Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Paleoindian Unifacial Stone Tool ‘Spurs’: Intended Accessories or Incidental Accidents?

  • Metin I. Eren ,

    metin.i.eren@gmail.com

    Affiliations Department of Anthropology, Marlowe Building, University of Kent, Canterbury, United Kingdom, Department of Archaeology, Cleveland Museum of Natural History, Cleveland, Ohio, United States of America

  • Thomas A. Jennings,

    Affiliation Department of Anthropology, University of West Georgia, Carrollton, Georgia, United States of America

  • Ashley M. Smallwood

    Affiliation Department of Anthropology, University of West Georgia, Carrollton, Georgia, United States of America

Abstract

Paleoindian unifacial stone tools frequently exhibit distinct, sharp projections, known as “spurs”. During the last two decades, a theoretically and empirically informed interpretation–based on individual artifact analysis, use-wear, tool-production techniques, and studies of resharpening–suggested that spurs were sometimes created intentionally via retouch, and other times created incidentally via resharpening or knapping accidents. However, more recently Weedman strongly criticized the inference that Paleoindian spurs were ever intentionally produced or served a functional purpose, and asserted that ethnographic research “demonstrates that the presence of so called ‘graver’ spurs does not have a functional significance.” While ethnographic data cannot serve as a direct test of the archaeological record, we used Weedman’s ethnographic observations to create two quantitative predictions of the Paleoindian archaeological record in order to directly examine the hypothesis that Paleoindian spurs were predominantly accidents occurring incidentally via resharpening and reshaping. The first prediction is that the frequency of spurs should increase as tool reduction proceeds. The second prediction is that the frequency of spurs should increase as tool breakage increases. An examination of 563 unbroken tools and 629 tool fragments from the Clovis archaeological record of the North American Lower Great Lakes region showed that neither prediction was consistent with the notion that spurs were predominately accidents. Instead, our results support the prevailing viewpoint that spurs were sometimes created intentionally via retouch, and other times, created incidentally via resharpening or knapping accidents. Behaviorally, this result is consistent with the notion that unifacial stone tools were multifunctional implements that enhanced the mobile lifestyle of Pleistocene hunter-gatherers.

Introduction

There is little doubt that throughout most the 20th century research on North American Paleoindian flaked stone tools has traditionally centered on bifaces, especially fluted projectile points ([1]; [2]∶142). There were, and still are, very good reasons for this focus. Questions involving mobility, raw material selection, core reduction, settlement patterns, and gearing-up can be profitably examined (e.g., [3]; [4]; [5]; [6]; [7]; [8]; [9]). Fluted point and biface flaking, resharpening, and shape patterns can reveal stylistic signatures, evolutionary trends, adaptation to environments, and evidence of cultural transmission (e.g., [10]; [11]; [12]; [13]; [14]; [15]). Furthermore, the study of bifaces and fluted points is one avenue for understanding aspects of Paleoindian tool-making and tool-using efficiency and function (e.g., [16]; [17]; [18]; [19]; [20]; [21]; [22]; [23]; [24]; [25]; [26]; [27]; [28]) and, perhaps, even less utilitarian aspects of Paleoindian life (e.g., [29]; [30]).

In more recent years, however, Paleoindian archaeologists have come to realize that while the study of bifacial implements is vital for answering numerous questions, constructing more holistic models of Paleoindian behavior requires the consideration of other stone tool classes ([1]; [2]). Thus, while there were early exceptions to the priority given to the study of projectile points (e.g., [31]; [32]; [33]), over the last 30 years there has been an increase of research focusing more heavily on other Paleoindian stone tool classes, especially unifacially flaked stone tools (e.g., [2]; [34]; [35]; [36]; [37]; [38]; [39]; [40]; [41]; [42]; [43]; [44]; [45]; [46]; [47]; [48]; [49]; [50]; [51]; [52]; [53]; [54]; [55]; [56]; [57]; [58]).

The tools of mobile foragers often possess properties that were dictated by, as well as helped enhance, a mobile lifestyle. Mobile tools offered the properties of portability, durability, maintainability, reliability, efficiency (effectiveness), and multi-functionality (versatility and flexibility) ([2]; [59]; [60]; [61]; [62]; [63]; [64]; [65]; [66]; [67]; [68]; [69]; [70]; [71]). Since unifacial stone tools were important, and often major, components of the Paleoindian mobile toolkit ([41]; [46]), and since Paleoindians were highly mobile ([72]; [73]), archaeologists have inferred from the archaeological record that unifacial tools were often designed for portability ([3]) and maintainability ([41]). However, one aspect Paleoindian unifacial tool technology remains unresolved. With regard to the property of multi-functionality of these tools, one longstanding question has been chronically debated. Were distinct, sharp projections–known as “spurs”–on Paleoindian unifacial stone tools functional “accessories” intentionally shaped by prehistoric flintknappers for the purpose of facilitating tasks such as “tattooing, piercing, ripping or tearing hide, and engraving bone, antler, wood, and ivory” ([74]∶731), or were spurs non-functional phenomena, the incidental formation of which is best explained by tool resharpening or knapping accidents?

The traditional answer to this question, assumed by numerous Paleoindian lithic analysts throughout much of the 20th century, was that spurs were intentionally created functional accessories. To many of these early researchers this conclusion was so intuitively obvious that the “spurred [insert unifacial tool type here]” was a commonly listed tool category (e.g., [75]; [76]; [77]). However, during the last two decades, a more theoretically and empirically informed interpretation–based on individual artifact analysis, use-wear, tool-production techniques, and studies of resharpening–suggested that spurs were sometimes created intentionally via retouch, and other times created incidentally via resharpening or knapping accidents ([35]; [36]; [39]; [45]; [52]; [54]; [55]; [56]; [78]).

Weedman ([74]∶731) strongly criticized the inference that Paleoindian spurs were ever intentionally produced or served a functional purpose, asserting that ethnographic research “demonstrates that the presence of so called ‘graver’ spurs does not have a functional significance.” Her stance is based on two sets of ethnographic observations. First, the ethnoarchaeological research of Clark and Kurashina ([79]) and Nissen and Dittemore ([80]) suggested to her that spurs could form incidentally via resharpening unifacial stone tools. Indeed, Weedman’s own ethnographic data from four distinct villages in Ethiopia–whose people did not intentionally create this functional accessory ([74]∶739)–show that the percentage of used-up scrapers exhibiting a spur ranged from 2.9% to 19.0%. While these observations underscore the possibility that some Paleoindian spurs were created incidentally via resharpening, they do not falsify the notion that prehistoric spurs could also have been created intentionally for functional purposes. Nevertheless, Weedman ([74]∶732) advocated that “spurs are the result of reshaping or resharpening.” Nearly 20 years earlier, Grimes et al. ([81]∶165) asserted as much:

“Spurred scrapers seem to represent one extreme of a spectrum of lateral edge modifications which includes constriction, notching, and ventral thinning. These modifications probably represent alternative means of adapting end scrapers to a haft. Spurred end scrapers may be the end product of periodic resharpening and bit attrition of notched specimens. In effect, this process would continually reduce the bit to notch distance, to the point where they ultimately converged, producing the characteristic spur. If this hypothesis is correct, spurred end scrapers may be a ‘diagnostic’ Palaeo-Indian artifact only in the sense that they are the by-product of a strategy for maximizing to longevity, a trait associated with Palaeoindian lithic technology.”

The second set of observations upon which Weedman ([74]) based her stance involved not only the act of resharpening, but the practical experience of the ethnographic knappers who were doing the resharpening. Her ethnographic data showed that “villages with the highest percentage of spurred scrapers have more hideworkers with three or fewer years or more than 30 years of experience knapping” ([74]∶738). Thus, the ethnographic record suggests there exists both a mechanism (i.e., resharpening) for the incidental creation of spurs, as well as a variable (i.e., age/experience) that explains how that mechanism may vary to create the low to high proportions of spurs in different assemblages. Based on this, Weedman concluded “ethnoarchaeological evidence eliminates a functional explanation for spurs” ([74]∶741–742), and hence, “it is quite probable that the presence of spurs on scrapers in archaeological assemblages worldwide contexts represent not formally made tools, but accidents” ([74]∶741).

Distinguishing between accidental and intentional spurs has important implications for understanding Paleoindian technological organization. If Weedman ([74]) is correct and spurs on Paleoindian unifaces are accidents of resharpening, then spur presence is related only to reduction intensity. Spurs can be used as a direct indicator of curation but are themselves not related to other aspects of technological organization. Alternatively, if spurs are intentionally produced for tool use, then spur presence can be used to understand other facets of Paleoindian technological organization including the role of tool multifunctionality.

While ethnographic analogy can provide a range of possible explanations for particular tool morphologies, documenting and understanding a pattern in ethnographic data is not a direct test of archaeological data ([82]; [83]). With regard to the overall incidental or intentional nature of Paleoindian spurs, it cannot be assumed that because ethnographic spurs from one small region in Africa are primarily created incidentally, Paleoindian spurs, or those present in “worldwide contexts,” were therefore also created incidentally. Any conclusion made about Paleoindian behavior can only come from a test of the Paleoindian archaeological record itself. So, while we disagree that Weedman’s ([74]) conclusions can be universally applied, her ethnographic observations are still scientifically testable. By using those ethnographic observations as a foundation for the creation of quantitative predictions of the Paleoindian archaeological record, this is the first paper to directly test the hypothesis that Paleoindian spurs were accidents occurring incidentally via resharpening and reshaping.

We identify two predictions stemming from Weedman’s ([74]) hypothesis. The first prediction is that if Paleoindian spurs were predominantly the incidental result of resharpening as Weedman ([74]) suggests, then we can predict that there will be increasingly higher frequencies of spurs present in sets of unifacial stone tools that are relatively more resharpened than there will be in sets of unifacial stone tools that are relatively less resharpened. This prediction is based on two reasons. First, as Grimes et al. ([81]) notes above, spurs may be the result of resharpening and bit attrition of notched specimens. In effect, the process would continually have reduced the working edge to notch distance, to the point where they ultimately converged, producing a spur. Second, as a unifacial stone tool is resharpened and becomes progressively smaller, rounder, and thicker, more force is required for retouch. The combination of increased retouch force and increasingly undesirable tool shape fosters a situation where mistakes (like spurs) are not only more likely to happen, but one in which those mistakes are harder to rectify (see [46]∶328). In aggregate, these two reasons allow us to reasonably predict, on the population level, that tools resharpened to a greater degree would be more likely to possess spurs when discarded than those tools that are relatively less resharpened.

Second, because “spurs frequently occur in assemblages with high breakage rates” ([74]∶741), if spurs are primarily the result of resharpening accidents we can predict to see a significant positive relationship between the percentage of broken unifacial stone tools at a site and spur frequency. If we do not see these patterns in the Paleoindian archaeological record, then the notion that Paleoindian spurs were primarily the accidental result of resharpening is not supported, and instead, they might be the result of intentional shaping.

Materials and Methods

Samples

To examine the first prediction data were recorded from 563 unbroken unifacial stone tools from seven Clovis sites in the North American Lower Great Lakes region. The analyzed class of “unifacial stone tools” was explicitly defined following Eren et al. ([43]). To examine the second prediction an additional 629 specimens were counted (TABLE 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Number of unifacial stone tool specimens (total, unbroken, and broken) recorded for this study relative to the actual or estimated number of specimens per assemblage.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t001

These specimens are curated at the Cleveland Museum of Natural History, Cleveland, Ohio (Paleo Crossing site); the State Museum of New York, Albany, New York (Potts site); the Royal Ontario Museum, Toronto, Canada (Udora site); the Museum of Anthropology at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan (Leavitt site); and by the private collectors Donald B. Simons in Michigan (Butler, Gainey sites) and Stanley Vanderlaan in New York (Arc site). No permits were required for the described study, which complied with all relevant regulations. All that was needed was permission from the museums or private collectors where the archaeological specimens are curated, and this was given freely.

The regional designation of these sites as “Clovis” is based on the presence of diagnostic artifacts of the Clovis period, namely fluted projectile points with relatively parallel lateral edges and the lack of full-face flutes. Furthermore, the Paleo Crossing site possesses a significant prismatic blade component ([84]), and the Arc site exhibits overshot flakes ([22]), both characteristics of Clovis sites in southern regions of North America. Two of the sites, Paleo Crossing and Sheriden Cave, have yielded average radiocarbon ages of 10,980±75 B.P. and 10,915±30 B.P., respectively ([85]; [86]).

Shott ([54]) suggested that the Leavitt site possessed “Parkhill style” projectile points, a Clovis point variant assumed to occur slightly later in time (though there is no chronometric evidence supporting this assertion in the Lower Great Lakes). Having investigated the Leavitt fluted projectile points ourselves, we agree with others (e.g. [87]; [88]) who classify the points, thus the site, as Clovis. Indeed, even Shott ([54]∶102) suggested that site exhibited similarity to Clovis with respect to particular variables. However, given that Shott ([54]∶102) ultimately concluded that the site is the “earliest Parkhill phase assemblage under study”, and given that the Parkhill Paleoindian phase is estimated to have commenced ca. 10,700 B.P. ([89]), even if one wishes to classify Leavitt as a Parkhill phase site rather than a Clovis one, a case can be made that the site is still representative of the initial colonization stages of the region.

A Quantitative Definition and Measurement of “Spurs”

A spur was defined as any projection no wider than 3 mm, but at least 1 mm long. A box of these dimensions was rendered on paper for an easy, objective, replicable means of quickly assessing whether projections met our metric criteria of a spur (see figure 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Two examples of how the presence of spurs was determined quantitatively.

A box of 3(a). A spur was any projection no wider than 3 mm, but at least 1 mm long (b) (compare with c).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.g001

Tool Resharpening Proxies

In order to assess whether spurs increased in frequency as resharpening advanced, two unifacial stone tool resharpening proxies were used. First, following Buchanan and Collard ([17]; [90]) and Iovita ([91]; [92]), we used tool size as a proxy for tool resharpening extent (mass loss). Although tool size is not always an appropriate proxy for stone tool reduction, for flaked tools that were likely hafted–like Clovis unifaces ([40]; [52]; [54]; [55])–it seems reasonable to use size as a rough proxy for reduction on the grounds that smaller tools are more likely to have been resharpened than larger tools within the same tool class ([90]∶262; see also [93]). Size itself can be measured numerous ways, and here simple tool mass (g) was used.

The second proxy used for tool resharpening was tool shape. A number of lithic analysts have shown that the allometry of unifacial stone tools changes as resharpening advances (e.g., [94]; [95]); namely, tool thickness plays a progressively larger role in tool shape because tool thickness is minimally effected by retouch ([2]; [5]; [50]). Indeed, Eren ([41]) has already shown for the data used here that there is a statistically significant relationship between tool size and shape and, specifically, that as tools got smaller, they also got rounder. Like tool size, tool shape (roundness) can be measured numerous ways, and here shape was assessed as geometric mean size-adjusted thickness (Tsa) ([41]). Size-adjustment of the data proceeds on a specimen-by-specimen basis, dividing each variable in turn by the geometric mean of all variables for that individual specimen. This procedure effectively equalizes the volume of all specimens in a sample, creating a dimensionless scale-free variable while maintaining the original shape information of the data ([96]∶854). The variables measured on each specimen to calculate geometric mean size-adjusted thickness were length, width and thickness (see [41]:Figure 5). Length was measured as the distance parallel to the axis of percussion between the platform (or most proximal point) and the most distal point on the specimen. Width was measured as the distance between the two lateral edges of the specimen at the mid-point of and perpendicular to the length measurement. Thickness was measured as the distance between the dorsal and ventral faces of the specimen at the same location as the width measurement.

Unifacial Stone Tool Breakage Rates Per Site

In order to estimate the number of broken unifacial stone tools as a percentage of all unifacial stone tools at an archaeological site, the number of broken specimens was simply divided by the number of all specimens. Although the samples of broken vs. unbroken specimens were acquired from a larger population of unknown size (because none of the sites have been fully excavated), the robust sample sizes that were counted suggest that we were at least approaching an accurate estimate of the true ratio of broken to unbroken tools (TABLE 1).

Statistical Analyses

To thoroughly compare the relationships between tool size, shape, and breakage and the presence of spurs, we employ multiple, independent statistical tests. Shapiro-Wilk tests demonstrate that tool mass is not normally distributed (W = 0.762, df = 560, p<0.001) and size-adjusted-thickness is normally distributed (W = 0.996, df = 560, p = 0.247). Therefore, we chose to use different statistical tests to assess comparisons involving these two measures. To test for differences in group means in comparisons involving mass, we use the Mann-Whitney U and Kruskal-Wallis non-parametric tests, and for comparisons involving size-adjusted-thickness, we use T-test and Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with subsequent pair-wise Bonferroni comparisons (for significant results only). To test for correlations between these variables and spur count and spur presence, we performed Spearman’s rho correlation analyses.

Results

Does Spur Frequency Increase as Resharpening Advances?

The relationship between tool mass (as a proxy for reduction) and tool spurs was evaluated using both total spur count and spur presence/absence. Beginning with spur count, tool mass does significantly (Kruskal Wallis chi2 = 6.137; df = 2; p = 0.046) decrease with increasing numbers of spurs (TABLE 2, FIGURE 2). This result does support the hypothesis that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. There also appears to be a significant (p = 0.018), negative correlation between tool mass and spur count, however, the Spearman’s rho of −0.100 shows this correlation to be extremely weak. This result is equivocal with regard to the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. To further test this relationship, a series of Spearman’s rho correlation analyses were applied to ranked groupings of the tool dataset. The 563 unifacial stone tools were divided into 10 groups of 56 specimens each (except for the last group, which has 59 specimens). Because some unifacial tools have more than one spur, and others have none, the total number of spurs can be counted and divided by the number of unifacial tools in each group to get a “spurs per uniface” value (TABLE 3). As unifacial stone tool size gets smaller (reduction proceeds) the spurs-per-uniface value significantly increases (ρ = 0.644, p = 0.044). This result does support the notion that spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. The problem with groups of equal sample size is that the range of masses becomes smaller in each group. Regrouping the tools into groups of equal, 5 g mass ranges gives 6 groups (TABLE 4). Spearman’s rho correlation analysis comparing these 6 groups to spurs-per-uniface yields a significant relationship that does support the notion that a greater number of spurs are a result of resharpening (ρ = 0.943, p = 0.005).

thumbnail
Table 3. As unifacial stone tool size gets smaller (reduction proceeds) the spurs-per-uniface value significantly increases (ρ = 0.644, p = 0.044).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. As unifacial stone tool size gets smaller (reduction proceeds) the spurs-per-uniface value significantly increases (ρ = 0.943, p = 0.005).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t004

Turning from spur count to spur presence/absence, there a significant (Mann-Whitney U = 34,310.000; p = 0.032) relationship between tool mass and spur presence/absence (TABLE 5, FIGURE 3). This significance is driven by differences in the heavy range of scrapers, and it does support the notion that spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. There are more very large scrapers (with masses larger than 30 g) that have no spurs. However, at the opposite end, it is clear that many small scrapers also have no spur. There does appear to be a significant (p = 0.032), negative correlation between tool mass and spur presence, however, the Spearman’s rho of −0.090 shows this correlation to be extremely weak. This result is equivocal with regard to the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. Spearman’s rho correlation analyses of the 10 equal-count groups (TABLE 6) shows that as unifacial stone tool size gets smaller the percentage of unifacial stone tools possessing a spur increases, but the relationship is not significant (ρ = 0.542, p = 0.106). This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. Spearman’s rho analyses of equal-mass-range groups (TABLE 7) shows that as mass decreases, the percentage of unifacial stone tools possessing a spur increases, and the relationship is significant (ρ = 0.829, p = 0.042). This result does support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Box plots of tool mass vs. spur presence/absence.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.g003

thumbnail
Table 5. Comparison of tool mass and spur presence/absence.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t005

thumbnail
Table 6. As unifacial stone tool size gets smaller (reduction proceeds) the % of unifaces with a spur does not significantly increase (ρ = 0.542, p = 0.106).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t006

thumbnail
Table 7. As unifacial stone tool size gets smaller (reduction proceeds) the the % of unifaces with a spur significantly increases (ρ = 0.829, p = 0.042).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t007

The relationship between tool size-adjusted-thickness (as a second proxy for reduction, Tsa) and tool spurs was evaluated using both total spur count and spur presence/absence. Beginning with spur count, Tsa does not significantly vary (ANOVA p = 0.245) with spur count (TABLE 8, FIGURE 4). This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. Likewise, there is no significant (Spearman’s ρ = 0.053, p = 0.215) correlation between Tsa and spur count. This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. Spearman’s rho correlation analyses of equal-count groups (TABLE 9) shows no significant relationship between Tsa and spurs-per-uniface (ρ = 0.394, p = 0.260). This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. Spearman’s rho correlation analyses of equal-mass-range groups (TABLE 10) shows no significant relationship between Tsa and spurs-per-uniface (ρ = .257, p = 0.623). This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening.

thumbnail
Table 9. As unifacial stone tools get thicker and rounder (size-adjusted thickness, Tsa, increases) the spurs-per-uniface value does not significantly increase (ρ = 0.394, p = 0.260).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t009

thumbnail
Table 10. As unifacial stone tools get thicker and rounder (size-adjusted thickness, Tsa, increases) the spurs-per-uniface value does not significantly increase (ρ = .257, p = 0.623).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t010

Moving from spur count to spur presence/absence, there is no significant relationship (T-test p = 0.142) between average Tsa and spur presence/absence (TABLE 11, FIGURE 5). This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. Likewise, there is no significant correlation (Spearman’s ρ = 0.061, p = 0.152) between Tsa and spur presence/absence. This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. Spearman’s rho correlation analyses of equal-count groups (TABLE 12) shows no significant relationship between Tsa and the percentage of unifacial stone tools possessing a spur (ρ = 0.470, p = 0.171). This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening. Spearman rho correlation analyses of equal-mass-range groups (TABLE 13) shows no significant relationship between Tsa and spurs-per-uniface (ρ = 0.600, p = 0.208). This result does not support the notion that most spurs are created incidentally or accidentally via resharpening.

thumbnail
Table 12. As unifacial stone tools get thicker and rounder (size-adjusted thickness, Tsa, increases) the % of unifaces with a spur does not significantly increase (ρ = 0.470, p = 0.171).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t012

thumbnail
Table 13. As unifacial stone tools get thicker and rounder (size-adjusted thickness, Tsa, increases) the % of unifaces with a spur does not significantly increase (ρ = 0.600, p = 0.208).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t013

Does Spur Frequency Increase with Increased Tool Breakage?

Spur frequency does not increase with increased tool breakage (TABLE 14). When the breakage percentage of unifacial tools per site is plotted against the percentage of unifacial stone tools with spurs per site there is no relationship (Spearman’s rho = 0.143, p = 0.760). Similarly, when the breakage percentage of unifacial tools per site is plotted against the spurs per uniface value per site there is no relationship (Spearman’s rho = 0.0336, p = 0.939). With regard to our second prediction, the analyzed data do not support the hypothesis that spurs were created accidentally during resharpening.

thumbnail
Table 14. There is no significant correlation between % of broken unifacial stone tools and spurs-per-uniface value (ρ = 0.0336, p = 0.939) nor % of unifaces with a spur (ρ = 0.143, p = 0.760).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078419.t014

Discussion

Examination of both the empirical predictions shows that they are inconsistent with the notion that sharp projections on unifacial stone tools were predominately created via incidental or accidental mechanisms. The first prediction, spur frequency increases with reduction, can be confidently rejected for this Paleoindian dataset. Using mass as a proxy for reduction produced equivocal results. Some statistical tests suggest that spur count and spur presence/absence significantly vary inversely with mass, but other tests suggest the opposite. When size-adjusted-thickness is used as a proxy for reduction, however, every statistical test yields results which allow us to reject the null hypothesis that spur count or spur presence increase with reduction. Likewise, the second prediction, spur frequency increases with tool breakage, shows that there is no relationship between these two variables.

Why do some of the mass comparisons appear to contradict other results, including all of the size-adjusted-thickness results? In our opinion, the most likely explanation involves the use of mass as a reduction measure. It is highly possible that mass is an imperfect measure of reduction given that initial flake-blank size (mass) likely varied. For example, a scraper made on a small initial flake-blank, but given intentional spurs, would appear to be resharpened if only mass is used as a reduction proxy. Size-adjusted-thickness, on the other hand, measures changes in tool shape, possibly providing a more reliable measure of tool reduction intensity that is independent of initial blank size.

Overall, these results do not support Weedman’s assertion that “it is quite probable that the presence of spurs on scrapers in archaeological assemblages worldwide contexts represent not formally made tools, but accidents” ([74]∶741). Instead, our examination of spurs present on unifacial tools made by Clovis foragers in the North American Lower Great Lakes region is consistent with the viewpoint that spurs were at least on occasion created intentionally via retouch. Given that sharp projections of spur-like morphology can arise from either intentional, as demonstrated in this paper, or incidental knapping, as suggested by Weedman ([74]), the status and function of worldwide collections of unifacial stone tool spurs must be empirically established on a case by case basis rather than assumed ([55]∶60). Our results demonstrate that spurs in this Paleoindian sample cannot be explained as predominately incidental.

The obvious and perhaps most direct way spur status and function can be demonstrated is via microwear analysis. In spite of several microwear studies that showed spurs to be highly worn with parallel striations indicating the slotting of bone ([75]; [77]∶90; [97]), Weedman ([74]∶723) argued that “there is notably an absence of microwear studies to verify suggested functions for spurs” because “the presence of wear, polish, and striations on spurs may be a consequence of contact with mastic or the hafting itself rubbing on the spur.” As such, the creation and analysis of experimental middle-range data sets might provide tremendous insight as to whether spur microwear resulting from direct versus incidental use is truly equifinal (e.g., [57]). Even then, however, demonstrating specific instances of Paleoindian spur function will probably remain challenging. Loebel’s ([46]) microwear analysis of Clovis endscrapers from Hawk’s Nest (Illinois), Gainey (Michigan), Nobles Pond (Ohio), and Shawnee-Minisink (Pennsylvania) suggested that wear on spurs could form via failed “last-ditch” resharpening attempts as edge angles became too steep to execute satisfactory edge rejuvenation. Thus, microwear resulting from inferred spur functions such as tattooing, piercing, ripping, or tearing hide, and engraving bone, antler, wood, and ivory might be obscured or eliminated entirely. Yet, on a unifacial tool from the Paleo Crossing site (Ohio), Miller ([98]) documented bright, pitted polish confined to lateral margins of flaked spurs indicating it was used to engrave bone or antler. Finally, Waters et al. ([58], see also [99]) report documented use-wear traces on end scrapers from the Gault site (Texas). Based on polish locations and striation directions, Wiederhold [99] argues that some end scrapers served multiple use-functions during their use-lives. One of those important functions produces extensive wear on spurs of 9 out of the 10 end scrapers examined. They do not, however, discuss what tasks these spurs may have been used for.

To our knowledge, the predictions and results presented above constitute the first explicit, quantitative study specifically examining the interpretation of spur production in any prehistoric context. Furthermore, our study contributes to the growing awareness among archaeologists that claims for prehistoric intentionality with regard to lithic technology–positive or negative–must be empirically and quantitatively supported, rather than assumed ([23]; [100]). Ethnographic studies such as those conducted by Weedman ([74]) are important resources for developing the empirical and quantitative hypotheses and predictions necessary for explaining patterns in the archaeological record. In this paper, we investigated Weedman’s conclusions based on ethnographic analysis and identified testable implications for the hypothesis that Paleoindian scraper spurs are an incidental result of tool resharpening. We showed that incidental resharpening cannot entirely explain the presence of spurs on early Paleoindian unifacial tools in the Great Lakes region. Spurs were more likely produced intentionally as part of the multi-use functions of these Clovis unifacial tools. We are not suggesting that all Paleoindian spurs are intentional or even that all Clovis spurs are intentional. In every case, hypotheses of intentionality must be tested. Indeed, given the significant regional variation in Clovis adaptations and technologies across the continent ([7]; [8]; [12]; [72]; [73]; [101]; [102]) it is highly possible that spurs on unifacial Clovis tools were produced for different functions and in different frequencies in the various environments inhabited by Clovis hunter-gatherers. There is likely to be temporal variation in the frequency of spurs as well, and thus the issue of intentional or incidental spur creation should also be examined in Early Archaic or Late Prehistoric contexts of North America.

However, in the specific temporal and geographic context of the North American Lower Great Lakes region, empirically and quantitatively supporting the hypothesis that at least some unifacial stone tool spurs were intentionally created–and thus the class of artifacts we explicitly define as “unifacial stone tools” were indeed at times multifunctional implements–has important behavioral implications for Clovis foragers. Based on chronometric data and toolstone procurement and discard patterns, the Clovis occupation of this region is widely interpreted to represent a colonization pulse into a recently deglaciated area ([3]; [40]; [41]; [61]; [85]; [86]; [87];[103]; [104]; [105]; [106]; [107]; [108]; [109]). The property of multi-functionality would have increased the portability of the overall toolkit by reducing the number of artifacts needed to be carried, and thus allowing Clovis colonizing foragers to more quickly explore and learn the landscape to reduce uncertainty and risk in space and time ([72]; [110]; [111]). Furthermore, the property of multi-functionality would have allowed Clovis foragers to better respond to “situational contingencies” ([61]; [62]) as they arose during the possibly risky endeavor of colonizing a new and unfamiliar landscape ([112]).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Alex Bentley, Michael J. O’Brien, and three anonymous reviewers whose comments significantly strengthened this manuscript. M.I.E. is grateful to the Leverhulme Trust Early Career Fellowship Program, as well as to Rebecca Catto, and Mustafa, Kathleen, and Nimet Eren.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MIE TAJ AMS. Performed the experiments: MIE TAJ AMS. Analyzed the data: MIE TAJ AMS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MIE TAJ AMS. Wrote the paper: MIE TAJ AMS.

References

  1. 1. Bamforth D (2009) Projectile points, people, and Plains Paleoindian perambulations. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 28 142–157.
  2. 2. Surovell T (2009) Toward a Behavioral Ecology of Lithic Technology: Cases from Paleoindian Archaeology. The University of Arizona Press, Tuscon.
  3. 3. Eren MI, Andrews BN (2013) Were Bifaces used as Mobile Cores by Clovis Foragers in the North American Lower Great Lakes Region? An Archaeological Test of Experimentally Derived Quantitative Predictions. American Antiquity 78: 166–180.
  4. 4. Loebel T (2012) Pattern or bias? A critical evaluation of Midwestern fluted point distributions using raster based GIS. Journal of Archaeological Science 39: 1205–1217.
  5. 5. Patten RJ (2005) Peoples of the Flute: A Study in Anthropolithic Forensics. Stone Dagger Publications, Denver.
  6. 6. Sellet F (2004) Beyond the point: projectile manufacture and behavioral inference. Journal of Archaeological Science 31: 1553–1566.
  7. 7. Smallwood A (2010) Clovis biface technology at the Topper site, South Carolina: evidence for variation and technological flexibility. Journal of Archaeological Science 37: 2413–2425.
  8. 8. Smallwood A (2012) Clovis technology and settlement in the American Southeast: using biface analysis to evaluate dispersal models. American Antiquity 77: 689–713.
  9. 9. Tankersley K (1994) The effects of stone and technology on fluted-point morphometry. American Antiquity 59: 498–510.
  10. 10. Buchanan B, Hamilton M (2009) A formal test of the origin of variation in North American early Paleoindian projectile points. American Antiquity 74: 279–298.
  11. 11. Hamilton M, Buchanan B (2009) The accumulation of stochastic copying errors causes drift in culturally transmitted technologies: quantifying Clovis evolutionary dynamics. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 28: 55–69.
  12. 12. Jennings TA (2013) The Hogeye Clovis cache, Texas: Quantifying lithic reduction signatures. Journal of Archaeological Science 40: 649–658.
  13. 13. Morrow J, Morrow T (1999) Geographic variation in fluted projectile points: a hemispheric perspective. American Antiquity 64: 215–230.
  14. 14. Shott M (2010) Stone-tool demography: reduction distributions in North American Paleoindian tools. In New Perspectives on Old Stones (pp. 275–293). Springer, New York.
  15. 15. Buchanan B, Collard M (2007) Investigating the peopling of North America through cladistic analyses of early Paleoindian projectile points. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 26: 366–393.
  16. 16. Buchanan B (2006) An analysis of Folsom projectile point resharpening using quantitative comparisons of form and allometry. Journal of Archaeological Science 33: 185–199.
  17. 17. Buchanan B, Collard M (2010) A geometric morphometrics-based assessment of blade shape differences among Paleoindian projectile point types from western North America. Journal of Archaeological Science 37: 350–359.
  18. 18. Buchanan B, Collard M, Hamilton M, O’Brien M (2011) Points and prey: an evaluation of the hypothesis that prey size predicts early Paleoindian projectile point form. Journal of Archaeological Science 38: 852–864.
  19. 19. Buchanan B, Kilby J, Huckell B, O’Brien M, Collard M (2012) A morphometric assessment of the intended function of cached Clovis points. PLoS One 7(2): 1–13 (e30530)..
  20. 20. Callahan E (1990) The basics of biface knapping in the eastern fluted point tradition: a manual for flintknappers and lithic analysts. Eastern States Archeological Federation.
  21. 21. Ellis C, Payne J (1995) Estimating failure rates in fluting based on archaeological data: examples from NE North America. Journal of Field Archaeology 22: 459–474.
  22. 22. Eren MI, Vanderlaan S, Holland J (2011) Overshot flaking at the Arc Site, Genesee County, New York: Examining the Clovis-Gainey Connection. The Open Anthropol J 4: 40–52.
  23. 23. Eren MI, Patten RJ, O’Brien MJ, Meltzer DJ (2013) Refuting the technological cornerstone of the Ice-Age Atlantic Crossing Hypothesis. Journal of Archaeological Science 40: 2934–2941.
  24. 24. Frison G (1989) Experimental use of Clovis weaponry and tools on African elephants. American Antiquity 54: 766–784.
  25. 25. Jennings TA, Pevny C, Dickens W (2010) A biface and blade core efficiency experiment: implications for Early Paleoindian technological organization. Journal of Archaeological Science 37: 2155–2164.
  26. 26. Patten RJ (2002) Solving the Folsom fluting problem. In Folsom Technology and Lifeways (pp. 299–308). Lithic Technology, Tulsa.
  27. 27. Praciunas M (2007) Bifacial cores and flake production efficiency: an experimental test of technological assumptions. American Antiquity 72: 334–348.
  28. 28. Shott M, Ballenger J (2007) Biface reduction and the measurement of dalton curation: a southeastern United States case study. American Antiquity 72: 153–175.
  29. 29. Deller D, Ellis C, Keron J (2009) Understanding Cache Variability: A Deliberately Burned Early Paleoindian Tool Assemblage from the Crowfield Site, Southwestern Ontario, Canada. American Antiquity 74: 371–397.
  30. 30. Ellis C, Deller D (2002) The Caradoc Site: A Late Paleoindian Ritual Artifact Deposit. Occasional Publications of the London Chapter, Ontario Archaeological Society No.8, London, Ontario.
  31. 31. Meltzer D (1981) A study of style and function in a class of tools. Journal of Field Archaeology 8: 313–326.
  32. 32. Wilmsen E (1970) Lithic analysis and cultural inference: A Paleo-Indian case (No. 16). University of Arizona Press, Tucson.
  33. 33. Wilmsen E (1974) Lindenmeier: A Pleistocene hunting society. Harper & Row, New York.
  34. 34. Collins M (1999) Clovis Blade Technology. University of Texas Press, Austin.
  35. 35. Cox S (1986) A re-analysis of the Shoop site. Archaeology of Eastern North America 14: 101–170.
  36. 36. Deller D, Ellis C (1992) Thedford II: a Paleo-Indian site in the Ausable River watershed of southwestern Ontario (No. 24). Univ of Michigan Museum.
  37. 37. Deller D, Keron J, King R, Ellis C (2011) Crowfield (AfHj-31): A Unique Paleoindian Fluted Point Site from Southwestern Ontario. University of Michigan.
  38. 38. Ellis C, Deller D (1988) Some Distinctive Paleo-Indian Tool Types from the Lower Great Lakes Area. Midcontinental Journal of Archaeology 13: 111–158.
  39. 39. Ellis C, Deller D (2000) An Early Paleo-Indian Site Near Parkhill, Ontario. Canadian Museum of Civilization, Archaeological Survey of Canada, Mercury Series Paper No. 159, Gatineau, Quebec.
  40. 40. Eren MI (2012) Were Unifacial Tools Regularly Hafted by Clovis Foragers in the North American Lower Great Lakes Region? An Empirical Test of Edge Class Richness and Attribute Frequency among Distal, Proximal, and Lateral Tool-sections. Journal of Ohio Archaeology 2: 1–15.
  41. 41. Eren MI (2013) The Technology of Stone Age Colonization: An Empirical, Regional-Scale Examination of Clovis Unifacial Stone Tool Reduction, Allometry, and Edge Angle from the North American Lower Great Lakes Region. Journal of Archaeological Science 40: 2101–2112.
  42. 42. Eren MI, Redmond BG, Kollecker MA (2005) Unifacial Stone Tool Analyses from the Paleo Crossing Site (33-ME-274), Ohio. Current Research in the Pleistocene 22: 43–45.
  43. 43. Eren MI, Chao A, Hwang WH, Colwell RK (2012) Estimating the Richness of a Population When the Maximum Number of Classes Is Fixed: A Nonparametric Solution to an Archaeological Problem. PLoS One 7(5): e34179.
  44. 44. Grimes J, Grimes B (1985) Flakeshavers: morphometric, functional and life-cycle analyses of a Paleoindian unifacial tool class. Archaeology of Eastern North America 13: 35–57.
  45. 45. Jackson L, Buehrle A (1998) The Sandy Ridge and Halstead Paleo-Indian Sites: Unifacial Tool Use and Gainey Phase Definition in South-central Ontario. University of Michigan Museum of Anthropology, Ann Arbor.
  46. 46. Loebel T (2013) Endscrapers, use-wear, and early Paleoindians in Eastern North America. In In The Eastern Fluted Point Tradition (pp. 315–330). University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City.
  47. 47. Lothrop J (1988) The Organization of Paleoindian Lithic Technology at the Potts Site. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Anthropology, State University of New York at Binghamton.
  48. 48. Lothrop J (1989) The organization of Paleoindian lithic technology at the Potts site. In Eastern Paleoindian Lithic Resource Use (pp. 99–138). Westview Press, Boulder.
  49. 49. Morris E, Blakeslee R (1987) Comment on the Paleoindian Occurrence of Spurred End Scrapers as Reported by Rogers. American Antiquity 52: 830–831.
  50. 50. Morrow J (1997) End scraper morphology and use-life: An approach for studying Paleoindian lithic technology and mobility. Lithic Technology 22: 70–85.
  51. 51. Rogers R (1986) Spurred end scrapers as diagnostic Paleoindian artifacts: a distributional analysis on stream terraces. American Antiquity 51: 338–341.
  52. 52. Rule P, Evans J (1985) The relationship of morphological variation to hafting techniques among Paleo-Indian endscrapers at the Shawnee Minisink site. In Shawnee Minisink: A Stratified Paleoindian Archaic Site in the Upper Delaware Valley of Pennsylvania (pp. 211–220). Academic Press, Orlando.
  53. 53. Sellet F (2006) Two steps forward, one step back: inference of mobility patterns from stone tools. In Archaeology and Ethnoarchaeology of Mobility (pp. 221–239). University Press of Florida, Gainesville.
  54. 54. Shott M (1993) The Leavitt Site: A Parkhill Phase Paleo-Indian Occupation in Central Michigan. Memoirs of the University of Michigan Museum of Anthropology, No. 25, Ann Arbor.
  55. 55. Shott M (1995) How much is a scraper? Curation, use rates, and the formation of scraper assemblages. Lithic Technology 20: 53–72.
  56. 56. Storck P (1997) The Fisher site: Archaeological, geological and paleobotanical studies at an Early Paleo-Indian site in southern Ontario, Canada. Museum of Anthropology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.
  57. 57. Tomenchuk J, Storck P (1997) Two newly recognized Paleoindian tool types: Single-and double-scribe compass gravers and coring gravers. American Antiquity 62: 508–522.
  58. 58. Waters M, Pevny C, Carlson D (2011) Clovis Lithic Technology: Investigation of a Stratified Workshop at the Gault Site, Texas. Texas A & M University Press, College Station.
  59. 59. Binford L (1979) Organization and formation processes: looking at curated technologies. Journal of Anthropological Research 35: 255–273.
  60. 60. Bleed P (1986) The optimal design of hunting weapons: maintainability or reliability. American Antiquity 51: 737–747.
  61. 61. Ellis C (2008) The Fluted Point tradition and the Arctic Small Tool tradition: What’s the connection? Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 27: 298–314.
  62. 62. Goodyear A (1989) A hypothesis for the use of cryptocrystalline raw materials among Paleoindian groups of North America. In Eastern Paleoindian Lithic Resource Use (pp. 1–10). Westview Press, Boulder.
  63. 63. Gould R (1969) Yiwara: Foragers of the Australian Desert. Charles Scribner’s Sons, New York.
  64. 64. Graf K (2010) Hunter-gatherer dispersals in the mammoth-steppe: technological provisioning and land-use in the Enisei River Valley, south-central Siberia. Journal of Archaeological Science 37: 210–223.
  65. 65. Kelly R, Todd L (1988) Coming into the country: Early Paleoindian hunting and mobility. American Antiquity 53: 231–244.
  66. 66. Kuhn S (1994) A formal approach to the design and assembly of mobile tool kits. American Antiquity 59: 426–442.
  67. 67. Nelson M (1991) The study of technological organization. Archaeological Method and Theory 3: 57–100.
  68. 68. Shott M (1986) Forager mobility and technological organization: an ethnographic examination. Journal of Anthropological Research 42: 15–51.
  69. 69. Torrence R (1983) Tools as optimal solutions. In Time, Energy, and Stone Tools (pp. 1–6). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  70. 70. Torrence R (2001) Hunter-gatherer technology: macro- and microscale approaches. In Hunter-Gatherers: An Interdisciplinary Perspective (pp. 73–95). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  71. 71. Yellen J (1976) Settlement patterns of the !Kung: an archaeological perspective. In Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and Their Neighbors (pp. 47–72). Harvard University Press, Cambridge.
  72. 72. Meltzer D (2002) What do you do when no one’s been there before? Thoughts on the exploration and colonization of New Lands. In The First Americans: The Pleistocene Colonization of the New World (pp. 27–58). Wattis Symposium Series in Anthropology, Memoirs of the California Academy of Sciences Number 27, San Francisco.
  73. 73. Meltzer D (2009) First Peoples in a New World: Colonizing Ice Age America. University of California Press, Berkeley.
  74. 74. Weedman K (2002) On the spur of the moment: effects of age and experience on hafted stone scraper morphology. American Antiquity 67: 731–744.
  75. 75. Irwin H, Wormington H (1970) Paleo-Indian Tool Types in the Great Plains. American Antiquity 35: 24–34.
  76. 76. Kraft H (1973) The Plenge site: A Paleo-indian occupation site in New Jersey. Archaeology of Eastern North America 1: 56–117.
  77. 77. MacDonald G (1968) Debert: A Palaeo-Indian Site in Central Nova Scotia (No. 16). Queen’s Printer.
  78. 78. Gramly R, Lothrop J (1984) Archaeological investigations of the Potts Site, Oswego County, New York, 1982 and 1983. Archaeology of Eastern North America 12: 122–158.
  79. 79. Clark J, Kurashina H (1981) A study of the work of a modern tanner in Ethiopia and its relevance for archaeological interpretation. Modern Material Culture: The Archaeology of Us, Academic Press, New York, 303–343.
  80. 80. Nissen K, Dittemore M (1974) Ethnographic data and wear pattern analysis: a study of socketed Eskimo scrapers. Tebiwa 17: 67–88.
  81. 81. Grimes J, Eldridge W, Grimes B, Vaccaro A, Vaccaro F, et al. (1984) Bull Brook II. Archaeology of Eastern North America 12: 159–183.
  82. 82. Hardy B (2009) Understanding Stone Tool Function: Methods and Examples from the Aurignacian Levels at Hohle Fels. Mitteilungen der Gesellschaft für Urgeschichte 18: 109–121.
  83. 83. McCall G (2012) Ethnoarchaeology and the organization of lithic technology. Journal of Archaeological Research 20: 157–203.
  84. 84. Eren MI, Redmond B (2011) Clovis blades at Paleo Crossing (33ME274), Medina County, Ohio. Midcontinental Journal of Archaeology 36: 173–194.
  85. 85. Brose D (1994) Archaeological investigations at the Paleo Crossing site, a Paleoindian occupation in Medina County, Ohio. In The First Discovery of America (pp. 61–76). The Ohio Archaeological Council, Columbus.
  86. 86. Waters M, Stafford Jr T, Redmond B, Tankersley K (2009) The age of the Paleoindian assemblage at Sheriden Cave, Ohio. American Antiquity 74: 107–112.
  87. 87. Simons D (1997) The Gainey and Butler sites as focal points for caribou and people. In Caribou and Reindeer Hunters of the Northern Hemisphere (pp. 105–131). Avebury, Aldershot.
  88. 88. Ellis C (2011) Measuring Paleoindian range mobility and land-use in the Great Lakes/Northeast. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 30: 385–401.
  89. 89. Ellis C, Deller B (1997) Variability in the archaeological record of northeastern early Paleo-indians: a view from southern Ontario. Archaeology of Eastern North America 25: 1–30.
  90. 90. Buchanan B, Collard M (2010) An Assessment of the Impact of Resharpening on Paleoindian Projectile Point Blade Shape Using Geometric Morphometric Techniques. In New Perspectives on Old Stones (pp. 255–273). Springer, New York.
  91. 91. Ioviţă R (2010) Comparing stone tool resharpening trajectories with the aid of elliptical Fourier analysis. In New Perspectives on Old Stones (pp. 235–253). Springer, New York.
  92. 92. Iovita R (2011) Shape variation in Aterian tanged tools and the origins of projectile technology: a morphometric perspective on stone tool function. PloS ONE 6(12): e29029.
  93. 93. Shott M (1989) Technological organization in Great Lakes Paleo-Indian assemblages. In Eastern Paleoindian Lithic Resource Use (pp. 221–237). Westview Press, Boulder.
  94. 94. Shott M, Weedman K (2007) Measuring reduction in stone tools: an ethnoarchaeological study of Gamo hidescrapers from Ethiopia. Journal of Archaeological Science 34: 1016–1035.
  95. 95. Blades B (2003) End scraper reduction and hunter-gatherer mobility. American Antiquity 68: 141–156.
  96. 96. Lycett S, von Cramon-Taubadel N, Foley R (2006) A crossbeam co-ordinate caliper for the morphometric analysis of lithic nuclei: a description, text, and empirical examples of application. Journal of Archaeological Science 33: 847–861.
  97. 97. Vaughan P (1985) Use Wear Analysis of Flaked Stone-tools. University of Arizona Press, Tucson.
  98. 98. Miller GL (2013) Illuminating activities at Paleo Crossing (33ME274) through microwear analysis. Lithic Technology.
  99. 99. Wiederhold J (2004) Toward the standardization of use–wear studies: constructing an analogue to prehistoric hide work. Unpublished Master’s thesis, Texas A&M University, College Station.
  100. 100. Lycett S, Chauhan P (2010) Analytical approaches to Palaeolithic technologies: an introduction. In New Perspectives on Old Stones (pp. 1–22). Springer, New York.
  101. 101. Cannon M, Meltzer D (2008) Explaining variability in early Paleoindian foraging. Quaternary International 191: 5–17.
  102. 102. Meltzer D (1993) Is there a Clovis adaptation? In From Kostenki to Clovis: Upper Paleolithic-Paleo-Indian Adaptations (pp. 293–310). Plenum Press, New York.
  103. 103. Anderson D (1990) The Paleoindian colonization of eastern North America: a view from the Southeastern United States. In Research in Economic Anthropology Supplement 5: Early Paleoindian Economies of Eastern North America (pp. 163–216). JAI Press, Greenwich.
  104. 104. Ellis C, Goodyear A, Morse D, Tankersley K (1998) Archaeology of the Pleistocene-Holocene transition in eastern North America. Quaternary International 49/50: 151–166.
  105. 105. Ellis C, Carr D, Loebel T (2011) The Younger Dryas and Late Pleistocene peoples of the Great Lakes region. Quaternary International 242: 534–545.
  106. 106. Redmond B, Tankersley K (2005) Evidence of early Paleoindian bone modification and use at the Sheriden Cave site (33WY252), Wyandot County, Ohio. American Antiquity 70: 503–526.
  107. 107. Storck P, Spiess A (1994) The significance of new faunal identifications attributed to an Early Paleoindian (Gainey Complex) occupation at the Udora Site, Ontario, Canada. American Antiquity 59: 121–142.
  108. 108. Tankersley K (1990) Late Pleistocene lithic exploitation in the Midwest and Midsouth: Indiana, Ohio, and Kentucky. In Research in Economic Anthropology Supplement 5: Early Paleoindian Economies of Eastern North America (pp. 259–302). JAI Press, Greenwich.
  109. 109. Tankersley K (1994) Was Clovis a colonization population in Eastern North America? In The First Discovery of America (pp. 95–116). The Ohio Archaeological Council, Columbus.
  110. 110. Meltzer D (2003) Lessons in landscape learning. In The Colonization of Unfamiliar Landscapes (pp. 222–241). Routledge, London.
  111. 111. Kelly R (2003) Colonization of new land by hunter-gatherers: expectations and implications based on ethnographic data. In Colonization of Unfamiliar Landscapes (pp. 44–58). Routledge, London.
  112. 112. Meltzer D (2004) Modeling the initial colonization of the Americas: issues of scale, demography, and landscape learning. In The Settlement of the American Continents (pp. 123–137). University of Arizona Press, Tucson.