Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Females and Males Rely on Different Cortical Regions in Raven’s Matrices Reasoning Capacity: Evidence from a Voxel-Based Morphometry Study

  • Wenjing Yang ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Wenjing Yang, Peiduo Liu

    Affiliations Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing, China, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

  • Peiduo Liu ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Wenjing Yang, Peiduo Liu

    Affiliations Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing, China, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

  • Dongtao Wei,

    Affiliations Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing, China, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

  • Wenfu Li,

    Affiliations Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing, China, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

  • Glenn Hitchman,

    Affiliations Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing, China, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

  • Xueping Li,

    Affiliation Institute of Education, China West Normal University, Nanchong, China

  • Jiang Qiu ,

    zhangql@swu.edu.cn (QZ); qiuj318@swu.edu.cn (JQ)

    Affiliations Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing, China, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

  • Qinglin Zhang

    zhangql@swu.edu.cn (QZ); qiuj318@swu.edu.cn (JQ)

    Affiliations Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing, China, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

Abstract

Raven’s Matrices test (RMT) is a non-verbal test designed to assess individuals’ ability to reason and solve new problems without relying extensively on declarative knowledge derived from schooling or previous experience. Despite a large number of behavioral studies that demonstrated gender differences in Raven’s Matrices reasoning ability, no neural evidence supported this difference. In this study, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was used in an attempt to uncover the gender-specific neural basis of Raven’s Matrices reasoning ability as measured by the combined Ravens Matrices test (CRT) in 370 healthy young adults. The behavioral results showed no difference between males and females. However, the VBM results showed that the relationship between reasoning ability and regional gray matter volume (rGMV) differed between sexes. The association between CRT scores and rGMV in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (associated with visuospatial ability) was significantly greater in males than in females, whereas the reverse was true for the inferior frontal cortex (relating to verbal reasoning ability) and the medial frontal cortex (engaged in information binding) where the association was greater in females. These findings suggest that males and females use differently structured brains in different ways to achieve similar levels of overall Raven’s Matrices reasoning ability.

Introduction

Raven’s Matrices Test (RMT) is a non-verbal test designed to assess individuals’ ability to reason and solve new problems without relying extensively on declarative knowledge derived from schooling or previous experience [1]. These matrices are composed of a series of nonverbal pictures, each with a missing element that completes a pattern. Subjects are asked to identify the missing element that completes the pattern. Good RMT performance requires that subjects perceive the relationships between cells in the matrices, determine the relationships between the columns and rows of the matrices and then integrate this information. The linguistically minimized nature of the RMT test allows for the measurement of reasoning capacity without the influence of language, education, and cultural factors. Thus, the RMT is considered one of the best indexes of individual differences in reasoning ability [2].

Sex differences in Raven’s Matrices reasoning ability are among the most controversial and interesting topics in this area of study. The results of studies to date have not allowed researchers to reach a clear consensus regarding their conclusions. A number of researchers contest that no gender differences exist in Raven’s Matrices ability [3][5]. In particular, Lynn et al. (2004) reported that no statistically significant difference existed between scores obtained by boys and girls on the Standard Progressive Matrices test for a sample of nine-hundred and twenty 7–10 year olds in Mexico. Furthermore, a recent standardization of the Progressive Matrices in Syria for people aged 7 to 18 years also found no sex differences [3]. However, some investigations of sex differences in the Raven’s Matrices test showed a male advantage [6][9]. Lynn (1998) has analyzed data from England, Hawaii, and Belgium and found that males outperformed females in the Standard Progressive Matrices Test. This result was repeated in a study using the Advanced Progressive Matrices [8]. Meanwhile, some other studies have observed an advantage for females [3], [10]. For example, Khaleefa and Lynn (2008) reported on a large standardization sample of 6–11 year olds who were tested using the Colored Progressive Matrices test in the United Arab Emirates. Girls performed significantly better than boys, but the difference was only small. In another study, the Standard Progressive Matrices was standardized on a sample of 6529 8–15-year olds in Kuwait. The results showed that a small sex difference favored girls [10].

A large body of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) studies have tried to investigate the neural mechanisms of Raven’s Matrices test [11][18]. Some PET studies have noted strong activation in the left parieto-occipital region during Raven’s test performance [19], [20]. Prabhakaran et al. (1997) examined brain activation during solving the Raven’s Progressive Matrices problems in seven young healthy participants. Right frontal and bilateral parietal regions of the brain were activated more by the visuospatial reasoning required by Raven’s test problems compared with control problems. Another study independently manipulated relational complexity and distractor demands in a RMT-like task and observed that bilateral DLPFC activation, extending into FPC in the left hemisphere, increased with relational complexity [15]. Generally, results from fMRI and PET studies suggest that Raven’s Matrices reasoning is associated with activation of a network of frontal and parietal brain regions, specifically the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the superior parietal lobule and intraparietal cortices [21].

To the best of our knowledge, no study has explored the gender-specific neural mechanism of Raven’s Matrices reasoning ability. Some studies identified two general problem solving strategies that could be used to solve the items of the Raven’s Matrices. One is the visual strategy, which involves applying operations of visual perception, such as the superimposition of images upon each other. The other is the verbal strategy, which consists of applying logical operations to features contained within the problem elements [22], [23]. Previous studies have already demonstrated that males and females may use different approaches to solve Raven’s Matrices spatial problems [24], [25]. It is well known that different brain regions underpin visual and verbal abilities [26][35]. If males and females use different strategies to solve Raven’s reasoning problems, it is reasonable to hypothesize that there will be gender-specific neural correlates of Raven’s Matrices reasoning ability [36]. Because functional imaging can only measure active processing which is constrained by the task, it can’t identify the neural substrates of sex-specific abilities. Structural imaging studies are particularly useful for investigating the anatomical correlates of personal characteristics involving a wide range of behaviors [37]. In this study, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was used to explore the gender-specific neural correlates of Raven’s Matrices reasoning ability. Haier et al. [38] used the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) to examine sex differences related to general intelligence. The results showed that the correlation between gray matter volume and general intelligence was stronger in the frontal and parietal lobes for males, whereas a stronger correlation was found in the frontal lobe along with Broca’s area for females. It is well known that RMT has been widely accepted as measurement of general intelligence [2], [39][42]. Based on the previous researches, we hypothesized that males with higher reasoning scores may have an increased regional gray matter volume (rGMV) in the parietal or frontal regions associated with visuospatial ability, whereas females with higher reasoning scores may have a larger rGMV in the inferior frontal regions associated with verbal-related abilities.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

The experiment was approved by the Academic Committee of the School of Psychology and the Brain Imaging Center Institutional Review Board of Southwest University in China. All participants signed an informed consent form prior to their inclusion in the study.

Participants

A total of 384 right-handed, healthy volunteers (194 females; mean age: 19.82±1.31; and 190 males; mean age: 20.22±1.37) from the Southwest University in China participated in this study, as part of our ongoing project to examine the association among brain imaging, creativity, and mental health. All participants were native Chinese speakers, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants were screened to confirm healthy development by a self-report questionnaire survey before the scanning, and thus, those participants who had a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, received mental health treatment or taken psychiatric medications were excluded. Among the participants, nine were excluded because they did not take part in the behavioral portion of the study. Another five participants were removed because of excessive head motions. Therefore, 370 participants (190 females, 180 males) were included in the VBM analyses.

Behavioral Examination

Raven’s reasoning matrices are available in three different forms for different aged participants: the Standard Progressive Matrices, the Colored Progressive Matrices and the Advanced Progressive Matrices [43]. In this study, we chose to use the Chinese version of the RMT, the combined Raven’s matrices test (CRT) [44][46]. Participants had up to 40 minutes to finish this test. The CRT consists of the Raven’s standard progressive Matrices (C, D and E sets) and Raven’s colored progressive Matrices (A, B and AB sets). Given that the Colored Progressive Matrices were designed for children aged 5 to 11 years old, some researchers combined the colored and standard Matrices to make the test generalizable to more people. The CRT can be applied to people aged 5 to 75.

Image Acquisition

MR images were acquired on a 3.0-T Siemens Trio MRI scanner (Siemens Medical, Erlangen, Germany). High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo sequence [repetition time (TR) = 1900 ms; echo time (TE) = 2.52 ms; inversion time (TI) = 900 ms; flip angle = 9 degrees; resolution matrix = 256×256; slices = 176; thickness = 1.0 mm; voxel size = 1×1×1 mm].

Preprocessing of Structural Data

The MR images were processed using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) implemented in Matlab 7.8 (MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Each MR image was first displayed in SPM8 to screen for artifacts or gross anatomical abnormalities. For better registration, the reorientation of the images was manually set to the anterior commissure. The images were segmented into gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid by using the new segmentation in SPM8. Subsequently, we performed Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration through Exponentiated Lie (DARTEL) algebra in SPM8 for registration, normalization, and modulation [47]. To ensure that regional differences in the absolute amount of GM were conserved, the image intensity of each voxel was modulated by the Jacobian determinants. Then, registered images were transformed to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. Finally, the normalized modulated images (GM and WM images) were smoothed with a 10 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel to increase signal-to-noise ratio.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses of GMV data were performed using SPM8. This study aimed to investigate whether the relationship between rGMV and Raven’s reasoning scores differed between males and females. Sex differences were tested using the condition by covariate interaction analysis [37], [48]. In the whole brain analysis, sex was treated as a condition. To control for possible confounding variables, age, scores of the CRT, and global volumes of GM were entered as covariates into the model. Aside from total brain volume, all covariates were modeled to make the unique relationship of each covariate with rGMV evident for each sex. The interaction effects between sex and the Raven’s reasoning score on the rGMV was assessed using t-contrasts.

To avoid edge effects around the borders between GM and WM, an absolute threshold masking of 0.2 was used, meaning that voxels with gray matter values lower than 0.2 were excluded from the analyses. For all analyses, the cluster-level statistical threshold was set at P<0.05, and corrected at the non-stationary cluster correction [49] with an underlying voxel level of P<0.001. This was an exploratory study, therefore, we did not use the FDR or FEW approaches for multiple comparison correction.

Results

Sample Descriptive Statistics

A total of 370 healthy participants (190 females, 180 males) were included in the VBM analysis. The mean CRT scores were (66.13±3.13) for males and (66.40±3.05) for females. No significant gender difference (P>0.05) in CRT scores was found between females and males.

VBM Results

A voxel-wise ANCOVA analysis showed that there was an interaction effect between sex and CRT scores on GMV in the following three regions: the first region was in and adjacent to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC, BA9, cluster size = 1130, t = 4.27), the second region was spread around the left IFC (BA45,cluster size = 1106, t = 5.21), and the third region was around the right medial frontal cortex (Medial FC, BA32, cluster size = 2018, t = 4.82) (see Table 1). The CRT scores were positively correlated with GMV in the right DLPFC (r = 0.217, p = 0.006) for males, whereas no significant correlation was found for the females in this region (r = 0.02, p = 0.776) (Figure 1A). The CRT scores of females were positively correlated with the GMV in one cluster adjacent to the left IFC (r = 0.176, p = 0.011), whereas a negative correlation was found for males (r = −0.284, p = 0.000) (Figure 1B). In addition, the Raven test scores for females also showed a positive correlation with the GMV in the cluster adjacent to the right Medial FC (r = 0.184, p = 0.008), whereas the scores for males were negatively correlated with the GMV of this cluster (r = −0.281, p = 0.000) (Figure1C). The scatterplot between CRT scores and regional gray matter volume (rGMV) is shown for illustration purpose. They likely overestimate the effects because the signal of peak voxel was extracted.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Sex modulates the effect of reasoning ability on gray matter in the Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the Medial frontal cortex (MFC), and the Inferior frontal cortex (IFC).

The color density represents the T score. The scatter plot between CRT scores and the rGMV is shown for illustration purpose only.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093104.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. Sex modulates the effect of reasoning ability on gray matter in the following regions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093104.t001

Discussion

This study investigated the sex-specific neuroanatomical differences underlying the reasoning ability as measured by the CRT in a large sample of participants. VBM analysis revealed distinct differences in the contribution of various cortical regions to reasoning ability between males and females. Males’ reasoning scores were positively correlated with the rGMV in the right DLPFC, whereas females’ scores showed a positive correlation with the rGMV in the left IFC and right Medial FC. Our results may indicate that females and males appear to rely on different neural substrates to achieve comparable reasoning performances in the CRT. Some studies have suggested that Raven’s reasoning Matrices may measure different abilities in males and females [24]. Our study provided a basis for explaining these findings in terms of neurological structure substrates.

The positive correlation between the rGMV of the DLPFC and the reasoning scores for the male participants indicates that males may rely more on the DLPFC to obtain higher reasoning scores [50]. DLPFC has been documented to serve an important function in maintaining spatial information and using this information to guide a correct response [26], [27], [51][54]. fMRI studies have revealed significant activation in the lateral prefrontal cortex while subjects performed visuospatial tasks such as the spatial navigation task, the delayed-response task, the mental rotation task or some tasks that require the temporary maintenance of spatial information [26], [52], [55][57]. Some evidence from visuospatial tasks which requires spatial representation and memory, such as the mental rotation task and the spatial navigation task, suggest that males have an advantage in visuospatial ability [28], [58][60]. Other studies have demonstrated that visual strategies are important in solving Raven’s matrices [22], [23] and males tend to use their visual-spatial ability while solving these items [8], [22], [61]. Males’ habitual use of this ability may cause an augmentation of the brain regions which underpin it [36]. Our results may therefore indicate that males rely more than females on the DLPFC associated with the visuospatial ability to solve the Raven’s Matrices reasoning problems.

The increased rGMV in the IFC for females may suggest that females obtain higher reasoning scores as measured by the CRT through verbal-analytic reasoning ability. A large body of neuroimaging evidence has shown that the left IFC is engaged in verbal correlated processes, such as phonologic and semantic operations [62][65]. The mental logic theory of reasoning suggests that reasoning relies on a language-like structure and should be supported by the language areas [66], [67]. Previous studies showed that verbal-analytic reasoning was an important strategy in solving spatial-format problems and females tend to use their verbal ability when solving these reasoning problems [23], [68]. This strategy requires the existence of a verbal representation of the stimuli and applies logical operations to features contained within elements of the problem matrices [22], [61]. Females’ frequent use of their verbal ability may be the underlying cause of the positive correlation between the reasoning ability and the rGMV of IFG, which is thought to underpin this ability [36]. The increased rGMV in the IFC also suggests that females rely more than males on the IFC when solving spatial-format problems.

In addition, the results also showed that an increased rGMV for females in the Medial FC was associated with a higher reasoning score. Some studies showed that some answers to simple items in the Raven’s Progressive Matrices tests are easily obtained by perception of the pattern as a gestalt, such that the appropriate piece for its completion can be identified without the use of reasoning [69]. During this matching process, it is thought that participants retain the original pattern in working memory and try to judge whether the optional pattern can match the original one. The initial pattern must be kept online, and these two discontinuous events must be bound together for a conclusion to be made. Some neuroimaging studies have shown that the Medial FC is engaged in bounding items together to obtain a successful associative memory of information [70][72]. Thus, the positive correlation for females between the rGMV of the Medial FC and reasoning ability may suggest that females rely more on the ability which binds separately presented items before making a decision. In addition, some studies documented that females tend to outperform males in perceptual tasks in which subjects must rapidly identify matching items [73]. The negative correlation between the males’ scores and the rGMV of the Medial FC may also support this notion.

There were some similarities between the results of our study and those of the first study which attempted to explore the sex specific neural mechanisms of general intelligence conducted by Haier et al. [38]. Haier’s study used the WAIS to assess general intelligence and observed that the correlation between gray matter volume and intelligence was stronger in the frontal and parietal lobes (BA 8, 9, 39, and 40) for males, whereas a stronger correlation was found in the frontal lobe (BA10) along with Broca’s area for females. It is well known that the Raven’s Matrices test is also a good assessment of general intelligence [2], [39][42]. Therefore, although the two studies used different measures of general intelligence, they both yielded results which support the notion that males and females may use different brain structures in different ways to achieve similar levels of general intelligence [39].

In conclusion, this study provided evidence supporting the gender-specific neuroanatomical structure in reasoning performance as measured by the CRT. The results suggest that males rely more on the DLPFC associated with visuospatial ability to achieve a high reasoning score, whereas females rely more on the IFC connected to verbal processing ability and the medial FC associated with information binding ability. No significant differences between genders in the behavioral results of reasoning performance as assessed by CRT were found. These results may indicate that males and females use different brain structures in different ways to achieve similar levels of overall Raven’s reasoning performance. One limitation of this study was that participants were college students who may all share a similar level of reasoning performance. Thus, caution should be taken before generalizing these explanations to a wider population.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: WY JQ PL QZ. Performed the experiments: WY PL. Analyzed the data: DW WL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: WY PL DW XL. Wrote the paper: WY GH.

References

  1. 1. Carpenter PA, Just MA, Shell P (1990) What one intelligence test measures: a theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological review 97: 404–431.
  2. 2. McGrew KS, Flanagan DP (1998) The intelligence test desk reference (ITDR): Gf-Gc cross-battery assessment. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
  3. 3. Khaleefa O, Lynn R (2008) A study of intelligence in the United Arab Emirates. Mankind quarterly 49: 58–64.
  4. 4. Lynn R, Backhoff E, Contreras-Niño LA (2004) Sex differences on< i> g</i>, reasoning and visualisation tested by the progressive matrices among 7–10 year olds: some normative data for Mexico. Personality and Individual Differences 36: 779–787.
  5. 5. Rushton JP, Jensen AR (2010) The rise and fall of the Flynn Effect as a reason to expect a narrowing of the Black–White IQ gap. Intelligence 38: 213–219.
  6. 6. Irwing P, Lynn R (2005) Sex differences in means and variability on the progressive matrices in university students: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychology 96: 505–524.
  7. 7. Rushton JP, Skuy M (2001) Performance on Raven’s Matrices by African and White university students in South Africa. Intelligence 28: 251–265.
  8. 8. Colom R, García-López O (2002) Sex differences in fluid intelligence among high school graduates. Personality and Individual differences 32: 445–451.
  9. 9. Lynn R (1998) Sex differences in intelligence: A rejoinder to Mackintosh. Journal of Biosocial Science 30: 529–532.
  10. 10. Abdel-Khalek AM, Lynn R (2006) Sex differences on the Standard Progressive Matrices and in educational attainment in Kuwait. Personality and individual differences 40: 175–182.
  11. 11. Prabhakaran V, Smith JA, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD (1997) Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Cognitive psychology 33: 43–63.
  12. 12. Christoff K, Prabhakaran V, Dorfman J, Zhao Z, Kroger JK, et al. (2001) Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage 14: 1136–1149.
  13. 13. Lee KH, Choi YY, Gray JR, Cho SH, Chae J-H, et al. (2006) Neural correlates of superior intelligence: Stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. Neuroimage 29: 578–586.
  14. 14. Haier RJ, Siegel Jr BV, Nuechterlein KH, Hazlett E, Wu JC, et al. (1988) Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography. Intelligence 12: 199–217.
  15. 15. Kroger JK, Sabb FW, Fales CL, Bookheimer SY, Cohen MS, et al. (2002) Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. Cerebral cortex 12: 477–485.
  16. 16. Desco M, Navas-Sanchez FJ, Sanchez-González J, Reig S, Robles O, et al. (2011) Mathematically gifted adolescents use more extensive and more bilateral areas of the fronto-parietal network than controls during executive functioning and fluid reasoning tasks. Neuroimage 57: 281–292.
  17. 17. Haier RJ, White NS, Alkire MT (2003) Individual differences in general intelligence correlate with brain function during nonreasoning tasks. Intelligence 31: 429–441.
  18. 18. Kalbfleisch ML, Van Meter JW, Zeffiro TA (2007) The influences of task difficulty and response correctness on neural systems supporting fluid reasoning. Cognitive Neurodynamics 1: 71–84.
  19. 19. Berman KF, Illowsy BP, Weinberger DR (1988) Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: IV. Further evidence for regional and behavioral specificity. Archives of General Psychiatry 45: 616–622.
  20. 20. Risberg J (1986) Regional cerebral blood flow in neuropsychology. Neuropsychologia 24: 135–140.
  21. 21. Jung RE, Haier RJ (2007) The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 135–154.
  22. 22. Lynn R, Allik J, Irwing P (2004) Sex differences on three factors identified in Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices. Intelligence 32: 411–424.
  23. 23. Mackintosh N, Bennett E (2005) What do Raven’s matrices measure? An analysis in terms of sex differences. Intelligence 33: 663–674.
  24. 24. Lim TK (1994) Gender-related differences in intelligence: Application of confirmatory factor analysis. Intelligence 19: 179–192.
  25. 25. Hyde JS, Geiringer ER, Yen WM (1975) On the empirical relation between spatial ability and sex differences in other aspects of cognitive performance. Multivariate Behavioral Research 10: 289–309.
  26. 26. D’Esposito M, Ballard D, Zarahn E, Aguirre G (2000) The role of prefrontal cortex in sensory memory and motor preparation: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 11: 400–408.
  27. 27. Funahashi S (2013) Space representation in the prefrontal cortex. Progress in Neurobiology 103: 131–155.
  28. 28. Astur RS, Ortiz ML, Sutherland RJ (1998) A characterization of performance by men and women in a virtual Morris water task: A large and reliable sex difference. Behavioural brain research 93: 185–190.
  29. 29. Clements A, Rimrodt S, Abel J, Blankner J, Mostofsky S, et al. (2006) Sex differences in cerebral laterality of language and visuospatial processing. Brain and language 98: 150–158.
  30. 30. Butler T, Imperato-McGinley J, Pan H, Voyer D, Cordero J, et al. (2006) Sex differences in mental rotation: Top–down versus bottom–up processing. Neuroimage 32: 445–456.
  31. 31. Rubia K, Hyde Z, Halari R, Giampietro V, Smith A (2010) Effects of age and sex on developmental neural networks of visual–spatial attention allocation. Neuroimage 51: 817–827.
  32. 32. Kaiser A, Kuenzli E, Zappatore D, Nitsch C (2007) On females’ lateral and males’ bilateral activation during language production: a fMRI study. International Journal of Psychophysiology 63: 192–198.
  33. 33. Meyer GF, Greenlee M, Wuerger S (2011) Interactions between auditory and visual semantic stimulus classes: evidence for common processing networks for speech and body actions. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 23: 2291–2308.
  34. 34. Gauthier CT, Duyme M, Zanca M, Capron C (2009) Sex and performance level effects on brain activation during a verbal fluency task: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Cortex 45: 164–176.
  35. 35. Wallentin M (2009) Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain and language 108: 175–183.
  36. 36. Kanai R, Rees G (2011) The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12: 231–242.
  37. 37. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Nouchi R, Sekiguchi A, Kotozaki Y, et al. (2014) Regional gray matter density is associated with achievement motivation: evidence from voxel-based morphometry. Brain Structure and Function 219: 71–83.
  38. 38. Haier RJ, Jung RE, Yeo RA, Head K, Alkire MT (2005) The neuroanatomy of general intelligence: sex matters. NeuroImage 25: 320–327.
  39. 39. Deary IJ, Penke L, Johnson W (2010) The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 201–211.
  40. 40. Alderton DL, Larson GE (1990) Dimensionality of Raven’s advanced progressive matrices items. Educational and Psychological Measurement 50: 887–900.
  41. 41. Cattell RB (1963) Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of educational psychology 54: 1–22.
  42. 42. Raven JC (1938) Progressive matrices: A perceptual test of intelligence. London: HK Lewis.
  43. 43. Raven J (2000) The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive psychology 41: 1–48.
  44. 44. Li D, Hu k-d, Cheng G-p, Jin Y (1988) The testing results report on the combined Raven’s test in Shanghai. Psychological Science: 27–31.
  45. 45. Wang D, Qian M (1989) The revised report of the combined Raven’s test in countryside of china. Reports of the psychological science 5: 23–27.
  46. 46. Wang D, Di M, Qian M (2007) A report on the third revision of combined raven’s test (CRT- C3) for children in China. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology 15: 559–568.
  47. 47. Ashburner J (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38: 95–113.
  48. 48. Yamasue H, Abe O, Suga M, Yamada H, Rogers MA, et al. (2008) Sex-linked neuroanatomical basis of human altruistic cooperativeness. Cerebral Cortex 18: 2331–2340.
  49. 49. Hayasaka S, Phan KL, Liberzon I, Worsley KJ, Nichols TE (2004) Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. Neuroimage 22: 676–687.
  50. 50. Newton JH, McGrew KS (2010) Introduction to the special issue: Current research in Cattell–Horn–Carroll–based assessment. Psychology in the Schools 47: 621–634.
  51. 51. Goldman-Rakic PS (1996) Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93: 13473–13480.
  52. 52. Curtis CE, D’Esposito M (2003) Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in cognitive sciences 7: 415–423.
  53. 53. Diwadkar VA, Carpenter PA, Just MA (2000) Collaborative activity between parietal and dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex in dynamic spatial working memory revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 12: 85–99.
  54. 54. Just MA, Carpenter PA, Maguire M, Diwadkar V, McMains S (2001) Mental rotation of objects retrieved from memory: an fMRI study of spatial processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130: 493–504.
  55. 55. Curtis CE, D’Esposito M (2006) Selection and maintenance of saccade goals in the human frontal eye fields. Journal of Neurophysiology 95: 3923–3927.
  56. 56. Postle BR, Berger JS, Taich AM, D’esposito M (2006) Activity in human frontal cortex associated with spatial working memory and saccadic behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12: 2–14.
  57. 57. Brown M, DeSouza J, Goltz HC, Ford K, Menon RS, et al. (2004) Comparison of memory-and visually guided saccades using event-related fMRI. Journal of neurophysiology 91: 873–889.
  58. 58. Markowska AL (1999) Sex dimorphisms in the rate of age-related decline in spatial memory: relevance to alterations in the estrous cycle. The Journal of neuroscience 19: 8122–8133.
  59. 59. Lewin C, Wolgers G, Herlitz A (2001) Sex differences favoring women in verbal but not in visuospatial episodic memory. Neuropsychology 15: 165–173.
  60. 60. Postma A, Jager G, Kessels RP, Koppeschaar HP, van Honk J (2004) Sex differences for selective forms of spatial memory. Brain and cognition 54: 24–34.
  61. 61. DeShon RP, Chan D, Weissbein DA (1995) Verbal overshadowing effects on Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices: Evidence for multidimensional performance determinants. Intelligence 21: 135–155.
  62. 62. Bookheimer S (2002) Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annual review of neuroscience 25: 151–188.
  63. 63. Stuss DT, Alexander MP, Hamer L, Palumbo C, Dempster R, et al. (1998) The effects of focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal fluency. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 4: 265–278.
  64. 64. Price CJ (2000) The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging. Journal of anatomy 197: 335–359.
  65. 65. Petersen SE, Fox PT, Posner MI, Mintun M, Raichle ME (1988) Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. Nature 331: 585–589.
  66. 66. Braine MD (1978) On the relation between the natural logic of reasoning and standard logic. Psychological Review 85: 1–21.
  67. 67. Rodriguez-Moreno D, Hirsch J (2009) The dynamics of deductive reasoning: An fMRI investigation. Neuropsychologia 47: 949–961.
  68. 68. Abad FJ, Colom R, Rebollo I, Escorial S (2004) Sex differential item functioning in the Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices: Evidence for bias. Personality and individual differences 36: 1459–1470.
  69. 69. Van der Ven A, Ellis J (2000) A Rasch analysis of Raven’s standard progressive matrices. Personality and Individual Differences 29: 45–64.
  70. 70. Qin S, Piekema C, Petersson KM, Han B, Luo J, et al. (2007) Probing the transformation of discontinuous associations into episodic memory: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 38: 212–222.
  71. 71. Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR (2004) Encoding-specific effects of social cognition on the neural correlates of subsequent memory. The Journal of Neuroscience 24: 4912–4917.
  72. 72. Wagner AD (1999) Working memory contributions to human learning and remembering. Neuron 22: 19–22.
  73. 73. Kimura D (1992) Sex differences in the brain. Scientific American 267: 118–125.