Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Mating Systems, Reproductive Success, and Sexual Selection in Secretive Species: A Case Study of the Western Diamond-Backed Rattlesnake, Crotalus atrox

Abstract

Long-term studies of individual animals in nature contribute disproportionately to our understanding of the principles of ecology and evolution. Such field studies can benefit greatly from integrating the methods of molecular genetics with traditional approaches. Even though molecular genetic tools are particularly valuable for species that are difficult to observe directly, they have not been widely adopted. Here, we used molecular genetic techniques in a 10-year radio-telemetric investigation of the western diamond-backed rattlesnake (Crotalus atrox) for an analysis of its mating system and to measure sexual selection. Specifically, we used microsatellite markers to genotype 299 individuals, including neonates from litters of focal females to ascertain parentage using full-pedigree likelihood methods. We detected high levels of multiple paternity within litters, yet found little concordance between paternity and observations of courtship and mating behavior. Larger males did not father significantly more offspring, but we found evidence for size-specific male-mating strategies, with larger males guarding females for longer periods in the mating seasons. Moreover, the spatial proximity of males to mothers was significantly associated with reproductive success. Overall, our field observations alone would have been insufficient to quantitatively measure the mating system of this population of C. atrox, and we thus urge more widespread adoption of molecular tools by field researchers studying the mating systems and sexual selection of snakes and other secretive taxa.

Introduction

The long-term study of individual organisms in nature plays a central role in our understanding of ecology and evolution [1]. Despite logistical difficulties, long-term investigations are necessary to interpret the processes affecting survival and reproduction played out over the course of multiple years, or even decades [2]. However, such studies are few and there is a strong taxonomic bias. For example, all of the studies cited in Clutton-Brock and Sheldon [1] concern mammals and birds. This bias toward endotherms is most likely related to the fact that these species can be observed in nature more easily. Consequently, a large number of terrestrial vertebrates remain severely understudied [3], [4]. These species often exhibit some combination of being small, cryptic, secretive, nocturnal, and sedentary. Hence, in our view, long-term studies on these taxa may disproportionately benefit from techniques that allow researchers to infer patterns of intraspecific interactions that are difficult or not possible to document by way of direct observation.

Molecular genetics can be used to genotype individuals, measure relatedness, determine parentage, and unravel patterns of social structure and mating systems that would otherwise be extremely difficult to document [5], [6]. Despite the obvious benefits of this approach in addressing questions in behavioral ecology and evolution, molecular genetic tools have not been widely adopted for some taxa. For example, the mating systems of hundreds of avian species have been quantified and characterized using molecular genetic approaches [7], [8]. In sharp contrast, although Gibbs and Weatherhead [9] highlighted the utility of molecular genetics to revolutionize our understanding of snake mating systems over a decade ago, only a handful of subsequent studies have used genetic tools in combination with field studies to provide detailed characterizations of sexual behavior and mating systems in wild snakes (see [10]-[14]).

Large species of snakes, such as many viperids, boids and pythonids, are ecologically and economically important, but most are unstudied in nature. They are important predators in many ecosystems and can occur in much higher densities than their endothermic counterparts [15]. Yet, despite sedentary lifestyles, their generally secretive habits make most species difficult to observe directly in nature [9], [16], [17]. However, they are ideal for long-term, individual-based studies employing radio-telemetry, and their relatively high population densities permit the study of large numbers of individuals in relatively small areas [18], [19]. Most ecological studies on viperids, boids and pythonids have focused on population-level measures of movements, habitat use, survival, and demography. Studies of individual behaviors and intraspecific interactions are less common than in other vertebrate groups, probably because acquiring such data through direct observation can be prohibitively time consuming. Use of molecular genetic tools in field studies of snakes and other ectotherms can be both an alternative and adjunct to direct observation [6], [9], [20], [21].

Here, we demonstrate the utility of combining traditional field techniques with molecular genetics in a long-term study of a North American pitviper (Serpentes; Viperidae). Specifically, we quantified the behavioral and genetic mating system of western diamond-backed rattlesnakes (Crotalus atrox) in southern Arizona for 10 consecutive years (2001-2010) by incorporating direct observation of radio-tagged individuals, opportunistic observation, and tissue sampling (blood, shed skins of adults and neonates) of both radio-tagged and incidental subjects. We used a polymorphic panel of 27 microsatellite markers to genotype all individuals sampled over the course of our decade long study, and infer patterns of parentage and mating from the genetic analyses. We asked the following questions: 1) Are litters sired by multiple males, and if so, what is the frequency of multiple paternity? 2) Do larger males sire more offspring? 3) Are genetic fathers the same males we see attending and courting females in the mating seasons prior to parturition? 4) Is litter size affected by level of paternity (i.e., single father vs. multiple fathers)? 5) Do males sire more offspring when they are spatially closer to mothers?

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Arizona State University approved this study (protocol 98-429R), and appropriate scientific permits were obtained from the Arizona Game and Fish Department.

Study system

We studied a single population of C. atrox in southern Arizona (USA) for 10 consecutive years (2001–2010). The research site, the Suizo Mountains (SMs) located in Pinal County (Arizona), is 40 km SSE of the city of Florence, 8 km W of State Route 79 [22], [23]. We measured, weighed, and collected blood samples for DNA analysis from all adults encountered at this site. We also surgically implanted radio-transmitters into a subset of adults. We tracked these individuals by foot minimally 2-4 times per month from 2001 to 2010. Tracking was increased substantially (sometimes daily or twice daily) from early August through mid-September, the period of birthing in C. atrox [22], [23]. For each radio-tracked subject located, UTM coordinates were recorded using a hand-held Global Positioning System (GPS) unit.

Research site

The focal area encompasses 3 km2 at the western edge of the SMs (32°40′08′ ′N, 111°07′22′ ′W, Conus 27). The SMs have a summit elevation of 947 m. The region is designated as Sonoran Desert, Arizona Upland Desert-Scrub subdivision [24], [25]. In addition to mountainous terrain, the general topography is bajada and desert flats, intersected by dry washes of varying sizes. Annual rain patterns of the Sonoran Desert are bimodal [26], [27]. Gentle to moderate broad frontal storms occur from late fall to early spring (November–March), and strong to violent, often localized convective storms occur from mid- to late summer (early July to mid-September), the period of the North American monsoon [26]. Free water is rarely available and highly unpredictable at the SMs.

Ecology

The western diamond-backed rattlesnake (C. atrox) is a large-bodied pitviper (Serpentes: Viperidae). Throughout its wide geographic distribution in the western United States and Mexico [28], [29], C. atrox exhibits minor morphological variation [30] and shallow genetic (mtDNA) differences [31], yet adult body size varies significantly [30]. In Arizona, adult body sizes of adult C. atrox in different populations show significant differences and male-biased sexual size dimorphism [32]. Arizona has broad physiographic structure and multiple biotic communities within a relatively narrow latitudinal range [24], and C. atrox occupies the southern half of the state and most of its biotic regions [29]. Adults of both sexes in C. atrox exhibit significantly larger snout-vent length (SVL) in regions of Arizona that are wetter and cooler [32], two variables that are associated with increases in their common prey, such as small mammals, birds and lizards [33]. Presumably, this is linked to increases in prey opportunities to acquire sufficient body reserves, especially in females, for reproduction and growth [22], [23], [34]. In some regions, C. atrox is extremely abundant and frequently reported as the dominant snake species, sometimes even the dominant vertebrate predator [15].

Phenology of mating behaviour

Knowledge of the behavioral and genetic mating system of C. atrox is in its infancy, and no study to date has robustly characterized male and female mating strategies and quantified reproductive success in nature. However, qualitative components of the mating system of C. atrox are characteristic of other species of North American rattlesnakes and other pitvipers [35]. In C. atrox and other pitviper species, there are two distinct mating seasons that occur prior to the period of ovulation (for recent reviews, see [36], [37]). In male C. atrox (adult min SVL  =  600 mm; [38]), spermatogenesis is initiated in spring and completed by late summer or early fall, and this sperm cohort is stored in the ductus deferens and used in the first mating season (late summer and fall), as well as the second mating season in spring that immediately follows hibernation (see [22], [23]).

At the SMs, the first mating season commences in mid- to late August, with males searching for and attending females, followed by courtship and coitus in early September, persisting through October. At SMs, our earliest observation of copulation is 2 September, and the latest is 15 October. Adult females typically undergo skin shedding (ecdysis) prior to or during the first mating season, whereas males typically shed from late October through November, at or near their den sites (G. W. Schuett & R. A. Repp unpubl. data). Unlike many rattlesnakes, female C. atrox from the SMs and nearby areas do not undergo major vitellogenesis in late summer and autumn; rather. females enter hibernation in November with small follicles [22], [23], [37], [39]. The period of sexual inactivity (hibernation) lasts about 120 to 130 days, from late October to early March.

The second mating season commences in mid-March, and persists to mid-May. In individuals that den (hibernate) communally, courtship, copulation, and male-male fights occur at or near the dens from mid- March to early April [22], [23] (G.W. Schuett & R.A. Repp, unpubl. data). After egress and mating in early spring, ova undergo rapid development (vitellogenesis) and ovulation occurs in late spring (May). Following the second mating season, adults of both sexes undergo ecdysis, typically from late May through June (G.W. Schuett & R.A. Repp unpubl. data). Births occur from early August to mid-September [22], [23], [39], but are centered in mid- to late August [23].

Sexual behaviour, male fighting, and coitus

Like all snakes, male C. atrox are the mate-seeking sex [40]-[42], and courtship by males involves stereotypic behaviours [43] (G.W. Schuett & R.A. Repp unpubl data). In rattlesnakes, including C. atrox, attendance and courtship in nature can be protracted [41], [44], requiring days or even weeks before mating is effected. Coitus in C. atrox lasts 24 h or longer [28], [43] (GW Schuett & RA Repp, unpubl. Data; Figure 1). Wild males do not mate multiply with the same female in a single breeding season (G.W. Schuett & R.A. Repp unpubl. data). Male C. atrox engage in ritualistic, physical combat (without use of venom) for priority-of-access to females during the mating seasons [45], [46], and larger males tend to win fights and maintain dominance [43], [47] (G.W. Schuett & R.A. Repp unpubl data).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Sexual behavior in Crotalus atrox.

(a) Pair of C. atrox in coitus. Unknown male (left) with female CA-3, September 13, 2001. (b) Pair of C. atrox in courtship below a den site. The lower body and tail of unknown male is wrapped over and around tail of female CA-44 (painted rattles), March 2012. Photographs by Roger Repp.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090616.g001

Frequency of female reproduction and operational sex ratio (OSR)

Many female viperid snakes are long-lived (> 20 years), iteroparous, and show patterns of reproduction that are less than annual [39]-[41]. But recent work on C. atrox at the SMs has demonstrated that while a biennial pattern is typical for females, annual reproduction is not uncommon [22], [23]. The adult sex ratio at SMs appears to be at unity (G.W. Schuett & R.A. Repp unpubl data) and the operational sex ratio (OSR), which is the ratio of sexually competing males to sexually active females, is roughly 2:1 owing to the female reproductive patterns (i.e., skipping reproduction). The OSR is an important metric in sexual selection analyses [40], [41], [48]-[50]. When the OSR is male-biased, and hence fewer opportunities for males to obtain mates, sexual selection is predicted to act more strongly on males [40], [41].

Determining female reproductive status

In each year subjects were radio-tracked and assigned a reproductive status of either pregnant (when they produced a litter) or non-pregnant. Following parturition, the reproductive status of a female changed to postpartum through the end of that year, and in the following year was re-assigned as either pregnant or non-pregnant. Like their viper relatives, female C. atrox are noticeably robust when pregnant [22], [23], [34], [39]. Thus, based on their increased mass, we were able to readily detect reproductive from non-reproductive females by mid- or late June. Births occur from early August to mid- September at sheltered sites, such as packrat (Neotoma albigularis) middens (nests), small mammal burrows, or rock shelves, but generally not at or near winter refugia [22], [23], [51]. Parturition was deemed imminent when female movements from these sites were greatly reduced or ceased; thus, radio-tracking efforts were typically increased to 1-2 times per day to better pinpoint birth dates. The number of offspring observed (or their molts) determined litter size. All mothers remained at birth sites until their progeny underwent their natal ecdysis, which occurred 5 to 7 days post-birth [22], [23]. Maternal attendance has been described in other populations of C. atrox [52], and it is a common feature in most species of rattlesnakes and other pitvipers of temperate North America [53], [54].

Capture and immediate processing

Subjects were captured using conventional snake hooks and plastic grabbers. The capture process, which involved grabbing a snake and placing it into a clear plastic tube for temporary restraint (1 m length; diameter varied in size), was done gently and typically required less than 1 min to minimize handling stress [55]. In many cases, individuals were located basking at or near the entrances of dens in spring (March-April). At the time of capture Global Positioning System (GPS) coordinates were collected as Universal Transverse Mercators (UTMs).

Within 24-h of the initial capture, subjects were measured (snout-vent length, tail length, head dimensions to the nearest 1-mm; body mass to the nearest 1.0 g) and sex confirmed (via probing) while under anesthesia (isoflurane). For identification purposes, individuals were photographed, implanted with a unique PIT-tag (AVID, Inc., Norco, California, U.S.A.), and their proximal rattle segments were colored using Sharpie pens. In subjects selected for radio-tracking, each had an appropriately sized (< 5% of the total body mass; [56]) temperature-sensitive radio-transmitter (models SI-2T and AI-2T, 11-16 g; Holohil Inc., Carp, Ontario, Canada) surgically implanted within the coelom following general procedures used for snakes [22], [23]. After processing, all subjects were released at the exact site of capture.

Tissue Sampling

All adult subjects had small amounts (0.1-0.3 ml) of blood harvested from caudal vessels for DNA parentage and kin analyses [23]. All blood was preserved in 95-100% ethanol, stored cold (0-10 C) and in darkness. Similar to other rattlesnakes [53], [54], neonates (littermates) of C. atrox remain with their mother until they shed their natal skin (4-7 days) and disperse [22] (G. W. Schuett & R. A. Repp unpubl. data). Because neonates are often difficult to sample without causing extensive damage to birth site microhabitat, we primarily used noninvasive sampling procedures to collect DNA from them. This was achieved by returning to birth sites (using GPS coordinates) after their post-shed dispersal and collected sheds found at or near the entrance of the birth site. Also, we opportunistically collected all other shed skins (all adults) found in the field, determining size and estimating sex of individuals by measuring the shed skin (SVL and tail length) and counting the subcaudal scales.

Genotyping

We extracted DNA from blood and scale tissues using a salt extraction protocol and from shed skins using Qiagen DNEasy Tissue Kits (QIAGEN). We genotyped all individuals using 27 of the 30 microsatellite markers that amplified in C. atrox in Pozarowski et al. [57] and used their same genotyping methodology.

We excluded three of the loci that showed significant levels of linkage disequilibrium in their analysis (Ca2_23, Crti47, and Scu07) [57]. These markers included 13 (Ca1_14, Ca1_20, Ca1_22, Ca1_31, Ca1_39, Ca1_43, Ca2_27, Ca2_38, Ca2_64, Ca2_71, Ca2_74, Ca2_81, Ca2_90) developed by Pozarowski et al. [57], two (Crti09, Crti10) developed by Goldberg et al. [58], six (Crti12, Crti14, Crti23, Crti32A, Crti37, Crti95) developed by Munguia-Vega et al. [59], three (CwA14, CwA29, CwB6) developed by Holycross et al. [60], two (MFR15, MFR23) developed by Oyler-McCance et al. [61], and one (Scu05) developed by Gibbs et al. [62].

PCR amplification and genotyping procedures follow Pozarowski et al. [57]. All forward primers were 5′ end labeled with a tag (5′-GGAAACAGCTATGACCATG-3′) for tailed PCR with an M13 primer labeled with a 6-FAM, HEX, NED or PET (Invitrogen and Applied Biosystems fluorophores) [63]. All reverse primers were 5′ end labeled with a PIG-tail (5′- GTTTCTT -3′) to prevent adenylation [55], [64].

Microsatellite loci were amplified in 10 µl reaction volumes containing: 1.0-3.5 µM MgCl, 0.8 µM dNTPs, 0.2 µM forward M13 primer, 0.4 µM reverse primer, 0.4 µM M13 fluorescently-label, and 0.1units Platinum Taq DNA polymerase (Invitrogen) and 10ng of template DNA. We used an initial 3 min denaturation at 94°C, followed by 35 cycles of denaturation (94°C for 30s), annealing (50°C-62°C for 30s), and extension (72°C for 30s) with a final extension of 3 min at 72°C. Amplified microsatellite loci were pooled according to size and fluorophores followed by fragment analysis with an ABI 3130xl sequencer. To score microsatellite alleles we used the genotype analysis software GeneMarker v1.85 (SoftGenetics, State College, PA, USA).

Spatial analyses

To perform spatial analyses, all UTM coordinates were transferred into ArcView 3.2 Spatial Analysis software (Environmental Systems Research Institute, Inc) and movement parameters were analyzed using the Animal Movement extension. Portions of these spatial data for females have been presented elsewhere [23,23]. We performed three different types of spatial analyses to examine relationships between male proximity and parentage: home range size, pairwise home range overlap between all individuals that were radio-tracked for at least one full year, and geographic midpoint comparisons.

Although kernel density estimators are commonly used for determining home range sizes of endotherms, recent analyses indicate they may not perform as well as minimum convex polygons (MCPs) for snakes and other herpetofauna [22], [23], [65]. Thus, we used ArcGIS 3.2 to compute 95% MCPs for each adult individual radio-tracked. These MCPs represent the smallest polygon that incorporates 95% of the relocations for an individual. To produce a single value for the degree of overlap for each pair, we calculated the average overlap for the two individuals in each pair as (AB/A + AB/B)/2, where A is the home range size of individual A, B is the home range size of individual B, and AB is the area shared by both A and B. Using this method we generated a pairwise matrix of average home range overlap values that could be compared to the probability of a male and a female sharing parentage in a litter.

Although we only had enough relocation data to calculate home ranges for the subset of individuals that had radio-transmitters, we computed the geographic center of all capture and recapture locations for all individuals using the online calculator Geo Midpoint (www.geomidpoint.com). This gave us a single location that represented the geographic center of all spatial locations where that individual had been recaptured over the course of our decade of fieldwork. We used the Geographic Distance Matrix Generator (biodiversityinformatics.amnh.org) to compute pairwise distances between geographic midpoints of all adult snakes. Because individuals use distinct spatial locations during summer (active home range) and winter (inactive overwintering range), we computed separate geographic midpoints for summer (May – September) and winter (October – April) relocations [23], [51].

Parentage analyses

We used the software COLONY 2 [66] to examine the relationships among litters of neonates and identify any potential mothers or fathers of those litters within our field sample. COLONY 2 is well suited for this analysis because our data set contains several clusters of individuals known to be siblings (e.g., neonates from the same litter), often with a mother identified from field data. COLONY 2 uses a full-pedigree likelihood approach to estimating parentage, jointly considering both sibship and parentage relationships. By examining data from multiple offspring simultaneously, the probability that both parental alleles are represented increases, leading to more accurate parentage inference. COLONY 2 also has robust genotyping error models that can account for a relatively high frequency of genotyping errors both from allelic drop out and other sources [67]. Furthermore, a recent analysis using simulated data found that COLONY 2 outperformed other popular parentage inference methods, and was highly accurate with the use of 15 or more polymorphic markers [68].

In order to maximize the ability of COLONY 2 to assign parentage in our data set, we first determined locus-specific error rates for the remaining 27 loci. We used the program MICROERRORANALYZER, which implements the likelihood error estimates detailed in Wang [69], to estimate rates of null alleles, allelic dropout, and false alleles in a data set containing known parent-offspring dyads. We discarded loci with combined estimated error rates >20%, leaving us with a total of 18 loci. We then used the locus-specific error rates for these 18 loci in COLONY 2. To assess the ability of COLONY 2 to assign parentage in our sample, we first analyzed our data without assigning sibships to their known mother. Because COLONY 2 was able to identify the field mother as the genetic mother in all cases, we assumed that the likelihood estimation approach used in the program was robust with respect to our data set. In subsequent analyses with COLONY 2 we assigned sibships to known mothers to increase the information available for paternity assignments, and then retained only those paternity assignments with a maximum likelihood probability > 0.95. We conducted two separate runs with our data, and because the parentage assignments were identical across runs, we used these parentage assignments in all subsequent analyses.

Male reproductive success and body size

We compared the body size (snout-vent length, SVL) of males siring progeny (paternity) to those with no detected paternity. Because not all males were measured in the year of which they were deemed fathers or were attending-courting females, we used a corrected estimate of SVL for the year in which parentage was documented (see [11]). Our corrected estimate was based on growth rate estimated from capture-recapture data of adult males at our site. We found males between 700-900 mm SVL increased in length (average) 35 mm per year, but male growth slowed at about 900 mm SVL, and males greater than 900 SVL increased in length (average) 11 mm per year. Thus, our corrected estimate of SVL used the closest year in which we measured the SVL of a male, and then adjusted that SVL up or down in a size-specific manner to reflect the number of intervening years between the paternity event and the measurement year.

Male reproductive success and spatial analyses

We examined the effects of home range size and spatial proximity to mothers (home range overlap and midpoint distances) as factors affecting paternity. For home range size, we compared the average home range size of all males that were identified as fathers in any litter to all males that were not identified as fathers. For spatial proximity, we compared pairwise measures of spatial proximity between the males and female pairs who had parentage in litters to male and female pairs who did not share parentage. The distributions of pairwise values within groups that contain many individuals that do not share parentage do not conform to a normal distribution (the median and modal values of such distributions are usually 0), which makes traditional parametric statistics inappropriate. Thus, we used bootstrap resampling procedures to compare average home range overlap for male-female pairs that share parentage to randomly generated male-female pairs. For each comparison, we generated 1000 samples of random pairs via bootstrapping, with each sample equal in size to the focal group sample size. We calculated two-tailed p-values from the largest confidence interval around the mean of the resampling distribution that did not contain the mean of the focal group. We used the program RESAMPLING STATS for these procedures [70].

We used our field records of male-female pairings to determine the number of times different males were observed in the field paired with females during the mating season, as well as the proportion of times females who produced litters that paired with fathers or non-fathers. Because C. atrox exhibits two mating seasons (late summer-early fall and spring) prior to ovulation, we counted pre-birth pairings that occurred in both the fall and spring mating seasons preceding births. Also, we used our field records to compare the sizes (corrected SVL, see above) of males that were found attending females.

Data Analysis

Unless noted otherwise, all statistical analyses were conducted with SYSTAT 12. Mean values are given as mean ± standard deviation. Before performing parametric tests, data were tested for the assumptions of normality and equal variances [71]. If data violated assumptions of normality or equal variances after transformations, we used non-parametric tests.

Genotyping data, morphological data, and spatial data (viewable via Google Earth) for all individuals are available from the public website, The Copperhead Institute (http://www.copperheadinstitute.org).

Results

Tissue sampling

We collected tissue samples from a total of 324 individuals. Of these, 25 did not produce DNA of sufficient quality for genotyping, leaving us with a total sample of 299 individual genotypes. Of these samples, 191 came from adult individuals sampled from 2001-2010. The other 108 samples came from neonates from 30 different litters produced by 18 different females [32]. Twenty-four of these litters were, to the best of our knowledge, complete litters, whereas only one individual was sampled from the remaining six litters. Mean litter size was 4.3±1.9 neonates. Although the mean litter size in our population is small relative to other regions, both litter size and adult body size vary geographically for this species [32], [34], [39]. Our findings of small and sometimes frequent (e.g., annual) litters are consistent with past research done at this and nearby sites [22], [23], [39], [72].

Radio-tracking

We surgically implanted radio-transmitters in 26 adult females and 20 adult males, which we radio-tracked for a minimum of 1 year; many subjects were radio-tracked for several consecutive years, with a maximum of 7 consecutive years (Table 1). We collected detailed data on space use, mating behavior, and conspecific associations for these 46 individuals, as well as opportunistic data from stochastic sampling and re-sampling of 145 additional adult individuals.

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of individuals tracked via radio-telemetry.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090616.t001

Genetic parentage

We sampled 108 neonates, of which 105 (97.2%) were associated with a marked female (“field mother”) who was presumed to be the parent (Table 2). In all cases, COLONY 2 correctly identified the field mother as the genetic mother of those offspring. Additionally, COLONY 2 was able to assign the 3 “orphan” neonates in our study with no field mother as offspring of sampled females. COLONY 2 assigned the 108 sampled neonates to 27 different fathers. Of these fathers, 18 (66.7%) were known (pit-tagged) subjects. The other 9 fathers were not marked but could be identified as individual genotypes by COLONY 2. We detected multiple paternity in exactly half of the complete litters we sampled (12 of 24 litters, 50%). Five of the 12 litters had three fathers, and two of these cases involved all three offspring in the litter being sired by different fathers (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary of genetic parents identified for all litters of genotyped neonates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090616.t002

Male body size and reproductive success

Males we identified as fathers did not differ significantly in SVL from males with no detected paternity (mean SVL fathers: 951±86 mm; mean SVL non-fathers: 936±104 mm, T = 0.7, p = 0.48) (Figure 2). Of the 24 litters we sampled that had more than one neonate, a single male sired 12 of them and the remaining 12 had two or three fathers (Table 2). Marked males that did not share paternity (single sires of litters) were significantly larger than marked males that shared paternity (mean SVL single paternity: 998±63 mm; mean SVL shared paternity: 916±89 mm, T = 2.6, p = 0.02) (Figure 2). However, there was no correlation between total number of offspring fathered and body size (Pearson Correlation, r = - 0.27 p = 0.14).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Size comparisons among males.

Comparisons show average size and standard error of males identified as genetic fathers versus those that were not (fathers versus non-fathers), males that were the only father identified in a litter versus those that shared paternity with other males (sole paternity verus shared paternity), males that were seen attending females during the mating season versus those that were not seen to do so (attend females versus not attend), and males that attended females for longer than one week versus those attending females less than a week (long attend versus short attend).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090616.g002

Male attendance, mating, and parentage

Males were often found attending females during the two mating seasons, occasionally for long periods (e.g., weeks). Attendance of females was influenced by male body size (Figure 1). Males that were found in attendance with females were larger in SVL than males that were not observed attending females (SVL attending males: 962±87 mm; SVL non-attending males: 913±107 mm, T = 2.4, p = 0.02) (Figure 2). Males that attended the same female for more than one week were significantly larger than males found in attendance for less than one week (SVL prolonged attendance: 1005±71 mm; SVL single attendance: 946±73 mm, T = 2.2, p = 0.03) (Figure 2).

Male attendance and mating behavior, however, was not strongly associated with male parentage. Of the 30 litters in our sample, 23 (76.7%) were from females that were being tracked by radio-telemetry prior to parturition. In 15 of these 23 litters (65.2%), females were found paired with one or more males in one or both mating seasons that preceded parturition. Of the 24 male-female pairings, females were paired with one male in 8 cases, two separate males in four cases, and three separate males in two cases. Of these 24 male-female pairings, 18 of them (75%) were with males that did not have paternity in the litter, and only 5 of them (21%) were with males that were positively identified as fathers (the remaining pairing was with an unmarked male with uncertain paternity, Table 2). Three of the pairings persisted beyond 7 days, and of these three cases, one male was the sole father of the subsequent litter and the other two males had no paternity in the subsequent litters.

Spatial analyses

Our sample of adult male snakes that were radio-tracked for at least one active season (N = 20) included four individuals identified as fathers. These four fathers did not have significantly larger home ranges than the 16 radio-tagged non-fathers (identified fathers: 19.5±16.1 hectares; non-fathers: 15.7±10.9 hectares, T = 0.64, p = 0.68).

We found a total of seven unique male-female pairs that shared parentage in a litter and were both radio-tracked for at least one active season. These seven pairs had an average proportion of home range overlap of 0.30±0.26, which was significantly greater than the home range overlap of random male-female pairs from the population (resampling distribution 95% CI = 0 – 0.23, p = 0.008).

In addition to home range overlap, we compared the distance between the geographic midpoints of the capture locations of male-female pairs that shared parentage. We compared active season midpoints separately from overwintering midpoints. For the active season, we found a total of 28 unique male-female pairs that shared parentage in a litter. These pairs were, on average, 439±331 meters apart, which is significantly closer than the distance between random male-female pairs (resampling distribution 95% CI  =  446 – 661 m, p = 0.03). For the overwintering comparison, we found 24 male-female pairs that shared parentage and had overwintering midpoints. Overwintering sites were, on average, 287±248 meters apart for these pairs, which was also significantly closer than random male-female pairs (resampling distribution 95% CI  =  304 – 549 m, p = 0.016). This tendency of fathers to be in closer spatial proximity to females with which they shared parentage was not an artifact of these fathers being more centrally located at the study site. The average distance between male midpoints and the midpoint of all female midpoints was not significantly different for either active season relocations (fathers 385±195 m, non-fathers 405±157 m, T = 0.36, p = 0.72) or winter relocations (fathers 226±273 m, non-fathers 195±132 m, T = 1.19, p = 0.24).

Female fecundity

The incidence of multiple paternity was not related to female fecundity, as the mean litter size for litters with single fathers (3.8±2.2) was not significantly different from the mean size of multiply sired litters (4.7±1.7, T = 0.9, p = 0.3). Females with multiply sired litters did not have larger home ranges than females with single-sired litters (home range size single sires: 6.3±4.7 ha; multiple sires: 5.8±3.9 ha, T = 0.1, p = 0.90).

Discussion

Sampling, genotyping and parentage analyses

We genotyped all adult and neonate snakes sampled in this study using a large number of polymorphic microsatellite loci markers and reconstructed parentage of the progeny by way of a full-pedigree likelihood approach. Most neonates were found in litters attended by solitary, post-partum females who were unambiguously assigned to be their mothers based on intensive radio-tracking [22], [23]. In female North American viperids, communal birthing is common [73], [74], but it is absent in C. atrox from our study site [22], [23]. Even when we exclude maternal information in the initial parentage analysis, COLONY 2 identified the attending female as the genetic mother of the sampled litter, indicating that the full-pedigree likelihood method is robust with respect to determining parentage in our samples [66]-[69], [75]. This step was important to confirm that adult females found in attendance with newborn litters were the true genetic mothers of those offspring, given recent genetic evidence that communally gestating midget-faded rattlesnakes (Crotalus concolor) have been found in close association with neonates that were not their progeny in so-called nursery aggregations [76].

Although blood was obtained in several instances from neonates, most DNA was derived from shed skins left by dispersing neonates 4-7 days after birth [31], [32]; thus, owing to various levels of probable environmental degradation of these delicate sheds, many of our markers had high levels of allelic dropout. These genotyping errors would almost certainly preclude exclusion methods for paternity assignment. In many studies using noninvasive tissue sampling, error rates are calculated by comparing replicate genotypes obtained from multiple PCRs using the same DNA sample (i.e., the “multi-tube” approach [77]. However, none of the replicate genotypes are guaranteed to be error-free, and so the consensus of a limited number of replicates could still contain errors [67]. For likelihood-based parentage analysis, it is still necessary to estimate an error rate of the consensus genotype. The error rate estimated from comparing replicate runs is the frequency of inconsistent genotypes per replicate, not the error rate of the consensus genotype [69]. Thus, even if replicate genotypes were constructed, some non-PCR based method of estimating genotyping error would still be desirable. If samples contain known parent-offspring dyads, genotyping error rates can be estimated from pedigree analysis using maximum likelihood approaches. We chose to use this approach, given the relatively large number of known mother-offspring pairs in our sample. Using a locus-specific maximum likelihood error estimation approach allowed us to effectively use the full-pedigree likelihood approach to estimate parentage with confidence. This approach has utility for other vertebrate taxa, since noninvasive tissue samples can often be reliably collected from groups of known sibships (i.e., tissues from shed skins, hatched eggshells, or litters attended by mothers, [78]).

Our analysis identified 18 males in our sample as fathers of 64 neonates; unmarked ( =  unknown) males sired the remaining 44 neonates. Twenty-three of the litters we genotyped were from females being actively radio-tracked in the year prior to parturition. Even though we were able to observe these females courting or mating on several occasions, only 25% of the males that paired with these females proved to father offspring in subsequent litters. Despite the fact that we were unable to assign a known (marked) father to a large proportion of neonates, the use of highly informative microsatellite markers was invaluable for estimating reproductive success. In snakes, owing to a range of possible mechanisms (e.g., polyandry, long-term sperm storage, cryptic female choice, sperm competition), strict behavioral observations are poor indicators of paternity [9], [20], [79]. In the present study, although molecular results bore out our behavioral observations of female reproduction (i.e., mother-progeny associations), estimates of paternal contributions would have been highly inaccurate had we relied solely on behavioral observations. Thus, recent triumphs in the study of reproductive success and mating systems in secretive species, such as snakes and other vertebrates, have been made possible over the past several decades by the revolutionary advancements in parentage analysis via microsatellite genotyping [6], [9], [11], [12], [14], [21].

Multiple paternity

Our behavioral observations and molecular results indicate that both male and female C. atrox copulate with multiple partners in one or both of the mating seasons, and that multiple paternity in litters consist of 2 to 3 fathers (reviewed in [79]). However, successful long-term sperm storage (LTSS) by females, which is documented in C. atrox, confounds our ability to precisely pinpoint the temporal estimates of copulations [35], [80]-[82]. In other words, although we are confident of the methods used to assign fathers to particular litters, copulations might have occurred outside of the years we sampled, perhaps even by snakes that had mated and died [83]. Schuett et al. [55] demonstrated successful LTSS in a field-collected C. atrox in autumn 1999, and held in laboratory isolation from all other snakes. That subject produced a healthy litter of 3 males and 3 females on 28 August 2000. In summer 2002, this female produced another healthy litter (4 males, 5 females) despite the fact that she remained in strict isolation (G. W. Schuett unpubl. data). In the second litter, spontaneous facultative parthenogenesis was ruled out owing to certain litter characteristics (see [82]).

Although the mean litter size was relatively small in the present study with 4.3 neonates per litter (see [39]), half that were sampled were multiply sired, with 21% having three fathers. This fits the broad pattern of high levels of multiple paternity found in other snakes [79] and other squamate reptiles [84], including two other viperid species where researchers have tested for multiple paternity of wild litters using molecular techniques [11], [84], [85].

Paternity was unequally distributed across our sample of adult males. Most males we sampled (77%) were not detected as fathers in our samples, whereas a few individuals (e.g., CA-50) had paternity in several litters (Table 2). Highly skewed distributions of paternity are typical in species with polygynandrous mating systems, which is believed to lead to sexual size dimorphism (SSD) [86]. However, we found no difference in the mean size of fathers and non-fathers in our sample, despite that fact that this species exhibits male biased SSD and male-male combat. This result may be due in part to the fact that our methods do not allow us to identify with certainty males that did not mate; certainly, non-fathers could have sired progeny in litters that were not in our samples.

Male body size and reproductive success

Male-biased SSD is documented in C. atrox [28], [30], [87], [88], though it is variable in degree among populations in Arizona [32]. Nonetheless, whether SSD in C. atrox is strictly an environmental outcome, influenced by sexual selection, or both, remains to be tested in future investigations. Although our analysis was unable to show that fathers were larger than non-fathers, which may be due to sample size (i.e., individuals could have paternity in unsampled litters), we found limited evidence for size-specific male mating strategies. Males that were the sole fathers of litters were larger (SVL) than males that shared paternity with other males. Field observations also indicated that males found attending females in the mating season were larger than males that we did not observe attending females. Furthermore, males that were found to attend females for prolonged periods (longer than one week) were larger still than males found attending females once or briefly. These patterns indicate that larger males are more likely to actively guard females, thus restricting access by other males. Although pre-copulatory mate-guarding behavior appears to lead to larger males being less likely to share paternity in a given litter, it should be noted that we found no overall relationship between the number of offspring fathered and male body size (reviewed in [79]).

Our results on male body size (SVL) are similar to previous studies that have investigated paternity in snakes with male-male combat. Blouin-Demers et al. [12] found that larger male black rat snakes sired more offspring than smaller males, and also sired a higher proportion of offspring per clutch. Ursenbacher et al. [11] found that larger male adders sired more offspring, and that single father litters were sired by larger males than multi-father litters. In a study that did not use molecular parentage methods, Madsen et al. [89] found that larger male adders dominated smaller ones in male combat bouts and tended to mate-guard females, but smaller males mated with females in the absence of other males. Dubey et al. [14] found that larger males of slatey-gray snakes (Stegonotus cucullatus, Colubridae) sired a greater proportion of offspring within a clutch than smaller males. In contrast, Weatherhead et al. [13] found that body size was not clearly related to reproductive success in northern water snakes, a species which lacks male-male combat. Also, Duvall & Schuett [52] showed that body size in male prairie rattlesnake (Crotalus viridis) was not related to mating success.

In snakes with male-male combat, body size is an important determinant of reproductive success, as would be expected given that male size is the primary determinant of winning male combats [47]. However, in all of the above examples, smaller males were also able to father some offspring: male adders as small as 37 cm, black ratsnakes as small as 81 cm, slatey-gray snakes as small as 85 cm, and western diamond-backed rattlesnakes as small as 70 cm SVL were all fathers. For adders, black ratsnakes, and western diamondback rattlesnakes, these sizes are on the lower end of the estimated minimum male size at sexual maturity [88], [90], [91]. Thus, even snakes with male-biased SSD and male-male combat may also exhibit alternative male mating tactics, whereby small males attempt to “sneak” copulations with females without engaging in combat with other males. Although data on alternative male mating tactics in snakes are limited, studies of adders [89] and garter snakes [92] indicate that small males may successfully employ different mating tactics than large males. Our data on paternity in C. atrox indicate that alternative tactics by smaller males may be effective, as there was no overall relationship between body size and reproductive success.

Spatial ecology of males and reproductive success

Our spatial analyses indicated that proximity of the male-female pairs was a significant factor associated with paternity in a given litter. This relationship was detected for both the active (March-October) and overwintering (November-February) seasons. On average, during the active season, fathers were captured within 450 meters of females that shared parentage with them. This distance is not unexpected, given that the average male home range in our population was 16 hectare (i.e., a 16 hectare circle would have a diameter of about 450 m). In support of this finding, we found a significant degree of home range overlap for those male-female pairs sharing parentage and were radio-tracked. Consequently, several different spatial analyses we performed indicated that males typically mate only with females that are likely to overlap some part of their home range [108], a finding that reinforces the general principle of spatial distribution of receptive females as a primary factor shaping mating systems [48]. Accordingly, we predicted that males with larger home ranges would also potentially mate with more females and sire more offspring. Although males who fathered offspring did not have significantly larger home ranges than males with no detected paternity, our sample size for this test was low, as only four genetic fathers had radio-transmitters. Given our data on the importance of spatial overlap for paternity, future analyses may reveal that males with larger home range sizes have greater relative reproductive success [109].

Snake mating systems and the evolution of polyandry

Over two decades ago, the first modeling attempts identifying, characterizing and quantifying the mating systems of snakes within formal selection theory described most species as being polygynous based on the available empirical evidence [40], [41], [49], [80]. Since that time a wealth of new information on populations, behavior, reproduction, and parental care of snakes has emerged [53], [82], [93], [94]. Accordingly, adjustments need to be made to accommodate these discoveries and shifts in perspectives. Based on behavioral and genetic information we have for C. atrox at the SMs, we suggest that adults assemble as itinerant pairs during the breeding seasons, with a mating system characterized as attendant polygynandry (see [93], pp 274-280 in [95]). This breeding system has the following characteristics: (1) males seek females, which are unevenly distributed and not clumped (see [41], [96]); (2) the OSR is male-biased (roughly 2:1) in any given year; (3) bisexual pairs last for various lengths (e.g., days to weeks) but are not permanent; (4) male defense of females (male combat) may occur; and (5) both sexes can mate multiple times with multiple partners per breeding season.

Our empirical and theoretical understanding of multiple mating in females, sometimes termed polygamy, has undergone a revolutionary paradigm shift, beginning with Parker’s [97] highly influential paper on sperm competition in 1970. This work has spawned thousands of studies investigating male and female mating frequency, sperm competition, cryptic female choice, and multiple paternity in plants and animals [98], and has led to a great expansion of our knowledge and understanding of mating systems and sexual selection [49], [95], [99]-[101]. It appears straightforward why some males mate with as many females as possible, based on the vast majority studies and theoretical models examining fitness benefits [95], [102]. This has been termed the “Darwin-Bateman paradigm” [103], which asserts that the reproductive success of males increases steeply (Bateman gradient) as the number of copulations with different females increases [100]. Nonetheless, several important topics remain problematic, and perhaps the most persistent one concerns the adaptive significance of polyandry, defined as multiple matings with different males by females [95], [101]. Why should females mate with multiple partners for fertilization of a single clutch or litter, especially in cases where female fecundity does not seem to increase with multiple mating, as we found for C. atrox in this study?

Various hypotheses addressing the adaptive significance (direct and indirect benefits) of the evolution for and maintenance of polyandy have been proposed and tested. Recent interest has been explosive [104], [105], and this renaissance sets the stage for reexamining earlier research and executing new empirical and modeling studies that expand beyond sexual selection and sexual conflict. Adaptive conclusions regarding polyandry in animals are complex and inconclusive, with some studies revealing benefits and others reporting none [106], [107]. The study of polyandry, sperm competition, long-term sperm storage, and cryptic female choice is in its infancy with regard to studies of snakes [79], [84], [93]. For most species, it is difficult to assess whether multiple matings in wild females have indirect benefits, such as fertilization insurance, genetic compatibility, sperm competition, or increased genetic diversity (reviewed in [104]). Instead, long-term field studies will have to be combined with experimental manipulations to address these hypotheses empirically.

Conclusions

Our use of genetic techniques to analyze parentage has allowed us to substantially increase our knowledge of the mating system of a population of western diamond-backed rattlesnakes (C. atrox) in the Sonoran Desert of Arizona. This species is similar to many other snake species in that they exhibit a “polygynandrous” mating system, with both sexes mating with multiple partners during the mating season or seasons. Larger males appear to use their larger body size to prevent other males from mating with females that they are guarding or have mated, but smaller males are able to successfully father offspring with females. Although molecular tools have now been used in a handful of snake species to assess parentage and multiple paternity [84], [85], [110], [111], their use in combination with long-term field studies remains quite limited. In C. atrox and other snakes, field observations of mating alone were insufficient to characterize the genetic mating systems, both because mating behavior is difficult to observe directly, and because the act of mating does not guarantee reproductive success in males (see [10]). Accordingly, we urge field researchers to adopt molecular analyses as a standard tool in long-term, individual-based studies of the ecology and behavior of snakes [1], and think that doing so will greatly improve and transform our understanding of mating systems, behavioral ecology and sexual selection of this large, diverse and important group of vertebrates.

Acknowledgments

Numerous individuals provided assistance in radio-tracking, but most noteworthy is Ryan Sawby. Over the years we greatly benefitted by discussions with S. Beaupre; D. Beck, M. Cardwell, D. DeNardo, M. Feldner, H. Greene, R. Hansen, D. Hardy Sr, S. Hoss, E. Nowak, R. Reiserer, J. Slone, and E. Taylor. Dale DeNardo performed many of the surgeries, for which we are most thankful. The insights of Thomas Madsen, A. Bentley, and an anonymous reviewer improved the paper, though any blunders remain our responsibility.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: GWS RAR. Performed the experiments: GWS RAR. Analyzed the data: RWC GWS CFS HWH MA. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: RWC GWS CFS HWH MA. Wrote the paper: RWC GWS RAR MA CFS HWH.

References

  1. 1. Clutton-Brock TH, Sheldon BC (2010) Individuals and populations: the role of long-term, individual-based studies of animals in ecology and evolutionary biology. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 25: 562–573
  2. 2. Clutton-Brock TH (1988) Reproductive Success: Studies of Individual Variation in Contrasting Breeding Systems. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  3. 3. Greene HW (2005) Organisms in nature as a central focus for biology. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 20: 23–27
  4. 4. Bonnet X, Shine R, Lourdais O (2002) Taxonomic chauvinism. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 17: 1–3.
  5. 5. De Woody JA (2005) Molecular approaches to the study of parentage, relatedness, and fitness: practical applications for wild animals. Journal of Wildlife Management 69: 1400–1418.
  6. 6. DeYoung RW, Honeycutt RL (2005) The molecular toolbox: genetic techniques in wildlife ecology and management. Journal of Wildlife Management 69: 1362–1384.
  7. 7. Griffith SC, Owens IP, Thuman KA (2002) Extra pair paternity in birds: a review of interspecific variation and adaptive function. Molecular Ecology 11: 2195–2212.
  8. 8. Schmoll T (2011) A review and perspective on context-dependent genetic effects of extra-pair mating in birds. Journal of Ornithology 152: 265–277
  9. 9. Gibbs HL, Weatherhead PJ (2001) Insights into population ecology and sexual selection in snakes through the application of DNA-based genetic markers. Journal of Heredity 92: 173–179.
  10. 10. Prosser MR, Weatherhead PJ, Gibbs HL, Brown GP (2002) Genetic analysis of the mating system and opportunity for sexual selection in northern water snakes (Nerodia sipedon). Behavioral Ecology 13: 800–807.
  11. 11. Ursenbacher S, Erny C, Fumagalli L (2009) Male reproductive success and multiple paternity in wild, low-density populations of the adder (Vipera berus). Journal of Heredity 100: 365–370
  12. 12. Blouin-Demers G, Gibbs HL, Weatherhead PJ (2005) Genetic evidence for sexual selection in black ratsnakes, Elaphe obsoleta. Animal Behaviour 69: 225–234
  13. 13. Weatherhead PJ, Prosser MR, Gibbs HL, Brown GP (2002) Male reproductive success and sexual selection in northern water snakes determined by microsatellite DNA analysis. Behavioral Ecology 13: 808–815.
  14. 14. Dubey S, Brown GP, Madsen T, Shine R (2009) Sexual selection favours large body size in males of a tropical snake (Stegonotus cucullatus, Colubridae). Animal Behaviour 77: 177–182
  15. 15. Nowak EM, Theimer TC, Schuett GW (2008) Functional and numerical responses of predators: where do vipers fit in the traditional paradigms? Biological Reviews 83: 601–620
  16. 16. Clark RW, Tangco S, Barbour MA (2012) Field video recordings reveal factors influencing predatory strike success of free-ranging rattlesnakes (Crotalus spp.). Animal Behaviour 84: 183–190
  17. 17. Clark RW (2006) Fixed videography to study predation behavior of an ambush foraging snake, Crotalus horridus. Copeia 2006: 181–187.
  18. 18. Reinert HK (1992) Radiotelemetric studies of pitvipers: data acquisition and analysis. In: Campbell JA, Brodie ED Jr., editors. Biology of the Pitvipers. Tyler, TX: Selva. pp. 185–197.
  19. 19. Dorcas ME, Willson JD (2009) Innovative methods for studies of snake ecology and conservation. In: Mullin SJ, Seigel RA, editors. Snakes: Ecology and Conservation. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. pp. 5-37.
  20. 20. Blouin MS (2003) DNA-based methods for pedigree reconstruction and kinship analysis in natural populations. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 18: 503–511
  21. 21. King RB (2009) Population and conservation genetics. In: Mullin SJ, Seigel RA, editors. Snakes: Ecology and Conservation. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. pp. 78–122.
  22. 22. Schuett GW, Repp RA, Hoss SK (2011) Frequency of reproduction in female western diamond-backed rattlesnakes from the Sonoran Desert of Arizona is variable in individuals: potential role of rainfall and prey densities. Journal of Zoology 284: 105–113
  23. 23. Schuett GW, Repp RA, Amarello M, Smith CF (2013) Unlike most vipers, female rattlesnakes (Crotalus atrox) continue to hunt and feed throughout pregnancy. Journal of Zoology 289: 101–110
  24. 24. Brown DE (1994) Biotic Communities of the American Southwest: United States and Mexico. Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press.
  25. 25. Epple AE, Epple LE (1995) Plants of Arizona. Helena MT: Falcon Publishing.
  26. 26. Mock CJ (1996) Climatic controls and spatial variations of precipitation in the western United States. Journal of Climate 9: 1111–1125
  27. 27. Phillips SJ, Comus PW (2000) A Natural History of the Sonoran Desert. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
  28. 28. Klauber LM (1972) Rattlesnakes: Their Habits, Life Histories, and Influence on Mankind. 2nd ed. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
  29. 29. Campbell JA, Lamar WW (2004) The Venomous Reptiles of the Western Hemisphere, 2 volumes. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
  30. 30. Spencer CL (2008) Geographic variation in western diamond-backed rattlesnake (Crotalus atrox) morphology. In: Hayes WK, Beaman KR, Cardwell MD, Bush SP, editors. The Biology of Rattlesnakes. Loma Linda, CA: Loma Linda Press. pp. 55-78
  31. 31. Castoe TA, Spencer CL, Parkinson CL (2007) Phylogeographic structure and historical demography of the western diamondback rattlesnake (Crotalus atrox): A perspective on North American desert biogeography. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 42: 193–212.
  32. 32. Amarello M, Nowak EM, Taylor EN, Schuett GW, Repp RA, et al. (2010) Potential environmental influences on variation in body size and sexual size dimorphism among Arizona populations of the western diamond-backed rattlesnake (Crotalus atrox). Journal of Arid Environments 74: 1443–1449
  33. 33. Repp RA, Schuett GW (2009) Natural history note. Crotalus atrox (western diamond-backed rattlesnakes). Adult predation on lizards. Herpetological Review 40: 353–354.
  34. 34. Rosen PC, Goldberg SR (2002) Female reproduction in the western diamond-backed rattlesnake, Crotalus atrox (Serpentes: Viperidae), from Arizona. The Texas Journal of Science 54: 347–356.
  35. 35. Aldridge RD, Duvall D (2002) Evolution of the mating season in the pitvipers of North America. Herpetological Monographs 16: 1–25
  36. 36. Graham SP, Earley RL, Hoss SK, Schuett GW, Grober MS (2008) The reproductive biology of male cottonmouths (Agkistrodon piscivorus): do plasma steroid hormones predict the mating season? General and Comparative Endocrinology 159: 226–235.
  37. 37. Taylor EN, DeNardo DF (2010) Hormones and reproductive cycles in snakes. In: Norris DO, Lopez HK, editors. Hormones and Reproduction of Vertebrates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Vol. 3. pp. 355–372.
  38. 38. Goldberg SR (2007) Testicular cycle of the western diamond-backed rattlesnake, Crotalus atrox (Serpentes: Viperidae), from Arizona. Bulletin of the Maryland Herpetological Society 43: 103–107.
  39. 39. Taylor EN, DeNardo DF (2005) Sexual size dimorphism and growth plasticity in snakes: an experiment on the western diamond-backed rattlesnake (Crotalus atrox). Journal of Experimental Zoology 303A: 598–607
  40. 40. Duvall D, Schuett GW, Arnold SJ (1993) Ecology and evolution of snake mating systems. In: Collins JT, Seigel RA, editors. Snakes: Ecology and Behavior. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. pp. 165–200.
  41. 41. Duvall D, Arnold SJ, Schuett GW (1992) Pitviper mating systems: ecological potential, sexual selection, and microevolution. In: Brodie ED Jr, Campbell JA, editors. Biology of the Pitvipers. Tyler, TX: Selva. pp. 321–336.
  42. 42. Kokko H, Wong BBM (2007) What determines sex roles in mate searching? Evolution 61: 1162–1175
  43. 43. Gillingham JC, Carpenter CC, Murphy JB (1983) Courtship, male combat and dominance in the western diamondback rattlesnake, Crotalus atrox. Journal of Herpetology: 265–270.
  44. 44. Duvall D, Schuett GW (1997) Straight-line movement and competitive mate searching in prairie rattlesnakes, Crotalus viridis viridis. Animal Behaviour 54: 329–334.
  45. 45. Shine R (1994) Sexual size dimorphism in snakes revisited. Copeia 1994: 326–346.
  46. 46. Schuett GW, Gergus EW, Kraus F (2001) Phylogenetic correlation between male–male fighting and mode of prey subjugation in snakes. Acta Ethologica 4: 31–49
  47. 47. Schuett GW (1997) Body size and agonistic experience affect dominance and mating success in male copperheads. Animal Behaviour 54: 213–224.
  48. 48. Emlen ST, Oring LW (1977) Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science 197: 215–223.
  49. 49. Arnold SJ, Duvall D (1994) Animal mating systems: a synthesis based on selection theory. American Naturalist: 317–348.
  50. 50. Madsen T, Shine R (1993) Temporal variability in sexual selection acting on reproductive tactics and body size in male snakes. American Naturalist 141: 167–171.
  51. 51. Repp RA, Schuett GW (2008) Western diamond-backed rattlesnakes, Crotalus atrox (Serpentes: Viperidae), gain water by harvesting and drinking rain, sleet, and snow. The Southwestern Naturalist 53: 108–114
  52. 52. Price AH (1988) Observations on maternal behavior and neonate aggregation in the western diamondback rattlesnake, Crotalus atrox (Crotalidae). The Southwestern Naturalist 33: 370–373.
  53. 53. Greene HW, May PG, Hardy DL Sr, Sciturro JM, Farrell TM (2002) Parental behavior by vipers. In: Schuett GW, Hoggren M, Douglas ME, editors. Biology of the Vipers. Eagle Mountain, UT: Eagle Mountain Publishing. pp. 179–206.
  54. 54. Reiserer RS, Schuett GW, Earley RL (2008) Dynamic aggregations of newborn sibling rattlesnakes exhibit stable thermoregulatory properties. Journal of Zoology 274: 277–283
  55. 55. Schuett GW, Grober MS, Van Kirk EA, Murdoch WJ (2004) Long-term sperm storage and plasma steroid profile of pregnancy in a western diamond-backed rattlesnake (Crotalus atrox). Herpetological Review 35: 328–332.
  56. 56. Reinert H, Cundall D (1982) An improved surgical implantation method for radio-tracking snakes. Copeia 1982: 702–705.
  57. 57. Pozarowski K, Bryan DS, Bosse R, Watson E, Herrmann H-W (2012) Development of polymorphic microsatellite loci for the rattlesnake species Crotalus atrox, C. cerastes, and C. scutulatus (Viperidae: Crotalinae) and cross-species amplification of microsatellite markers in Crotalus and Sistrurus species. Conservation Genetics Resources 4: 955–961
  58. 58. Goldberg CS, Edwards T, Kaplan ME, Goode M (2003) PCR primers for microsatellite loci in the tiger rattlesnake (Crotalus tigris, Viperidae). Molecular Ecology Notes 3: 539–541
  59. 59. Munguia-Vega A, Pelz-Serrano K, Goode M, Culver M (2009) Eleven new microsatellite loci for the tiger rattlesnake (Crotalus tigris). Molecular Ecology Resources 9: 1267–1270.
  60. 60. Holycross AT, Douglas ME, Higbee JR, Bogden RH (2002) Isolation and characterization of microsatellite loci from a threatened rattlesnake (New Mexico ridge-nosed rattlesnake, Crotalus willardi obscurus). Molecular Ecology Notes 2: 537–539
  61. 61. Oyler-McCance SJ, St John J, Parker JM, Anderson SH (2005) Characterization of microsatellite loci isolated in midget faded rattlesnake (Crotalus viridis concolor). Mol Ecol Notes 5: 452–453
  62. 62. Gibbs HL, Prior K, Parent C (1998) Characterization of DNA microsatellite loci from a threatened snake: the eastern massasauga rattlesnake (Sistrurus c. catenatus) and their use in population studies. Journal of Heredity 89: 169–173.
  63. 63. Schuelke M (2000) An economic method for the fluorescent labeling of PCR fragments. Nature Biotechnology 18: 233–234.
  64. 64. Brownstein MJ, Carpten JD, Smith JR (1996) Modulation of non-templated nucleotide addition by Taq DNA polymerase: primer modifications that facilitate genotyping. Biotechniques 20: 1004.
  65. 65. Row JR, Blouin-Demers G (2006) Kernels are not accurate estimators of home-range size for herpetofauna. Copeia 2006: 797–802.
  66. 66. Jones OR, Wang J (2010) COLONY: a program for parentage and sibship inference from multilocus genotype data. Molecular Ecology Resources 10: 551–555
  67. 67. Wang J (2004) Sibship reconstruction from genetic data with typing errors. Genetics 166: 1963–1979.
  68. 68. Harrison HB, Saenz-Agudelo P, Planes S, Jones GP, Berumen ML (2012) Relative accuracy of three common methods of parentage analysis in natural populations. Molecular Ecology 22: 1158–1170
  69. 69. Wang J (2010) Effects of genotyping errors on parentage exclusion analysis. Molecular Ecology 19: 5061–5078.
  70. 70. Blank S, Seiter C, Bruce P (2001) Resampling Stats. Arlington, VA: Resampling Stats Inc.
  71. 71. Zar JA (1996) Biostatistical Analysis. 3rd ed. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
  72. 72. DeNardo DF, Taylor EN (2011) Hormones and reproduction in free-ranging snakes. In: Sever DM, Aldridge RD, editors. Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Snakes. St. Louis MO: Science Publishers. pp. 265–287.
  73. 73. Graves BM, Duvall D (1995) Aggregation of squamate reptiles associated with gestation, oviposition, and parturition. Herpetological Monographs 9: 102–119.
  74. 74. Graves BM, Duvall D (1993) Reproduction, rookery use, and thermoregulation in free-ranging, pregnant Crotalus v. viridis. Journal of Herpetology 27: 33–41.
  75. 75. Walling CA, Pemberton JM, Hadfield JD, Kruuk LEB (2010) Comparing parentage inference software: reanalysis of a red deer pedigree. Molecular Ecology 19: 1914–1928
  76. 76. Parker JM, Spear SF, Oyler-McCance S (2013) Crotalus oreganus concolor nursery aggregation. Herpetological Review 43: 658–659.
  77. 77. Pompanon F, Bonin A, Bellemain E, Taberlet P (2005) Genotyping errors: causes, consequences and solutions. Nature Reviews Genetics 6: 847–846
  78. 78. Trimbos KB, Broekman J, Kentie R, Musters CJM, Snoo GR (2009) Using eggshell membranes as a DNA source for population genetic research. Journal of Ornithology 150: 915–920
  79. 79. Jellen BC, Aldridge RD (2011) Paternity patterns. In: Aldridge RD, Sever DM, editors. Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Snakes. Enfield, NH: Science Publishers. pp. 619-644.
  80. 80. Schuett GW (1992) Is long-term sperm storage an important component of the reproductive biology of temperate pitvipers? In: Campbell JA, Brodie ED Jr, editors. Biology of the Pitvipers. Tyler, TX: Selva. pp. 169–184.
  81. 81. Sever DM, Hamlett WC (2001) Female sperm storage in reptiles. Journal of Experimental Zoology 292: 187–199
  82. 82. Booth W, Schuett GW (2011) Molecular genetic evidence for alternative reproductive strategies in North American pitvipers (Serpentes: Viperidae): long-term sperm storage and facultative parthenogenesis. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 104: 934–942
  83. 83. Zamudio KR, Sinervo B (2000) Polygyny, mate-guarding, and posthumous fertilization as alternative male mating strategies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97: 14427–14432
  84. 84. Uller T, Olsson M (2008) Multiple paternity in reptiles: patterns and processes. Molecular Ecology 17: 2566–2580
  85. 85. Simonov E, Wink M (2011) Cross-amplification of microsatellite loci reveals multiple paternity in Halys pit viper (Gloydius halys). Acta Herpetologica 6: 289–295.
  86. 86. Andersson M (1994) Sexual Selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  87. 87. Fitch HS (1981) Sexual size differences in reptiles. University of Kansas Publications of the Museum of Natural History Miscellaneous Publications 70: 1–72.
  88. 88. Beaupre SJ, Duvall D, O'Leile J (1998) Ontogenetic variation in growth and sexual size dimorphism in a central Arizona population of the western diamondback rattlesnake (Crotalus atrox). Copeia 1998: 40–47.
  89. 89. Madsen T, Shine R, Loman J, Hakansson T (1993) Determinants of mating success in male adders, Vipera berus. Animal Behaviour 45: 491–499.
  90. 90. Blouin-Demers G, Prior KA, Weatherhead PJ (2006) Comparative demography of black rat snakes (Elaphe obsoleta) in Ontario and Maryland. Journal of Zoology 256: 1–10
  91. 91. Prestt I (1971) An ecological study of the viper Vipera berus in southern Britain. Journal of Zoology 164: 373–418.
  92. 92. Shine R, Langkilde T, Wall M, Mason RT (2005) Alternative male mating tactics in garter snakes, Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis. Animal Behaviour 70: 387–396
  93. 93. Rivas JA, Burghardt GM (2005) Snake mating systems, behavior, and evolution: the revisionary implications of recent findings. Journal of Comparative Psychology 119: 447–454
  94. 94. Booth W, Smith CF, Eskridge PH, Hoss SK, Mendelson JR, et al. (2012) Facultative parthenogenesis discovered in wild vertebrates. Biology Letters 8: 983–985
  95. 95. Shuster SM, Wade MJ (2003) Mating Systems and Strategies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  96. 96. Ims RA (1988) The potential for sexual selection in males: effect of sex ratio and spatiotemporal distribution of receptive females. Evolutionary Ecology 2: 338–352.
  97. 97. Parker GA (1970) Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Biological Reviews 45: 525–567
  98. 98. Birkhead TR, Moller AP, editors (1998) Sperm Competition and Sexual Selection. Cambridge, UK: Academic Press.
  99. 99. Kvarnemo C, Simmons LW (2013) Polyandry as a mediator of sexual selection before and after mating. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 368: 20120042
  100. 100. Parker GA, Birkhead TR (2013) Polyandry: the history of a revolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 368: 20120335
  101. 101. Shuster SM, Briggs WR, Dennis PA (2013) How multiple mating by females affects sexual selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 368: 20120046
  102. 102. Bateman AJ (1948) Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. Heredity 2: 349–368.
  103. 103. Dewsbury DA (2005) The Darwin-Bateman paradigm in historical context. Integrative and Comparative Biology 45: 831–837
  104. 104. Avise JC, Tatarenkov A, Liu JX (2011) Multiple mating and clutch size in invertebrate brooders versus pregnant vertebrates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108: 11512–11517
  105. 105. Pizzari T, Wedell N (2013) The polyandry revolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 368: 20120041–20120041
  106. 106. Wright LI, Fuller WJ, Godley BJ, McGowan A, Tregenza T, et al. (2013) No benefits of polyandry to female green turtles. Behavioral Ecology 24: 1022–1029
  107. 107. DiBattista JD, Feldheim KA, Gruber SH, Hendry AP (2008) Are indirect genetic benefits associated with polyandry? Testing predictions in a natural population of lemon sharks. Molecular Ecology 17: 783–795
  108. 108. Brown GP, Weatherhead PJ (1999) Female distribution affects mate searching and sexual selection in male northern water snakes (Nerodia sipedon). Behavioural Ecology Sociobiology 47: 9–16.
  109. 109. Fisher DO, Lara MC (1999) Effects of body size and home range on access to mates and paternity in male bridled nailtail wallabies. Animal Behaviour 58: 121–130.
  110. 110. Wusterbarth TL, King RB, Duvall MR, Grayburn WS, Burghardt GM (2010) Phylogenetically widespread multiple paternity in New World natricine snakes. Herpetological Conservation and Biology 5: 86–93.
  111. 111. Lukoschek V, Avise JC (2011) Genetic monandry in six viviparous species of true sea snakes. Journal of Heredity 102: 347–351