whereas other environments favor white-throated males [19], [23], [30], [71]. Alternatively, divergent runaway processes in allopatry may have culminated in the presence of white-throated males in some populations but blue-throated males in others [12], [19], [72]. Under any of the above scenarios for the origin of the blue and white male morphs, females might be less likely to accept males as mates if those males exhibit a throat color that does not occur in the female’s home population. This prediction holds if females are reproductively incompatible with allopatric males and does not necessarily depend on how that incompatibility arose [6], [9], [36], [40], [72][73]. Our purpose in this study is not to distinguish between alternative hypotheses for how the blue and white male morphs initially arose, but rather to assess whether they currently have effects that could help explain patterns of gene flow within the S. grammicus species complex.

If females respond to male throat color, we expect females from populations with blue males to readily identify white-throated males as allopatric, whereas females from populations with white males readily identify blue-throated males as allopatric. When choosing between one sympatric and one allopatric male, we expect females to discriminate more when the allopatric male is of an unfamiliar morph, compared with choices between males of the same color morph. We tested this hypothesis using a repeated-measures, binary choice design. We predicted that individual females would reject allopatric males more strongly when choosing between allopatric and sympatric males differing completely in throat color than when choosing between allopatric and sympatric males that were more similar to one another in throat color.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

This project was conducted in strict accordance with guidelines from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Chancellor’s Animal Research Committee) at the University of California, Santa Cruz (permit Sineb0902). Lizards were collected under permits issued by the Secretaría del Medio Ambiente y Recursos Naturales of México (folio FAUT007). When sampling took place on private land, we obtained permission from property owners prior to collecting animals.

Field Sites

We used lizards from two field sites: Cerro Peña Nevada, Nuevo León (23.83154°N, 99.89381°W; 2800 m above sea level) and San Antonio de las Alazanas, Coahuila (25.22193°N, 100.39331°W; 2800 m above sea level). For brevity, we will refer to these two localities as CPN and SAA, respectively. Lizard habitat was similar at both sites and consisted primarily of conifers such as Pinus, Abies, and Pseudotsuga spp. [75], [76]. Additional details regarding these two field sites are provided in [65]. Male lizards at CPN exhibit an orange/yellow/blue throat color polymorphism (Fig. 1), whereas males at SAA exhibit an orange/yellow/white polymorphism (Fig. 2). The two localities are approximately 180 km apart [77], so there is probably no direct migration between them. However, both sites are located within the Sierra Madre Oriental mountain range, throughout which S. grammicus is common at high elevations [50], [78], [79], so we cannot rule out the possibility that CPN and SAA are linked by an unbroken chain of other populations. Sceloporus grammicus from these two localities are closely related, based on allozyme electrophoresis studies, and the two populations likely both belong to the same chromosomal race (2N = 32) [50], [79], which indicates they might not exhibit reduced hybrid fitness, although no tests of this hypothesis have been performed. Mitochondrial and nuclear DNA-based phylogeographic studies also support a close relationship between CPN and SAA [69].

Lizard Capture and Husbandry

From July-September, 2011, we captured 59 adult females from CPN (Fig. 1M) and 83 adult females from SAA (Fig. 2M). Due to the constraints of our experimental design (see below), we used only 17 randomly-chosen adult females from each population in our mate choice experiments [46]. Similarly, we captured 134 adult males from CPN (Fig. 1M) and 98 adult males from SAA (Fig. 2M) but used only 46 males from CPN and 35 males from SAA in our experiments. We captured all lizards by hand or by noose. We considered lizards adults if they had secondary sexual coloration (blue belly patches in males and throat patches in both sexes) and a snout-vent length (SVL) greater than 40 mm [80], [81].

Within one week of capture, we transported lizards to the Universidad Juárez del Estado de Durango (UJED), Gómez Palacio, Durango, México. We isolated males and females in separate coolers during transport and kept individual lizards in cloth bags to prevent interactions between them. At UJED, we maintained all lizards in a room with large windows open to the ambient air and UV light. Lizards were kept in individual terraria with opaque walls. We misted terraria with water daily and fed the lizards 4–5 crickets (Acheta domesticus) every other day. We placed the terraria on Flexwatt © heat tape, coupled to a ZooMed thermostat set at 32°C [82]. We allowed lizards to acclimate to the laboratory for at least 5 days before we used them in behavior trials [83]. At the end of our study (October 2011), we released all lizards at their original capture locations.

Throat Color Scores

We scored throat color using methods developed for Uta stansburiana, a confamilial lizard with a phenotypically similar throat color polymorphism. In U. stansburiana, the throat color polymorphism is genetically based and appears to be controlled by either one locus with orange, yellow, and blue alleles or two very tightly linked loci, based on breeding studies [3], [16], [46], [84][87] and gene mapping [88]. For details regarding how we assigned color scores and assessed their repeatability, please see the Supplementary Materials (Text S1) and [65].

Experimental Design

Each individual female participated in two mate choice trials, one designated the “different color” (DC) trial and the other designated the “similar color” (SC) trial (Fig. 3). We assigned DC and SC male pairs to each female randomly, and we randomized the order in which we performed the trials to remove any effect of the order in which females were presented with the two pairs of males [89]. We made minor alterations to the randomized trial order on an ad-hoc basis, to avoid using individual lizards in more than one trial per day. We conducted mate choice trials between 10 a.m. and 4 p.m., because those were the hours during which we g003primarily observed lizard activity in the field (E. Bastiaans & G. Morinaga, pers. obs.).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Schematic representation of our mate choice experiment.

All females participated in one SC trial and one DC trial, each of which included two males, one from SAA and one from CPN. In all cases, DC trials included a pure blue male from CPN and a pure white male from SAA. In trial pairings with an identical morph SC trial, the males included in the SC trial were either both pure yellow or both yellow-orange. In trial pairings with a dosage effect SC trial, the possible male pairings in the SC trial were blue-orange (CPN) vs. white-orange (SAA) or blue-yellow (CPN) vs. white-yellow (SAA).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.g003

In all mate choice trials, females chose between one male from CPN and one male from SAA (i.e., one sympatric and one allopatric male). In DC trials (Fig. 3), the male from CPN was of the pure blue morph (Fig. 1B), and the male from SAA was of the pure white morph (Fig. 2B). We performed two categories of SC trials (although any given female participated in only one), “identical morph” trials and “dosage effect” trials (Fig. 3). In the identical morph SC trials, the CPN and SAA males were of the same morph, which meant they could either be two pure yellow males (Figs. 1C, 2C) or two yellow-orange males (Figs. 1D, 2D).

The dosage effect SC trials were intended to assess whether females respond to intermediate morph males in which only one color patch is of an unfamiliar color in the same way they respond to males whose entire throat is of an unfamiliar color. In dosage effect SC trials (Fig. 3), we therefore used males that had one color patch in common and one color patch that differed between them. Thus, pairs could consist either of a blue-orange male from CPN (Fig. 1E) and a white-orange male from SAA (Fig. 2E); or of a blue-yellow male from CPN (Fig. 1F) and a white-yellow male from SAA (Fig. 2F).

We formed male pairs of each category randomly, under the constraint that males within a pair could not differ by more than 2 mm in SVL. We paired males of similar SVL to avoid possible confounding effects of male body size on female preference [90], [91]. However, these color morph and SVL constraints on male pair formation limited the number of possible male pairs, so that we had to choose between either using individual males multiple times in our experiments or performing a very small number of total trials. We chose to use individual males multiple times and account for the potential effects of male identity on female choice by including male identity as a random factor in our statistical analyses (see below) [36], [45].

We assigned SC and DC male pairs to each female randomly, under the constraint that the female was not captured within 40 m of either the SC or the DC sympatric male. This distance cutoff was double our previous estimate of the average diameter of male territories at CPN and SAA [65], and was intended to prevent trials from including males and females who were familiar with one another [74], [92].

Mate Choice Trials

We conducted mate choice trials in a chamber measuring 70 cm wide x 46 cm long x 46 cm tall, with a plywood floor and Plexiglas © walls covered in opaque paper. The floor of the chamber was covered in sand from the UJED campus, and we changed the sand after every trial to prevent scent cues from influencing future trials [91], [93]. An opaque cardboard barrier extended perpendicular to the two long walls, halfway across the chamber (Fig. 3). This created two male display areas, into which we placed the two males used in a trial. Males were tethered to the back of the trial chamber with black surgical thread, which was tied loosely around their hips. The tethers allowed males to reach the front of their display areas, but they could not see around the barrier. This trial design prevented direct male-male interactions from influencing female mate choice [44], [45]. Each male display area contained a rock to serve as a substrate for displays, and we scrubbed the rocks with water after every trial to remove scent cues [91], [93]. We heated the rocks to approximately 30–35°C using lamps attached to the side of the trial chamber [65], [82]. Before each trial, we placed a cardboard barrier across the center of the trial chamber, perpendicular to the barrier between the two male display areas. This barrier divided the trial chamber into three sections: the two male display areas and a larger area in front of them. We placed the female into this front section and assigned males to either the left or right display areas randomly, to avoid bias that might result from females’ inherent preference for a certain side of the trial chamber or from lateralization of the visual aspects of mate choice or aggressive behavior [94], [95]. We allowed the lizards used in each trial to acclimate to the chamber for five minutes before raising the cardboard barrier separating the female from the males. Trials lasted for 20 minutes and were videotaped using a Flip Video digital camera (Cisco Systems) mounted on a tripod above the trial chamber. During the trials, no human observer entered the lizards’ lines of sight. Females had full access to both males, but males could not leave their display areas. This design minimized the probability that males could force females to copulate and increased the likelihood that female behaviors directed toward males would be an expression of female preference for or rejection of those males [44], [45].

Behavior Scoring

All trial videos were scored by one observer (MJB), who was blind to the populations of origin of the lizards, the color morphs of the males, and the categories of the trials [96]. Every fourth trial was independently scored by another observer (EB), and differences were resolved through discussion. If disagreements could not be easily resolved, the trial was re-scored without reference to previous scores. The observer viewed each trial three times and recorded the behaviors of one lizard per viewing. Because the camera was positioned above the trial chamber, the males’ throat colors were not visible to the observer during scoring.

We scored female behaviors (Table 1) based on literature records of female reproductive behavior in closely related lizards, and based on a pilot experiment conducted in summer 2009 (E. Bastiaans and G. Morinaga, pers. obs.). The observer tallied the number of times a female performed each behavior during each trial, recorded the time point at which each behavior was performed, and also recorded the direction of each female behavior (left male or right male). Female behaviors were recorded as directed towards a male if she looked directly at the male or moved toward the male while performing them, or if she performed the behavior while located inside a male’s display area [45].

thumbnail
Table 1. Female behaviors scoreda during mate choice trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.t001

To test for an effect of male display on female preference, we recorded the number of head-bobs and push-ups performed by each male. Push-ups involve raising and lowering the body, using either two legs or all four legs. Head-bobs are rapid, shallower motions of the head, usually performed after the body is raised in a motion similar to that of a push-up. Head-bobs and push-ups are the most prominent courtship behaviors performed by male Sceloporus [97][99]. However, because push-ups and head-bobs can be difficult to distinguish and are usually performed together (pers. obs. E. Bastiaans and M.J. Bastiaans), we summed them as an index of the overall intensity of male display behavior during the mate choice trials [45].

We estimated association time between a female and an individual male by looking for sequences of behaviors directed towards that male. We then subtracted the first time point from the last time point for each sequence of behaviors, and summed the time periods for all sequences of behaviors directed towards each male during the trial. For instances in which a female directed only a single behavior towards a male (rather than directing a sequence of behaviors towards the male), we added 1 second to her association time with that male.

Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using JMP 9.0 and 10.0 (SAS Institute, 2011, 2012) or R 2.15.1 and 3.0.1 [100]. We tested the distributions of dependent variables and employed GLMs or non-parametric statistics in cases where there were marked deviations from normality.

We used principal components analysis (PCA) to summarize female behaviors and interpreted the resulting principal components (PC) axes based on the published literature and on trials in which females made unambiguous choices. Some female behaviors we recorded are part of female rejection displays in other phrynosomatid lizards (e.g., lateral compression, push-ups, biting) [92], [94], [101], [102], whereas others have been used as indications of female preference (e.g., approach) [44]. However, the meaning of these female display behaviors has not been assessed in the S. grammicus complex specifically, and female sceloporines may perform similar behaviors in multiple contexts [74]. We assumed that copulations represented definitive choices of males by females, but the low number of copulations we observed during our experiment prevented us from using copulation alone as an index of female choice. Of the 34 total females in our experiment, 7 copulated with one of the males with whom they interacted. These interactions included 3 copulations with an SC sympatric male, 3 copulations with an SC allopatric male, and 1 copulation with a DC sympatric male. No females copulated with DC allopatric males. We used these unambiguous interactions to support our literature-based hypotheses regarding which behaviors indicate preference for or rejection of a male in S. grammicus from these two localities, in this particular context [45]. This technique allowed us to develop a female preference index based on the behaviors we observed most frequently.

To summarize female behaviors, we first separated all trials into individual male-female interactions (i.e., two interactions per trial, or four total interactions per female) and ignored all information except which behaviors the female had performed and whether or not she had copulated with the male toward whom she performed those behaviors. We assigned interactions in which copulation occurred a score of 1 and interactions in which copulation did not occur a score of 0. We performed a principal components analysis (PCA) on a matrix of correlations between the incidences of all female behaviors except copulation [103], [104]. We saved the first three PC axes, because their eigenvalues were greater than 1 [105] (Table 2). We then performed generalized linear mixed model regression of copulation probability vs. these first three PC axes, including female ID as a random factor to avoid pseudoreplication. We performed the analysis with a binomial distribution and logit link, using the lmer function of the lme4 package in R 2.15.1 [100]. We removed non-significant effects in a stepwise procedure. Neither PC1 nor PC3 were significantly associated with copulation, but PC2 was significantly negatively associated with copulation probability (z1,134 = −2.54, P = 0.0111) (Fig. 4). Due to the negative relationship between PC2 and copulation probability, we considered PC2 to be an index of female rejection behavior (Fig. 4, Table 3). For details regarding the loadings of each behavior we scored on PC2, please see the Supplementary Material (Text S1).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Logistic regression of copulation probability (0 = no, 1 = yes, left y-axis) vs. female behavior PC2 score (x-axis).

The dark grey line shows the fitted logistic regression equation, while the histograms show how many male-female interactions fell into each bin of PC2 scores (right y-axis).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Loading matrix for PC axes 1–3 from a PCA on female behaviors other than copulation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Composite variables used in this study, with the methods used to calculate them and explanations of why they were used.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.t003

Based on the negative association between PC2 and copulation (Fig. 4) and also on the positive loadings (Table 2) of putative rejection behaviors such as lateral compression and push-ups [92], [94], [101], [102] on PC2, we used each female’s PC2 score towards each male with whom she interacted as a measure of her rejection of that male. Separately for each SC trial and each DC trial, we calculated a “discrimination index” to measure how much the female rejected the allopatric relative to the sympatric male (Table 3). We calculated the discrimination index by subtracting the female’s PC2 score towards the sympatric male from her PC2 score towards the allopatric male [44]. Finally, we subtracted each female’s discrimination index in her SC trial from her discrimination index in the DC trial to calculate a “discrimination differential” for each female (Table 3). This discrimination differential measured the degree to which a female directed more rejection behaviors toward the allopatric than the sympatric male in her DC trial relative to the degree to which she directed more rejection behaviors towards the allopatric than the sympatric male in her SC trial. A positive value of this discrimination differential would indicate that a female discriminated more between allopatric and sympatric males during her pure blue vs. pure white DC trial than during her SC trial (in which the males were more similar in coloration) (Fig. 3). If females can discriminate more easily between allopatric and sympatric males when the allopatric male is of an unfamiliar morph than when the allopatric male is of a familiar morph, we expect the discrimination differentials to be greater for females who participated in trial pairings with identical morph SC trials (Fig. 3) than for females who participated in trial pairings with dosage effect SC trials (Fig. 3). We used a general linear mixed model to examine the effect of SC trial type on female discrimination differential, while also assessing the effects of male display intensity (number of head bobs plus number of push-ups), female population, and male identity (as a random effect). We also conducted a similar analysis of whether female throat color morph influenced discrimination differential. Please see the Supplementary Material (Text S1) for further details on this analysis.

Our overall hypothesis in this study was that females would discriminate more between allopatric and sympatric males when the allopatric males had an unfamiliar throat color than when the allopatric males were of a familiar throat color morph. This hypothesis also predicts that females would reject allopatric males more strongly in dosage effect SC trials than in identical morph SC trials (Fig. 3). We tested this hypothesis by using a general linear model to examine the effect of SC trial type on female discrimination index within SC trials. Please see the Supplementary Material (Text S1) for further details about this analysis.

Because association time has been used as a measure of female preference in other work on mate choice [33], [37], [48], [102], we also conducted an analysis in which we compared the amount of time a female spent performing behaviors toward each male with our behavior-based rejection index (i.e., PC2) as a predictor of copulation. We concluded that our rejection index was a more appropriate measure of female behavior in this case, but for completeness, we conducted a parallel analysis using association time, rather than our PCA-based behavioral metric.

For comparability with our discrimination differential analysis, we first calculated the difference between the time a female spent with the allopatric male vs. the sympatric male within a trial (i.e., time with DC allopatric male - time with DC sympatric male and time with SC allopatric male - time with SC sympatric male). Then, we subtracted the time difference in the SC trial from the time difference in the DC trial. For brevity, we will refer to this measure as “association time differential.”

Results

Compliance with Assumptions of Statistical Tests

The distribution of the composite variable “discrimination differential” (Table 3) deviated significantly from normality (Shapiro-Wilk test, W = 0.92, P = 0.015). However, the skewness and kurtosis of the distribution (calculated using R 2.15.1 [100]) indicated the distribution was only slightly negatively skewed (skewness = −0.115) and moderately leptokurtic (kurtosis = 2.17). The variance was homogeneous with respect to the independent variable of most interest to our analysis (SC trial type; Bartlett’s test, P = 0.33). We thus relied on the relative robustness of ANOVA family models to minor violations of normality and performed our analysis using a general linear model approach with restricted maximum likelihood (REML).

Effects of Male Identity on Discrimination Differential

Our sample size was not sufficient to allow us to include random effects of the identities of all four males with whom a female had interacted on her discrimination differential, while also considering all fixed effects of interest (female population, male display rates, and SC trial type). We therefore first fit a model including only our 4 random effects: SC sympatric male identity, SC allopatric male identity, DC sympatric male identity, and DC allopatric male identity (Table 4). Only DC sympatric male identity accounted for a variance component significantly different from zero (based on its 95% confidence interval, Table 4), so that was the only random effect we retained in subsequent tests.

thumbnail
Table 4. Components of variance in female discrimination differential explained by male identity (ID), estimated as random effects from a general linear model with REML.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.t004

Effects of SC Trial Type and Male Display Rates on Discrimination Differential

We initially fit a general linear model including DC sympatric male identity as a random effect and SC trial type, female population, SC sympatric male display intensity, SC allopatric male display intensity, DC sympatric male display intensity, DC allopatric male display intensity, and all 2-way interactions between SC trial type and the other fixed factors. We removed non-significant factors in a stepwise procedure, arriving at a final model that included SC trial type (F2.041, 33 = 370, P = 0.0024), SC allopatric male display intensity (F2.037, 33 = 38.0, P = 0.024), DC allopatric male display intensity (F2.033, 33 = 71.8, P = 0.013), and the interaction SC trial type by SC allopatric male display intensity (F2.032, 33 = 32.0, P = 0.029). The proportion of variance in discrimination differential among females that was explained by DC sympatric male identity continued to be significantly greater than zero (variance component = 7.82, 95% CI = 3.64, 12.0).

The average discrimination differential was greater for females participating in trial pairings that included identical morph SC trials (Fig. 3) than for females participating in trial pairings that included dosage effect SC trials (Fig. 3, Fig. 5). Calculating the 95% confidence intervals (CI) of the least-squares (LS) means for each category of females revealed that the discrimination differential for females who experienced dosage effect SC trials was not significantly different from zero (LS mean = −0.609, 95% CI = −1.69, 0.477). The discrimination differential for females who experienced identical morph SC trials, however, was significantly greater than zero (LS mean = 1.50, 95% CI = 0.401, 2.60).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Least-squares (LS) mean discrimination differential was greater for females who participated in trial pairings with identical morph SC trials than for females in trial pairings with dosage effect SC trials.

In addition, the LS mean discrimination differential was significantly greater than zero for females participating in trial pairings with identical morph SC trials but not for females participating in pairings with dosage effect SC trials. Circles represent LS means, while bars are ±95% confidence intervals. Dashed line represents a discrimination differential of zero.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.g005

SC allopatric male display intensity had a negative effect on discrimination differential. This negative effect was stronger for trial pairings that included dosage effect SC trials than for pairings that included identical morph SC trials. That is, SC allopatric male display intensity had a stronger negative effect on discrimination differential for females who participated in dosage effect SC trials than for females who participated in identical morph SC trials. DC allopatric male display intensity had a positive effect on discrimination differential.

Effects of Female Morph on Discrimination Differential and Comparison of SC Trial Types

In a parallel analysis to the one we conducted of the effect of SC trial type on discrimination differential, we found no effect of female morph on discrimination differential (F4,8.108 = 0.58, P = 0.68) (Fig. S1). We also compared discrimination indices (Table 3) between identical morph and dosage effect SC trials. Although the difference in discrimination indices was in the direction predicted by our hypothesis (i.e., greater for dosage effect trials than for identical morph trials), it was not close to significance (F1,28.31 = 0.25, P = 0.62) (Fig. S2). Please see the Supplementary Materials (Text S1) for further information about both these tests.

Effects of SC Trial Type and Male Display Rates on Association Time Differential

Association time differential was normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk test, W = 0.97, P = 0.51). As with discrimination differential, we first performed a test including only random effects of the four male identities. The confidence intervals for all variance components overlapped zero, so we removed random effects from the model and fit a standard, fixed-effects linear model to investigate the possible effects of SC trial type and male display intensities.

We initially fit a model including female population, SC trial type, all four male display rates, and the interaction effects of all four male display rates with SC trial type on association time differential. We removed non-significant effects in a stepwise procedure, arriving at final model that included nearly significant effects of SC trial type (F1,30 = 3.5, P = 0.07) and SC allopatric male display rate (F1, 30 = 2.9, P = 0.10) and a significant effect of SC sympatric male display rate (F1, 30 = 5.4, P = 0.03). The association time differential was nearly significantly greater for females participating in identical morph SC trials than for females participating in dosage effect SC trials, but the 95% CI of association time differential for both types of females overlapped zero (Dosage Effect SC Trial: LS mean = −178 s, CI = −447 s, 90 s; Identical Morph SC Trial: LS mean = 370 s, CI = −161 s, 901 s). The effect of SC allopatric male display rate on association time differential was nearly significantly negative, and the effect of SC sympatric male display rate was significantly positive. For discussion of these results and further analyses supporting our decision to use our PCA-based behavioral metric as our primary measure of female response to males, please see the Supplementary Materials (Text S1).

Discussion

Our findings suggest that female S. grammicus 1) respond to throat color when evaluating potential mates, 2) are capable of distinguishing between sympatric and allopatric males, and 3) differentiate between sympatric and allopatric males more strongly when allopatric males exhibit a throat color which is absent from a female’s home population. Previous research has revealed cases in which individuals mate assortatively based on sexual signals which are polymorphic within their population [35], [47] or based on sexual signals which differ between populations of the same species or between closely related species [25], [33], [36], [37], [74], [106]. Most of this research has focused on the possible role of morph losses in speciation [16], [40], or on the role of divergence in sexual signals in driving speciation [30], [107], [108]. To our knowledge, the S. grammicus complex currently represents the only known example of a taxon with polymorphic sexual signals in which some morphs (yellow and orange, in this case) occur in most or all populations, but other morphs are population-specific and never co-occur (blue vs. white in males) (Figs. 1 & 2, [65], [68]).

Our results indicate that females distinguished more between sympatric and allopatric males in pure blue vs. pure white trials than they did in trials where the two males were of the same color morph (i.e, yellow vs. yellow or yellow-orange vs. yellow-orange) (Fig. 3, Fig. 5). However, females did not distinguish more between sympatric and allopatric males in pure blue vs. pure white trials than in trials where the two males were of mixed morphs that differed by one color patch (i.e., blue-orange vs. white-orange or blue-yellow vs. white-yellow) (Fig. 3, Fig. 5). Other interpopulation mate choice trials have found that preferences can be asymmetric, with members of one population discriminating more strongly than members of the other population [74]. However, neither female population nor its interaction with SC trial type had significant effects on discrimination differential, indicating that the response of CPN females to white-throated males was similar to the response of SAA females to blue-throated males.

Few copulations occurred during our experiment, which was why we framed our analysis in terms of female rejection behaviors rather than female acceptance behaviors (Table 3). One male in each trial was from an allopatric population, so we would not, a priori, have necessarily expected females to accept those males as mates. The high incidence of rejection behaviors we observed is consistent with this hypothesis [31], [34], [36]. In addition, territory quality is an important influence on female mate choice in natural populations of many polygynous lizards [48], [90], [109], [110], so the fact that females could not evaluate male territories may have reduced female willingness to mate.

The large component of the variation in female discrimination differential explained by DC sympatric male identity occurred even though the number of unique males in this category was not substantially less than the number of unique males in the other three categories (Table 4). Our experiment did not assess female preference for traits other than throat color, nor evaluate female preference functions within populations. However, the effect of male identity for one experimental category, but not others, suggests female mate preference may be multimodal and context-dependent in S. grammicus, as it is in several other taxa, including some lizards [44], [46], [111][113]. Future research should investigate female preference functions for throat color and other traits within populations [34], [35], [45], [47], [90], [114] as well as possible variation in those functions among populations [74], [113], [115], [116].

The relationship between male display intensity (total number of head bobs plus total number of push-ups) and female discrimination differential may indicate that throat color, per se, is not the only signal to which females respond. The display intensities of sympatric males did not affect female discrimination differential for either trial category. However, the display intensities of allopatric males had significant effects in both SC and DC trials. SC allopatric male display intensity had a negative effect on female discrimination differential, indicating that females’ responses to SC and DC pairings were more similar when SC allopatric males displayed more. In contrast, the effect of DC allopatric male display intensity on discrimination differential was positive, indicating that females’ responses to DC and SC pairings differed more when DC allopatric males displayed more. Taken together, these two findings may suggest that, within trials, females distinguished more between sympatric and allopatric males when allopatric males displayed more. Allopatric males who displayed more may have exposed their throat coloration or other aspects of their courtship display more clearly to females, enhancing the females’ ability to identify their foreign origin [117], [118]. Head-bob and push-up displays vary within and among other species of Sceloporus [98], [99], [119], so it is possible that subtle variation exists between the courtship displays of males from SAA and CPN. Allopatric males who displayed more could therefore have been easier for females to differentiate from sympatric males, although our experiment did not explicitly test this hypothesis.

We chose CPN and SAA for our comparison of “blue male” and “white male” populations specifically because they are ecologically similar [75], [76]. Indeed, broader sampling of populations from northern and central Mexico has not revealed any obvious ecological pattern explaining why some populations of the S. grammicus complex exhibit the blue male morph, whereas other exhibit the white male morph [69]. The behavioral and sexual signal differences between CPN and SAA occur despite their ecological similarity and the apparently close evolutionary relationship between these two populations [50], [65], [79]. Our findings suggest that further studies of behavioral variation and female responses to sympatric vs. allopatric color morphs across other populations within the S. grammicus species complex are warranted. Such studies may help resolve both why the “blue vs. white” variation we have documented exists and what its current effects on gene flow within the species complex may be.

Some of the previously identified hybrid zones between chromosome races in the S. grammicus complex [51], [54][59] also represent contacts between populations with blue males and populations with white males, but this is not always the case [69]. While we did not perform karyotyping at CPN or SAA ourselves, the most recent data available [50] indicate that both populations most likely exhibit the 2N = 32 karyotype. It is therefore unlikely that these two populations represent the ends of a hybrid zone between two chromosome races, so we do not know whether crosses between them would exhibit the reduced hybrid fitness previously documented between populations of the S. grammicus complex differing in karyotype [59]. However, a recent study in Uta stansburiana, another species with throat color morphs that differ in reproductive tactics, found that male morphs varied in their reproductive compatibility across recently diverged populations with no difference in chromosome number [40]. That study did not assess whether females varied in their ability to differentiate allopatric and sympatric males of different morphs, and our study does not address what selective forces may have favored females’ ability to distinguish allopatric and sympatric males.

Our findings regarding the response of females to allopatric vs. sympatric male morphs, however, provide new insight into the mechanisms by which color polymorphism may drive speciation [14], [39]. Previous work [40] showed that postmating reproductive isolation could vary by morph in a similar system, but this study shows that premating reproductive isolation may also be morph-dependent in taxa where polymorphic sexual signals vary among populations. It is possible that similar patterns occur in other species with polymorphic sexual signals. Further sampling of multiple populations of such species may reveal ecological or phylogenetic patterns that account for variation in the number and frequency of morphs, and it may also clarify the processes by which polymorphic populations diverge during speciation [13][15], [39].

Supporting Information

Figure S1.

Female discrimination differential (LS means, error bars show 95% CI) versus female throat color morph. We found no effect of female color morph on discrimination differential.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.s001

(TIF)

Figure S2.

Female discrimination index (LS means, error bars show 95% CI) did not differ significantly between dosage effect and identical morph SC trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.s002

(TIF)

Text S1.

Supplementary Materials and Methods, Results, and Discussion sections.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.s003

(DOCX)

Data S1.

Data supplement, including female and male identities, throat color morphs, and behaviors performed during mate choice trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093197.s004

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank the residents of San Antonio de las Alazanas, Mesa de las Tablas, and San Antonio de Peña Nevada for permission to sample on their land. We thank the faculty, administration, and staff of the Universidad Juárez del Estado de Durango for laboratory space and the use of their facilities. M. Borja Jiménez, O. Cano Villegas, J. Castañeda Gaytán, R. Israel Olivo, and G. Martínez assisted with fieldwork, lizard husbandry, and behavioral trials. B. Lyon and P. Raimondi provided crucial statistical advice. S. Balenger, G. Bastiaans, R. Cooper, A. Díaz de la Vega, V. Luja, B. Lyon, D. Paranjpe, G. Pogson, J. Sapp, E. Swanger, M. Zuk, academic editor D. Rubenstein, and two anonymous reviewers provided helpful comments on the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: EB JGCG FMDLC BS. Performed the experiments: EB GM JGCG JCM. Analyzed the data: EB MJB BB BS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: EB JGCG FMDLC. Wrote the paper: EB.

References

  1. 1. Hover EL (1985) Differences in Aggressive Behavior between Two Throat Color Morphs in a Lizard, Urosaurus ornatus. Copeia 1985: 933–940
  2. 2. Rand MS (1988) Courtship and Aggressive Behavior in Male Lizards Exhibiting Two Different Sexual Colorations. Am Zool 28: 153A.
  3. 3. Sinervo B, Lively CM (1996) The rock-paper-scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies. Nature 380: 240–243
  4. 4. Pryke SR, Griffith SC (2006) Red dominates black: agonistic signalling among head morphs in the color polymorphic Gouldian finch. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 273: 949–957.
  5. 5. Huyghe K, Vanhooydonck B, Herrel A, Tadić Z, Van Damme R (2007) Morphology, performance, behavior and ecology of three color morphs in males of the lizard Podarcis melisellensis. Integr Comp Biol 47: 211–220
  6. 6. Korzan WJ, Fernald RD (2007) Territorial Male Color Predicts Agonistic Behavior of Conspecifics in a Color Polymorphic Species. Behav Ecol 18: 318–323
  7. 7. Vercken E, Clobert J (2008) Ventral colour polymorphism correlates with alternative behavioural patterns in female common lizards (Lacerta vivipara). Ecoscience 15: 320–326
  8. 8. Dijkstra PD, Van Dijk S, Groothuis TGG, Pierotti MER, Seehausen O (2009) Behavioral Dominance Between Female Color Morphs of a Lake Victoria Cichlid Fish. Behav Ecol 20: 593–600
  9. 9. Neff BD, Svensson EI (2013) Polyandry and alternative mating tactics. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 368. Available: http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/368/1613/20120045. Accessed: 21 Jan 2013.
  10. 10. Olsson M, Stuart-Fox D, Ballen C (2013) Genetics and evolution of colour patterns in reptiles. Semin Cell Dev Biol 24: 529–541
  11. 11. Miles DB, Sinervo B, Hazard LC, Svensson EI, Costa D (2007) Relating endocrinology, physiology and behaviour using species with alternative mating strategies. Funct Ecol 21: 653–665
  12. 12. West-Eberhard MJ (1983) Sexual Selection, Social Competition, and Speciation. Q Rev Biol 58: 155
  13. 13. West-Eberhard MJ (1986) Alternative adaptations, speciation, and phylogeny (A Review). Proc Natl Acad Sci 83: 1388–1392.
  14. 14. Gray SM, McKinnon JS (2007) Linking color polymorphism maintenance and speciation. Trends Ecol Evol 22: 71–79
  15. 15. Forsman A, Ahnesjö J, Caesar S, Karlsson M (2008) A Model of Ecological and Evolutionary Consequences of Color Polymorphism. Ecology 89: 34–40.
  16. 16. Corl A, Davis AR, Kuchta SR, Sinervo B (2010) Selective loss of polymorphic mating types is associated with rapid phenotypic evolution during morphic speciation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107: 4254–4259
  17. 17. Pizzatto L, Dubey S (2012) Colour-polymorphic snake species are older. Biol J Linn Soc 107: 210–218
  18. 18. Boughman JW (2002) How sensory drive can promote speciation. Trends Ecol Evol 17: 571–577
  19. 19. Kraaijeveld K, Kraaijeveld-Smit FJL, Maan ME (2011) Sexual selection and speciation: the comparative evidence revisited. Biol Rev 86: 367–377
  20. 20. Schluter D (2009) Evidence for Ecological Speciation and Its Alternative. Science 323: 737–741
  21. 21. Alonzo SH, Calsbeek R (2010) The unstable dynamics of multiple alternative reproductive tactics. J Evol Biol 23: 2614–2624
  22. 22. Runemark A, Hansson B, Pafilis P, Valakos ED, Svensson EI (2010) Island biology and morphological divergence of the Skyros wall lizard Podarcis gaigeae: a combined role for local selection and genetic drift on color morph frequency divergence? BMC Evol Biol 10: 269
  23. 23. Maan ME, Seehausen O (2011) Ecology, sexual selection and speciation. Ecol Lett 14: 591–602
  24. 24. Ritchie MG (2007) Sexual Selection and Speciation. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst 38: 79–102
  25. 25. Robertson JM, Rosenblum EB (2009) Rapid divergence of social signal coloration across the White Sands ecotone for three lizard species under strong natural selection. Biol J Linn Soc 98: 243–255
  26. 26. Servedio MR, Noor MAF (2003) The Role of Reinforcement in Speciation: Theory and Data. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst 34: 339–364
  27. 27. Lukhtanov VA, Kandul NP, Plotkin JB, Dantchenko AV, Haig D, et al. (2005) Reinforcement of pre-zygotic isolation and karyotype evolution in Agrodiaetus butterflies. Nature 436: 385–389
  28. 28. Gonzalez-Voyer A, den Tex R-J, Castelló A, Leonard JA (2013) Evolution of acoustic and visual signals in Asian barbets. J Evol Biol: n/a–n/a. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/jeb.12084.
  29. 29. Hochberg ME, Sinervo B, Brown SP (2003) Socially Mediated Speciation. Evolution 57: 154–158
  30. 30. Seehausen O, Terai Y, Magalhaes IS, Carleton KL, Mrosso HDJ, et al. (2008) Speciation through sensory drive in cichlid fish. Nature 455: 620–626
  31. 31. Elmer KR, Lehtonen TK, Meyer A (2009) Color Assortative Mating Contributes to Sympatric Divergence of Neotropical Cichlid Fish. Evolution 63: 2750–2757
  32. 32. Boughman JW (2001) Divergent sexual selection enhances reproductive isolation in sticklebacks. Nature 411: 944–948
  33. 33. Reynolds RG, Fitzpatrick BM (2007) Assortative Mating in Poison-Dart Frogs Based on an Ecologically Important Trait. Evolution 61: 2253–2259
  34. 34. Egger B, Obermüller B, Eigner E, Sturmbauer C, Sefc KM (2008) Assortative mating preferences between colour morphs of the endemic Lake Tanganyika cichlid genus Tropheus. Hydrobiologia 615: 37–48.
  35. 35. Pryke SR (2010) Sex chromosome linkage of mate preference and color signal maintains assortative mating between interbreeding finch morphs. Evolution 64: 1301–1310
  36. 36. Zoppoth P, Koblmüller S, Sefc KM (2013) Male courtship preferences demonstrate discrimination against allopatric colour morphs in a cichlid fish. J Evol Biol 26: 577–586
  37. 37. Summers K, Symula R, Clough M, Cronin T (1999) Visual mate choice in poison frogs. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 266: 2141–2145
  38. 38. Feldman CR, Flores-Villela O, Papenfuss TJ (2011) Phylogeny, biogeography, and display evolution in the tree and brush lizard genus Urosaurus (Squamata: Phrynosomatidae). Mol Phylogenet Evol 61: 714–725
  39. 39. McLean CA, Stuart-Fox D (2014) Geographic variation in animal colour polymorphisms and its role in speciation. Biol Rev: n/a–n/a. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12083.
  40. 40. Corl A, Lancaster LT, Sinervo B (2012) Rapid Formation of Reproductive Isolation between Two Populations of Side-Blotched Lizards, Uta stansburiana. Copeia 2012: 593–602.
  41. 41. Thompson CW, Moore MC (1991) Throat colour reliably signals status in male tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus. Anim Behav 42: 745–753
  42. 42. Healey M, Uller T, Olsson M (2007) Seeing red: morph-specific contest success and survival rates in a colour-polymorphic agamid lizard. Anim Behav 74: 337–341
  43. 43. Vercken E, Clobert J (2008) The role of colour polymorphism in social encounters among female common lizards. Herpetol J 18: 223–230.
  44. 44. Hamilton PS, Sullivan BK (2005) Female mate attraction in ornate tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus: a multivariate analysis. Anim Behav 69: 219–224
  45. 45. Bleay C, Sinervo B (2007) Discrete genetic variation in mate choice and a condition-dependent preference function in the side-blotched lizard: implications for the formation and maintenance of coadapted gene complexes. Behav Ecol 18: 304–310
  46. 46. Lancaster LT, Hipsley CA, Sinervo B (2009) Female Choice for Optimal Combinations of Multiple Male Display Traits Increases Offspring Survival. Behav Ecol 20: 993–999
  47. 47. Pérez i de Lanuza G, Font E, Carazo P (2012) Color-assortative mating in a color-polymorphic lacertid lizard. Behav Ecol. Available: http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2012/10/11/beheco.ars164. Accessed: 22 Oct 2012.
  48. 48. Swierk L, Ridgway M, Langkilde T (2012) Female lizards discriminate between potential reproductive partners using multiple male traits when territory cues are absent. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 66: 1033–1043
  49. 49. Hall W (1973) Comparative Population Cytogenetics, Speciation, and Evolution of the Iguanid Lizard Genus Sceloporus [Ph.D. dissertation]. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
  50. 50. Sites JW (1983) Chromosome Evolution in the Iguanid Lizard Sceloporus grammicus. I. Chromosome Polymorphisms. Evolution 37: 38–53.
  51. 51. Marshall J, Arévalo E, Benavides E, Sites JL, Sites JW (2006) Delimiting Species: Comparing Methods for Mendelian Characters Using Lizards of the Sceloporus grammicus (Squamata: Phrynosomatidae) Complex. Evolution 60: 1050–1065.
  52. 52. Hall WP (2009) Chromosome Variation, Genomics, Speciation and Evolution in Sceloporus Lizards. Cytogenet Genome Res 127: 143–165
  53. 53. Lara-Gongora G (1983) Two new species of the lizard genus Sceloporus (Reptilia, Sauria, Iguanidae) from the Ajusco and Ocuilan Sierras, Mexico. Bull Md Herpetol Soc 19: 1–14.
  54. 54. Arévalo E, Casas G, Davis SK, Lara G, Sites JW (1993) Parapatric Hybridization between Chromosome Races of the Sceloporus grammicus Complex (Phrynosomatidae): Structure of the Ajusco Transect. Copeia 1993: 352–372.
  55. 55. Reed KM, Greenbaum IF, Sites JW (1995) Cytogenetic Analysis of Chromosomal Intermediates from a Hybrid Zone Between Two Chromosome Races of the Sceloporus grammicus Complex (Sauria, Phrynosomatidae). Evolution 49: 37–47.
  56. 56. Reed KM, Greenbaum IF, Sites JW (1995) Dynamics of a Novel Chromosomal Polymorphism within a Hybrid Zone Between Two Chromosome Races of the Sceloporus grammicus Complex (Sauria, Phrynosomatidae). Evolution 49: 48–60.
  57. 57. Sites JW, Barton NH, Reed KM (1995) The Genetic Structure of a Hybrid Zone Between Two Chromosome Races of the Sceloporus grammicus Complex (Sauria, Phrynosomatidae) in Central Mexico. Evolution 49: 9–36.
  58. 58. Marshall JC, Sites JW (2001) A comparison of nuclear and mitochondrial cline shapes in a hybrid zone in the Sceloporus grammicus complex (Squamata; Phrynosomatidae). Mol Ecol 10: 435–449
  59. 59. Reed KM, Sites JW (1995) Female Fecundity in a Hybrid Zone Between Two Chromosome Races of the Sceloporus grammicus Complex (Sauria, Phrynosomatidae). Evolution 49: 61–69.
  60. 60. Méndez de la Cruz F (1989) Estudio comparativo de la reproducción, tipología y aloenzimas de dos poblaciones cercanas de Sceloporus grammicus (Reptilia: Iguanidae) de la Sierra del Ajusco, México [Ph.D. dissertation]. Distrito Federal, México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.
  61. 61. Ramírez-Bautista A, Jiménez-Cruz E, Marshall J (2004) Comparative life history for populations of the Sceloporus grammicus complex (Squamata: Phrynosomatidae). West North Am Nat 64: 175–183.
  62. 62. Jiménez-Cruz E, Ramírez-Bautista A, Marshall JC, Lizana-Avia M, de Oca AN-M (2005) Reproductive Cycle of Sceloporus grammicus (Squamata: Phrynosomatidae) from Teotihuacán, México. Southwest Nat 50: 178–187.
  63. 63. Hernández-Salinas U, Ramírez-Bautista A, Leyte-Manrique A, Smith GR (2010) Reproduction and Sexual Dimorphism in Two Populations of Sceloporus grammicus (Sauria: Phrynosomatidae) from Hidalgo, Mexico. Herpetologica 66: 12–22
  64. 64. Ramírez-Bautista A, Stephenson BP, Lozano A, Uribe-Rodríguez H, Leyte Manrique A (2012) Atypical reproductive cycles in a population of Sceloporus grammicus (Squamata: Phrynosomatidae) from the Mexican Plateau. Ecol Evol: n/a–n/a. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.310.
  65. 65. Bastiaans E, Morinaga G, Gaytán JGC, Marshall JC, Sinervo B (2013) Male aggression varies with throat color in 2 distinct populations of the mesquite lizard. Behav Ecol 24: 968–981
  66. 66. Thompson CW, Moore MC (1991) Syntopic occurrence of multiple dewlap color morphs in male tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus. Copeia 1991: 493–503.
  67. 67. Rand MS (1990) Polymorphic Sexual Coloration in the Lizard Sceloporus undulatus erythrocheilus. Am Midl Nat 124: 352–359.
  68. 68. Leaché AD, Reeder TW (2002) Molecular Systematics of the Eastern Fence Lizard (Sceloporus undulatus): A Comparison of Parsimony, Likelihood, and Bayesian Approaches. Syst Biol 51: 44–68
  69. 69. Bastiaans E (2013) Variation in Reproductive Behavior and Sexual Signals Within and Among Populations of an Incipiently Speciating Lizard [Ph.D. Dissertation]. Santa Cruz, CA: University of California, Santa Cruz.
  70. 70. Coyne JA, Orr HA (2004) Speciation. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. 545 p.
  71. 71. Endler JA, Basolo AL (1998) Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection. Trends Ecol Evol 13: 415–420
  72. 72. Lande R (1981) Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proc Natl Acad Sci 78: 3721–3725.
  73. 73. Mendelson TC, Shaw KL (2012) The (mis)concept of species recognition. Trends Ecol Evol 27: 421–427
  74. 74. Hardwick KM, Robertson JM, Rosenblum EB (2013) Asymmetrical mate preference in recently adapted white sands and black lava populations of Sceloporus undulatus. Curr Zool 59.
  75. 75. Villanueva-Diaz J, Stahle D, Luckman B, Cerano-Paredes J, Therrell M, et al. (2007) Winter-spring precipitation reconstructions from tree rings for northeast Mexico. Clim Change 83: 117–131
  76. 76. Encina-Domínguez JA, Encina-Domínguez FJ, Mata-Rocha E, Valdes-Reyna J (2008) Aspectos estructurales, composición florística y caracterización ecológica del bosque de oyamel de la Sierra de Zapalinamé, Coahuila, México. Bol Soc Botánica México: 13–24.
  77. 77. Ersts PJ (2013) Geographic Distance Matrix Generator. American Museum of Natural History, Center for Biodiversity and Conservation. Available: http://biodiversityinformatics.amnh.org/open_source/gdmg. Accessed: 24 Jan 2013.
  78. 78. Sites JW, Dixon JR (1981) A New Subspecies of the Iguanid Lizard, Sceloporus grammicus, from Northeastern Mexico, with Comments on Its Evolutionary Implications and the Status of S. g. disparilis. J Herpetol 15: 59–69
  79. 79. Sites JW, Greenbaum IF (1983) Chromosome Evolution in the Iguanid Lizard Sceloporus grammicus. II. Allozyme Variation. Evolution 37: 54–65.
  80. 80. Zúñiga-Vega JJ, Méndez-de la Cruz FR, Cuellar O (2008) Body growth in one montane population of Sceloporus grammicus (Sauria, Phrynosomatidae) in Central Mexico. Amphib-Reptil 29: 127–134
  81. 81. Lemos-Espinal JA, Ballinger RE, Smith GR (1998) Comparative Demography of the High-Altitude Lizard, Sceloporus grammicus (Phrynosomatidae), on the Iztaccihuatl Volcano, Puebla, México. Gt Basin Nat 58: 375–379.
  82. 82. Andrews RM, Méndez de la Cruz FR, Villagrán Santa Cruz M (1997) Body Temperatures of Female Sceloporus grammicus: Thermal Stress or Impaired Mobility? Copeia 1997: 108–115.
  83. 83. Denardo DF, Licht P (1993) Effects of Corticosterone on Social Behavior of Male Lizards. Horm Behav 27: 184–199
  84. 84. Sinervo B, Zamudio KR (2001) The Evolution of Alternative Reproductive Strategies: Fitness Differential, Heritability, and Genetic Correlation Between the Sexes. J Hered 92: 198–205.
  85. 85. Bleay C, Comendant T, Sinervo B (2007) An experimental test of frequency-dependent selection on male mating strategy in the field. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 274: 2019–2025
  86. 86. Lancaster LT, McAdam AG, Wingfield JC, Sinervo B (2007) Adaptive social and maternal induction of antipredator dorsal patterns in a lizard with alternative social strategies. Ecol Lett 10: 798–808
  87. 87. Lancaster LT, McAdam AG, Sinervo B (2010) Maternal Adjustment of Egg Size Organizes Alternative Escape Behaviors, Promoting Adaptive Phenotypic Integration. Evolution 64: 1607–1621
  88. 88. Sinervo B, Chaine A, Clobert J, Calsbeek R, Hazard L, et al. (2006) Self-recognition, color signals, and cycles of greenbeard mutualism and altruism. Proc Natl Acad Sci 103: 7372–7377
  89. 89. Kozak GM, Head ML, Lackey ACR, Boughman JW (2013) Sequential mate choice and sexual isolation in threespine stickleback species. J Evol Biol 26: 130–140
  90. 90. Calsbeek R, Sinervo B (2002) Uncoupling direct and indirect components of female choice in the wild. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99: 14897–14902
  91. 91. López P, Aragón P, Martín J (2003) Responses of female lizards, Lacerta monticola, to males’ chemical cues reflect their mating preference for older males. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55: 73–79
  92. 92. Cooper WE (1988) Aggressive Behavior and Courtship Rejection in Brightly and Plainly Colored Female Keeled Earless Lizards (Holbrookia propinqua). Ethology 77: 265–278
  93. 93. Duvall D (1979) Western fence lizard (Sceloporus occidentalis) chemical signals. I. Conspecific discriminations and release of a species-typical visual display. J Exp Zool 210: 321–325
  94. 94. Hews DK, Castellano M, Hara E (2004) Aggression in females is also lateralized: left-eye bias during aggressive courtship rejection in lizards. Anim Behav 68: 1201–1207
  95. 95. Templeton JJ, Mountjoy DJ, Pryke SR, Griffith SC (2012) In the eye of the beholder: visual mate choice lateralization in a polymorphic songbird. Biol Lett 8: 924–927
  96. 96. Garland T, Hankins E, Huey RB (1990) Locomotor Capacity and Social Dominance in Male Lizards. Funct Ecol 4: 243–250.
  97. 97. Vinegar MB (1972) The Function of Breeding Coloration in the Lizard, Sceloporus virgatus. Copeia 1972: 660–664.
  98. 98. Carpenter C (1978) Comparative display behavior in the genus Sceloporus (Iguanidae). Milwaukee Public Mus Contrib Biol Geol 18: 1–71.
  99. 99. Martins EP (1993) A Comparative Study of the Evolution of Sceloporus Push-Up Displays. Am Nat 142: 994–1018.
  100. 100. R. Core Development Team (2012) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  101. 101. Cooper WE Jr, Crews D (1987) Hormonal induction of secondary sexual coloration and rejection behaviour in female keeled earless lizards, Holbrookia propinqua. Anim Behav 35: 1177–1187
  102. 102. Swierk L, Myers A, Langkilde T (2013) Male mate preference is influenced by both female behaviour and morphology. Anim Behav. Available: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347213001632. Accessed: 7 May 2013.
  103. 103. Thoen C, Bauwens D, Verheyen RF (1986) Chemoreceptive and behavioural responses of the common lizard Lacerta vivipara to snake chemical deposits. Anim Behav 34: 1805–1813
  104. 104. Downes SJ, Bauwens D (2002) Does reproductive state affect a lizard’s behavior toward predator chemical cues? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52: 444–450
  105. 105. Norman GR, Streiner DL (2008) Biostatistics: The Bare Essentials. PMPH-USA. 412 p.
  106. 106. Van der Sluijs I, Van Dooren TJM, Hofker KD, van Alphen JJM, Stelkens RB, et al. (2008) Female mating preference functions predict sexual selection against hybrids between sibling species of cichlid fish. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 363: 2871–2877
  107. 107. Seehausen, Alphen V (1999) Lande (1999) Color polymorphism and sex ratio distortion in a cichlid fish as an incipient stage in sympatric speciation by sexual selection. Ecol Lett 2: 367–378
  108. 108. Barluenga M, Stölting KN, Salzburger W, Muschick M, Meyer A (2006) Sympatric speciation in Nicaraguan crater lake cichlid fish. Nature 439: 719–723
  109. 109. Tokarz RR (1995) Mate Choice in Lizards: A Review. Herpetol Monogr 9: 17–40
  110. 110. Abell AJ (1997) Estimating Paternity with Spatial Behaviour and DNA Fingerprinting in the Striped Plateau Lizard, Sceloporus virgatus (Phrynosomatidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41: 217–226
  111. 111. Sullivan BK, Kwiatkowski MA (2007) Courtship displays in anurans and lizards: theoretical and empirical contributions to our understanding of costs and selection on males due to female choice. Funct Ecol 21: 666–675
  112. 112. Uetz GW, Roberts JA, Taylor PW (2009) Multimodal communication and mate choice in wolf spiders: female response to multimodal versus unimodal signals. Anim Behav 78: 299–305
  113. 113. Ronald KL, Fernández-Juricic E, Lucas JR (2012) Taking the sensory approach: how individual differences in sensory perception can influence mate choice. Anim Behav 84: 1283–1294
  114. 114. Van der Sluijs I, van Alphen JJM, Seehausen O (2008) Preference polymorphism for coloration but no speciation in a population of Lake Victoria cichlids. Behav Ecol 19: 177–183
  115. 115. Endler JA, Houde AE (1995) Geographic Variation in Female Preferences for Male Traits in Poecilia reticulata. Evolution 49: 456–468
  116. 116. Simmons LW, Zuk M, Rotenberry JT (2001) Geographic Variation in Female Preference Functions and Male Songs of the Field Cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Evolution 55: 1386–1394
  117. 117. Seehausen O, van Alphen JJM, Witte F (1997) Cichlid Fish Diversity Threatened by Eutrophication That Curbs Sexual Selection. Science 277: 1808–1811
  118. 118. Engström-Öst J, Candolin U (2007) Human-induced water turbidity alters selection on sexual displays in sticklebacks. Behav Ecol 18: 393–398
  119. 119. Martins EP, Bissell AN, Morgan KK (1998) Population differences in a lizard communicative display: evidence for rapid change in structure and function. Anim Behav 56: 1113–1119
  120. 120. Speck FG (1924) A Note on the Breeding Habits of Sceloporus. Copeia: 35–36. doi:https://doi.org/10.2307/1437113.
  121. 121. Noble GK, Bradley HT (1933) The Mating Behavior of Lizards; Its Bearing on the Theory of Sexual Selection. Ann N Y Acad Sci 35: 25–100
  122. 122. Cooper J, Burns N (1987) Social significance of ventrolateral coloration in the fence lizard, Sceloporus undulatus. Anim Behav 35: 526–532
  123. 123. Martins EP (1991) Individual and sex differences in the use of the push-up display by the sagebrush lizard, Sceloporus graciosus. Anim Behav 41: 403–416
  124. 124. Quinn VS, Hews DK (2000) Signals and behavioural responses are not coupled in males: aggression affected by replacement of an evolutionarily lost colour signal. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 267: 755–758.