Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Avian Influenza Infection Alters Fecal Odor in Mallards

  • Bruce A. Kimball ,

    bruce.a.kimball@aphis.usda.gov

    Affiliation United States Department of Agriculture, Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service, Wildlife Services, National Wildlife Research Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Kunio Yamazaki †,

    † Deceased.

    Affiliation Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Dennis Kohler,

    Affiliation United States Department of Agriculture, Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service, Wildlife Services, National Wildlife Research Center, Fort Collins, Colorado, United States of America

  • Richard A. Bowen,

    Affiliation Department of Biomedical Sciences, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, Colorado, United States of America

  • Jack P. Muth,

    Affiliation Department of Microbiology, Immunology and Pathology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, Colorado, United States of America

  • Maryanne Opiekun,

    Affiliation Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Gary K. Beauchamp

    Affiliation Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America

Abstract

Changes in body odor are known to be a consequence of many diseases. Much of the published work on disease-related and body odor changes has involved parasites and certain cancers. Much less studied have been viral diseases, possibly due to an absence of good animal model systems. Here we studied possible alteration of fecal odors in animals infected with avian influenza viruses (AIV). In a behavioral study, inbred C57BL/6 mice were trained in a standard Y-maze to discriminate odors emanating from feces collected from mallard ducks (Anas platyrhynchos) infected with low-pathogenic avian influenza virus compared to fecal odors from non-infected controls. Mice could discriminate odors from non-infected compared to infected individual ducks on the basis of fecal odors when feces from post-infection periods were paired with feces from pre-infection periods. Prompted by this indication of odor change, fecal samples were subjected to dynamic headspace and solvent extraction analyses employing gas chromatography/mass spectrometry to identify chemical markers indicative of AIV infection. Chemical analyses indicated that AIV infection was associated with a marked increase of acetoin (3-hydroxy-2-butanone) in feces. These experiments demonstrate that information regarding viral infection exists via volatile metabolites present in feces. Further, they suggest that odor changes following virus infection could play a role in regulating behavior of conspecifics exposed to infected individuals.

Introduction

It has long been speculated that infections may cause odor changes in animals and humans [1][4]. For example, it was shown recently that infection with Mycobacterium tuberculosis produces unique volatiles in the breath of infected donors [5]. Several cancers have been detected from odors in breath or urine by trained dogs [6]. Much of the research involving volatile metabolites associated with infection has focused on parasites [7][9], though influenza [10] and mammary tumor virus [11] have been studied in mice. The mechanisms underlying alteration of body odor by infection are poorly understood, but they may involve alteration of immune function [11]. Odor changes may also result directly from disease pathology [12].

There is also convincing evidence that healthy individuals modify their social behaviors when exposed to infected conspecifics themselves or their body odors. Avoidance of individuals on the basis of odors associated with illness may serve to reduce the probability of disease spread [13]. Alternatively, it would be adaptive for the infective agent to increase both inter- and intraspecific interactions via attraction to these same odors. Attraction has been clearly demonstrated for interspecific interactions between human hosts and insect vectors [14].

The hypothesis that immune activation may produce a meaningful alteration of body odor led us to study odor changes resulting from administration of vaccines in mice (unpublished data). Using biosensor panels of trained mice, we demonstrated that body odor was altered by immunization with either rabies or West Nile Virus vaccine. Based on this promising result, we conducted similar experiments with an avian influenza virus (AIV) to evaluate whether infection produces a distinctive odor change in a relevant species; mallard ducks (Anas platyrhynchos). The mallard is considered an excellent research model for influenza research [15]. We examined feces because they are a source of social odors [16] and because they are the primary avian waste excretion product (unlike rodents which employ both feces and urine to serve this function). Signaling of viral infection could have important implications for social interactions in brood-forming species, such as mallards. Although auditory and visual senses are highly developed in avian species, the role of olfaction is often overlooked in birds. However, avian olfaction has been implicated in many behaviors such as navigation and foraging [17] and predator avoidance [18]. Thus, it is quite possible that infections could be recognized by brood mates on the basis of fecal odor change.

We hypothesized that infection with a low pathogenic influenza virus (H5N2) would alter the volatile profile of feces. We first postulated that these changes would be discriminable by mice trained in a Y-maze to identify odors associated with AIV infection. The Y-maze has successfully been used to discriminate between many different sources of odor variation, including: fetal odortype [19], [20], disease [11], [21], age [22], and diet [23]. Successful demonstration that infection resulted in fecal odor change verified that volatile emissions differ between the feces of AIV-infected and non-infected ducks. Having successfully demonstrated this occurrence, we employed a series of chemical analyses to identify volatiles that are significantly altered (qualitatively or quantitatively) in feces as a result of AIV infection.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health and the Guide for the Care and Use of Agricultural Animals in Agricultural Research and Teaching of the Federation of Animal Science Societies. Animal procedures were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of the Monell Chemical Senses Center (#1123), USDA National Wildlife Research Center (#2021), and Colorado State University (#09-1317A).

Duck Feces

Eight farm-raised mallards of mixed gender were used in this study. Six ducks were infected ocularly, intranasally and orally with 1 ml of brain heart infusion broth containing 1×106 pfu/ml of A/Mallard/MN/346250/00 (H5N2). Cloacal swabs were collected on days three and four following experimental infection. Infectivity was confirmed by real-time RT-PCR and inoculation into 10-day old embryonating chicken eggs.

Two pooled feces samples were collected from each duck. Feces were collected daily for seven days immediately preceding experimental infection and again for days three through 10 post-infection. Eight pre-treatment and eight post-treatment samples were stored frozen at −80°C. To facilitate safe transport and handling of feces, all 16 samples were subjected to 2.7 Mrad of cesium irradiation for 27 hours. Lack of infectivity was confirmed by inoculation into 10-day old embryonating chicken eggs.

Biosensor Assays

Six female C57BL/6 in-bred mice (born and raised in our laboratory) were trained to discriminate between feces from non-infected and infected ducks in a Y-maze as previously described [24]. The reward for correct response was a drop of water (the mouse having been restricted of water for 23 hours). Air was conducted through two odor chambers, each containing feces (0.5 g) exposed in Petri dishes for odor delivery to a maze arm. Gates placed at the entrance of each arm and at reward delivery locations inside the arms were manually raised and lowered in timed sequence to permit the training or testing of each mouse in a session of up to 48 consecutive trials.

In training sessions, fecal samples from three ducks (#3, 4, and 13; Table 1) were presented pair-wise (representing samples collected before and after experimental infection) and mice were rewarded for correctly choosing the maze arm associated with feces collected post-infection. For sessions 1 through 10, only one pre-infection and one post-infection sample were used (Table 2). Because the number of donors was limited, pairs (of samples with identical infection status) were used for sessions 11 through 13. Furthermore, pairs were frequently used for rewarded trials in generalization trials. Given the small number of donors, pairing of samples was used to artificially create unique odor stimuli. Training continued until each biosensor achieved greater than 80% concordance (correctly responding to feces from infected duck). Throughout training, about 25% of trials in a session were intentionally not rewarded, even if a correct response was evident.

thumbnail
Table 1. Donor IDs, treatments, and use of feces in behavioral assays.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075411.t001

Sessions of unrewarded generalization trials were initiated when each biosensor demonstrated >80% concordance. Fecal samples from three infected ducks (#5, 11, and 12; Table 1) and two uninfected control ducks (#1 and 2; Table 1) were used in unrewarded generalization trials. These generalization samples were unfamiliar to the biosensors (not used in training trials) and were “double-blind” (the infection status of these paired samples was unknown to the maze operator). Each session consisted of four or five generalization trials interspersed among rewarded trials employing training samples. All within-duck comparisons (pre- versus post-infection) were subjected to the bioassay as well as some between-duck comparisons.

Cumulative responses of the full panel of trained mice were calculated for each generalization trial. Success rates (number of correct trials divided by the total number of generalization trials) were subjected to statistical tests of binomial proportion using the continuity correction for small numbers of observations [25]. A success rate of 0.5 was the null hypothesis for all tests.

Fecal Analysis: Headspace Volatiles

Headspace analyses were conducted with a HT3 dynamic headspace analyzer (Teledyne Tekmar, Mason, OH, USA) outfitted with Supelco Trap K Vocarb 3000 thermal desorption trap (Sigma-Aldrich Co., St. Louis, MO, USA) attached to a Thermo Trace GC-MS equipped with a single quadrapole mass spectrometer (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA) and a 30 m×0.25 mm id Stabiliwax-DA fused-silica capillary column (Restek, Bellefonte, PA, USA). Fecal samples (0.5 g) were maintained at 50°C, swept with helium for 60 min (flow rate of 75 mL/min), and the volatiles collected on the thermal desorption trap. Trap contents were desorbed at 260°C. The GC oven program had an initial temperature of 40°C (held for 3.0 min) followed by a ramp of 7.0°C/min to a final temperature of 230°C (held for 6.0 min). The MS was used in scan mode from 41 to 400 m/z.

Samples were analyzed in triplicate. Chromatographic data were converted to NetCDF format for baseline correction, noise elimination, and peak alignment processing using Metalign [26]. Multivariate results of this process consist of all mass spectrometric responses (m/z) exceeding a pre-defined threshold at each scan event. Sample means were exported to Unscrambler (Camo Software Inc., Woodbridge, NJ) for principal components analysis (PCA). An iterative process was used to select observations which contributed to a PCA model with visually evident segregation of AIV infection status in 2D scatter plots. Original chromatographic data were evaluated to determine the potential identity of the volatile compounds giving rise to the PCA results.

Fecal Analysis: Solvent Extraction

Semi-quantitative analyses of acetoin (3-hydroxy-2-butanone) and 1-octen-3-ol in fecal samples were conducted by subjecting 0.25–0.75 g fecal samples to extraction with 10.0 mL ethanol (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburg, PA) in 25-mL screw-cap culture tubes. Analytical standards were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich (Milwaukee, WI). The tubes and contents were placed in a vortex mixer (Fisher Multitube Vortexer, Fisher Scientific) for 30 minutes and centrifuged at 2000 rpm (Thermo IEC Centra CL2, Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA). Non-volatile extractives (e.g. plant pigments from duck diets) were removed by passing green-colored extracts through graphitized carbon solid phase extraction (SPE) cartridges (Supelco, Bellefonte, PA). Sample eluates were colorless, indicating that plant pigments were removed from the solvent extracts.

Splitless injections (1 µl) were made into a Thermo Scientific ISQ GC-MS equipped with a single quadrapole mass spectrometer (Thermo Scientific) and a 30 m×0.25 mm id Stabiliwax-DA fused-silica capillary column (Restek, Bellefonte, PA, USA). The GC oven program had an initial temperature of 40°C (held for 1.0 min) followed by a ramp of 3.0°C/min to 112°C and a ramp of 25°C/min to 235°C (held for 3.0 min). The MS was used in scan mode from 33 to 400 m/following an 8.5 min solvent delay. Selection ion monitoring (SIM) chromatograms were produced from the sum of the m/z responses of 45 and 57. Peak area responses were normalized by dividing peak area by sample mass (g). Peak area ratios were also calculated by dividing the 1-octen-3-ol peak area response by the acetoin response. Peak and peak ratio responses were evaluated by t-test to determine if they were impacted by AIV infection. Because the pre-treatment fecal sample from duck 11 was exhausted during bioassays, peak responses from extracts of the post-infection sample were not included in statistical analyses.

Several experiments were conducted to evaluate method performance. A standard solution containing both acetoin (5.69 µg/mL) and 1-octen-3-ol (5.05 g/mL) was repeatedly passed through SPE columns to evaluate irreversible loss of the analytes. Replicate extractions were also conducted with the post-infection sample from one duck (duck 5) to evaluate repeatability.

Results

Viral Infection

Results of real-time RT-PCR and chicken egg inoculation confirmed infectivity in test mallards three days after experimental infection with the virus. Furthermore, the virus was no longer viable in the fecal samples following irradiation.

Bioassay

All six trained mice demonstrated greater than 80% concordance (choosing the maze arm associated with fecal odors from infected ducks) in 13 training sessions. These mice generalized the trained response correctly for all within-duck comparisons (pre- vs. post-infection for ducks #5, 11, and 12; Table 3). As anticipated, trained mice did not discriminate between pre- and post-infection samples from sham-treated ducks (#1 and 2; Table 3). Together, these results indicate that trained mice associated the water reward with the odor of H5N2 infection (not individual duck identity). Furthermore, lack of discrimination among feces from non-infected ducks (Ducks #1 and 2; Table 3) during generalization sessions indicated that sample collection period (pre- vs. post-infection) was not the source of within-duck fecal odor changes.

thumbnail
Table 3. Bioassay results of unrewarded generalization trials (pair-wise, within-subjects comparisons of feces collected before and after experimental AIV infection).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075411.t003

Results from between-duck generalizations were not as definitive. As would be predicted from mice trained to identify the odor of feces from AIV-infected ducks, mice did not discriminate between feces samples collected from two ducks collected during the pre-infection period (duck 12 versus duck 1; Table 4). However, trained mice were also expected to select the maze arm scented with the odor of infected feces when paired with feces of healthy ducks, even feces collected in the post-infection period. Although, trained mice correctly identified post-infection feces (duck 5) when paired with the post-infection feces from control duck 2 (Table 4), the mice did not discriminate when duck 5 post-infection feces was paired with post-infection feces from control duck 1 (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Bioassay results of unrewarded generalization trials (pair-wise, between-subjects comparisons of feces).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075411.t004

Chemical Analysis

Data processing of headspace data identified >1900 significant mass spectral responses for each sample. Reconstruction of a composite chromatogram from these responses indicated that 96 individual chromatographic peaks were present in the headspace of the fecal samples. Principal Components Analysis (PCA) of headspace data identified two mass spectral responses that adequately segregated feces samples on the basis of AIV infection status. These responses were tentatively identified as acetoin (m/z = 88 at scan 2141) and 1-octen-3-ol (m/z = 57 at scan 2876).

Peak area ratios (1-octen-3-ol:acetoin) obtained from solvent extraction ranged from 0 to 0.88 across all ducks and collection periods (Table 5). Feces from infected ducks yielded significantly lower peak area ratios (mean = 0.089) than feces from non-infected ducks (mean = 0.535; p = 0.0014 Table 5). Though not highly significant themselves, the low peak ratios of infected ducks were a product of increased acetoin peak responses (p = 0.095) and reduced 1-octen-3-ol responses (p = 0.10) resulting from AIV infection. Results of method evaluation experiments indicated the method was highly repeatable (5.8%) and clean-up of the solvent extracts did not impact the peak ratio response (p = 0.32).

thumbnail
Table 5. Peak area responses (×105) of acetoin and 1-octen-3-ol determined in ethanol extracts of duck feces.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075411.t005

Discussion

Trained mice readily discriminated feces of non-infected and infected mallards on the basis of volatile metabolites during training and generalized this response to novel fecal samples differing in infection status (Table 3). That a distinctive fecal odor difference was recognized by trained mice suggests that AIV infection in mallards may be “advertised” to other members of the population as a change in fecal odor. However, not all between-subjects comparisons were successfully discriminated by trained mice (Table 4). Significant individual odor variation among ducks may have made some between-subjects discriminations difficult for trained mice. Thus, it is probable that the odor cues used for discrimination are present both before and after AIV infection, except that infection produces a significant quantitative change in these volatiles. Chemical analyses similarly indicated that fecal volatiles changed quantitatively due to infection. The significant decreases in peak area ratios suggest that decreased 1-octen-3-ol and increased acetoin peak responses are associated with AIV infection.

These compounds (1-octen-3-ol and acetoin) have been identified as potential biomarkers for diagnosing gastrointestinal diseases in humans [27]. Acetoin is an enzymatic decarboxylation product of pyruvate. Many Bacillus bacteria, which produce acetoin from pyruvate in the presence of glucose [28], have been identified in duck feces [29]. Additionally, accumulation of cellular pyruvate resulting from viral infection has been known for some time [30]. Multiple type A influenza variants have been shown to impact intracellular glycolytic flux resulting in increased production of pyruvate [31]. Thus, elevated levels of acetoin observed in feces of infected mallards may have resulted from increased pyruvate available to gastrointestinal bacteria. As an enzymatic product, it is unlikely that fecal acetoin was an artifact of sample irradiation.

Two test systems (i.e. trained biosensors and chromatographic analysis) independently confirmed that alterations of fecal volatiles corresponded with AIV infection. However, there is no evidence that acetoin and 1-octen-3-ol represent the very same cues learned by the biosensors when they were trained to discriminate feces on the basis of infection status. Fecal odors associated with infection may involve many aspects of disease pathology and likely result in a myriad of qualitative and quantitative alterations. It is also unclear whether other infectious agents may produce similar or different changes in fecal volatiles. However, pathogen specificity is not a prerequisite for communicating health status to conspecifics. Odor changes in response to infection may be as general as fluctuation of body temperature, yet detectible changes could alert members of the brood to the presence of a potentially transmissible pathogen.

Chemical communication of infection can be adaptive to either the host population (causing individuals in the brood to avoid infected conspecifics) or the pathogen (making infected individuals attractive to other conspecifics). Avoidance of urine odors associated with influenza has been demonstrated in mice [10]. A number of studies have documented that parasite infection alters body odor and the odor of infected mammals is avoided [32], [33]. Conversely, children harboring the parasite Plasmodium falciparum attracted twice as many mosquitoes as children uninfected with the malarial parasites [14]. This conflict (attractiveness vs. avoidance) represents a fascinating aspect of pathogen recognition. The host and pathogen genomes can be thought as engaging in a struggle: the pathogen benefits when infected individuals attract new hosts – while healthy individuals benefit by avoiding infected individuals. While either outcome is possible, the chemical means for communicating infection exists. In the case of AIV infection in mallards, the virus and native gut bacteria appear to work in concert to signal the presence of an exogenous pathogen via alteration of individual fecal odor. Furthermore, recognition of these signals by brood mates may be linked to social interactions and has the potential to influence pathogen transmission.

Acknowledgments

All the authors read and approved the final manuscript, with the exception of Dr. Yamazaki who passed away suddenly on 11 April 2013. Most importantly, our friend and colleague Dr. Yamazaki actively contributed to the design and realization of this work. Mention of specific products does not constitute endorsement by the United States Department of Agriculture, Colorado State University, or Monell Chemical Senses Center.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: BAK KY RAB GKB. Performed the experiments: KY DK JPM MO. Analyzed the data: BAK KY GKB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: DK RAB JPM. Wrote the paper: BAK DK GKB.

References

  1. 1. Distelheim IH, Dravnieks A (1973) A Method for Separating Characteristics of Odors in Detection of Disease Processes I. International Journal of Dermatology. 12: 241–244.
  2. 2. Liddell K (1976) Smell As A Diagnostic Marker. Postgraduate Medical Journal 52: 136–138.
  3. 3. Penn D, Potts WK (1998) Chemical signals and parasite-mediated sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13: 391–396.
  4. 4. Shirasu M, Touhara K (2011) The scent of disease: volatile organic compounds of the human body related to disease and disorder. Journal of Biochemistry 150: 257–266.
  5. 5. Syhre M, Manning L, Phuanukoonnon S, Harino P, Chambers ST (2009) The scent of Mycobacterium tuberculosis - Part II breath. Tuberculosis 89: 263–266.
  6. 6. Moser E, McCulloch M (2010) Canine scent detection of human cancers: A review of methods and accuracy. Journal of Veterinary Behavior-Clinical Applications and Research 5: 145–152.
  7. 7. Kavaliers M, Choleris E, Agmo A, Muglia LJ, et al. (2005) Involvement of the oxytocin gene in the recognition and avoidance of parasitized males by female mice. Animal Behaviour 70: 693–702.
  8. 8. Kavaliers M, Colwell DD, Braun WJ, Choleris E (2003) Brief exposure to the odour of a parasitized male alters the subsequent mate odour responses of female mice. Animal Behaviour 65: 59–68.
  9. 9. Kavaliers M, Colwell DD (1995) Odors of Parasitized Males Induce Aversive Responses in Female Mice. Animal Behaviour 50: 1161–1169.
  10. 10. Penn D, Schneider G, White K, Slev P, Potts W (1998) Influenza infection neutralizes the attractiveness of male odour to female mice (Mus musculus). Ethology 104: 685–694.
  11. 11. Yamazaki K, Boyse EA, Bard J, Curran M, Kim D, et al. (2002) Presence of mouse mammary tumor virus alters the body odor of mice. PNAS 99: 5612–5615.
  12. 12. Garner CE, Smith S, Costello BD, White P, Spencer R, et al. (2007) Volatile organic compounds from feces and their potential for diagnosis of gastrointestinal disease. FASEB Journal 21: 1675–1688.
  13. 13. Kavaliers M, Choleris E, Pfaff DW (2005) Genes, odours and the recognition of parasitized individuals by rodents. Trends in Parasitology 21: 423–429.
  14. 14. Lacroix R, Mukabana WR, Gouagna LC, Koella JC (2005) Malaria infection increases attractiveness of humans to mosquitoes. Plos Biology 3: 1590–1593.
  15. 15. Jourdain E, Gunnarsson G, Wahlgren J, Latorre-Margalef N, Brojer C, et al. (2010) Influenza Virus in a Natural Host, the Mallard: Experimental Infection Data. PLoS One 5: e8935.
  16. 16. Albone ES (1984) Mamalian Semiochemistry. New York: John Wiley and Sons. 360 p.
  17. 17. Rajchard J (2008) Exogenous chemical substances in bird perception: a review. Veterinarni Medicina 53: 412–419.
  18. 18. Amo L, Galvan I, Tomas G, Sanz JJ (2008) Predator odour recognition and avoidance in a songbird. Functional Ecology 22: 289–293.
  19. 19. Beauchamp GK, Yamazaki K, Curran M, Bard J, Boyse EA (1994) Fetal H-2 Odortypes Are Evident in the Urine of Pregnant Female Mice. Immunogenetics 39: 109–113.
  20. 20. Beauchamp GK, Katahira K, Yamazaki K, Mennella JA, Bard J, et al. (1995) Evidence Suggesting That the Odortypes of Pregnant-Women Are A Compound of Maternal and Fetal Odortypes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 92: 2617–2621.
  21. 21. Matsumura K, Opiekun M, Oka H, Vachani A, Albelda SM, et al. (2010) Urinary Volatile Compounds as Biomarkers for Lung Cancer: A Proof of Principle Study Using Odor Signatures in Mouse Models of Lung Cancer. PLoS One 5: e8819.
  22. 22. Osada K, Yamazaki K, Curran M, Bard J, Smith BPC, et al. (2003) The scent of age. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270: 929–933.
  23. 23. Kwak J, Willse A, Matsumura K, Curran Opiekun M, Yi W, et al.. (2008) Genetically-based olfactory signatures persist despite dietary variation. PLoS One e3591.
  24. 24. Yamazaki K, Boyse EA, Bard J, Curran M, Kim D, et al. (2002) Presence of mouse mammary tumor virus alters the body odor of mice. PNAS 99: 5612–5615.
  25. 25. Carriere KC (2001) How good is a normal approximation for rates and proportions of low incidence events? Communications in Statistics-Simulation and Computation 30: 327–337.
  26. 26. Lommen A (2009) MetAlign: Interface-Driven, Versatile Metabolomics Tool for Hyphenated Full-Scan Mass Spectrometry Data Preprocessing. Analytical Chemistry 81: 3079–3086.
  27. 27. Garner CE, Smith S, Costello BD, White P, Spencer R, et al. (2007) Volatile organic compounds from feces and their potential for diagnosis of gastrointestinal disease. FASEB Journal 21: 1675–1688.
  28. 28. Xiao ZJ, Qiao SL, Ma CQ, Xu P (2010) Acetoin production associated with the increase of cell biomass in Bacillus pumilus ATCC 14884. African Journal of Microbiology Research 4: 1997–2003.
  29. 29. Murphy J, Devane ML, Robson B, Gilpin BJ (2005) Genotypic characterization of bacteria cultured from duck faeces. Journal of Applied Microbiology 99: 301–309.
  30. 30. Spizizen J (1957) The Effect of Virus Infection on Pyruvate Metabolism. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta 23: 333–341.
  31. 31. Ritter JB, Wahl AS, Freund S, Genzel Y, Reichl U (2010) Metabolic effects of influenza virus infection in cultured animal cells: Intra- and extracellular metabolite profiling. Bmc Systems Biology 4: 61.
  32. 32. Kavaliers M, Choleris E, Pfaff DW (2005) Recognition and avoidance of the odors of parasitized conspecifics and predators: Differential genomic correlates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29: 1347–1359.
  33. 33. Hutchings MR, Kyriazakis I, Anderson DH, Gordon IJ, Coop RL (1998) Behavioural strategies used by parasitized and non-parasitized sheep to avoid ingestion of gastro-intestinal nematodes associated with faeces. Animal Science 67: 97–106.