Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Prevalence, Comorbidity and Heritability of Hoarding Symptoms in Adolescence: A Population Based Twin Study in 15-Year Olds

  • Volen Z. Ivanov ,

    volen.ivanov@ki.se

    Affiliations Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden, Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

  • David Mataix-Cols,

    Affiliation Departments of Psychosis Studies and Psychology, King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, London, England

  • Eva Serlachius,

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

  • Paul Lichtenstein,

    Affiliation Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

  • Henrik Anckarsäter,

    Affiliation Centre for Ethics Law and Mental Health, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden

  • Zheng Chang,

    Affiliation Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

  • Clara Hellner Gumpert,

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

  • Sebastian Lundström,

    Affiliations Centre for Ethics Law and Mental Health, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden, Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Institution of Neuroscience and Physiology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden

  • Niklas Långström,

    Affiliation Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

  • Christian Rück

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

Abstract

Background

Hoarding Disorder (HD) is often assumed to be an ‘old age’ problem, but many individuals diagnosed with HD retrospectively report first experiencing symptoms in childhood or adolescence. We examined the prevalence, comorbidity and etiology of hoarding symptoms in adolescence.

Methods

To determine the presence of clinically significant hoarding symptoms, a population-based sample of 15-year old twins (N = 3,974) completed the Hoarding Rating Scale-Self Report. Co-occurring Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD), Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) were estimated from parental report. Model-fitting analyses divided hoarding symptom scores into additive genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental effects.

Results

The prevalence of clinically significant hoarding symptoms was 2% (95% CI 1.6–2.5%), with a significantly higher prevalence in girls than boys. Exclusion of the clutter criterion (as adolescents do not have control over their environment) increased the prevalence rate to 3.7% (95% CI 3.1–4.3%). Excessive acquisition was reported by 30–40% among those with clinically significant hoarding symptoms. The prevalence of co-occurring OCD (2.9%), ASD (2.9%) and ADHD (10.0%) was comparable in hoarding and non-hoarding teenagers. Model-fitting analyses suggested that, in boys, additive genetic (32%; 95% CI 13–44%) and non-shared environmental effects accounted for most of the variance. In contrast, among girls, shared and non-shared environmental effects explained most of the variance, while additive genetic factors played a negligible role.

Conclusions

Hoarding symptoms are relatively prevalent in adolescents, particularly in girls, and cause distress and/or impairment. Hoarding was rarely associated with other common neurodevelopmental disorders, supporting its DSM-5 status as an independent diagnosis. The relative importance of genetic and shared environmental factors for hoarding differed across sexes. The findings are suggestive of dynamic developmental genetic and environmental effects operating from adolescence onto adulthood.

Introduction

Hoarding has been defined as the acquisition of and failure to discard a large number of possessions, difficulties using living spaces due to clutter and significant impairment and/or emotional distress due to the hoarding behavior [1]. Hoarding can be a serious health problem and may result in social isolation, family burden, eviction, and even death in extreme cases [2]. Although traditionally studied in the context of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD), mounting research indicates that hoarding is in fact seldom OCD-related [3]. The idea that hoarding might be a separate psychopathological entity has led to the inclusion of a new disorder in DSM-5 named Hoarding Disorder (HD) [4]. The diagnostic criteria for HD have been empirically tested and found to be valid, reliable and perceived as useful and acceptable by both professionals and sufferers alike [5], [6].

Hoarding is often considered a problem of old age; most clinically referred samples consist of adults or older adults. However, anecdotal evidence and some retrospective studies suggest that the origins of hoarding may be in childhood or adolescence [7], [8]. Indeed, many patients report specific events in childhood (such as parents forcefully discarding possessions) as the origin of their current problematic behavior [9]. HD is thought to follow a chronic and progressively deteriorating course over time, with a retrospectively reported onset between ages 11 and 15 [8]. In adults, hoarding may be highly prevalent with estimates of point prevalence ranging from 2% to almost 6% [10], [11]. The actual prevalence of hoarding symptoms during childhood and adolescence in the general population remains unknown. Because parental control over the living space may limit the extent and consequences of hoarding in young people, any prevalence estimates should take this into account.

The most commonly reported psychiatric comorbidities of HD are Major Depressive Disorder, Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Social Phobia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), whereas OCD is less frequently associated with hoarding [6], [12]. Several studies have examined the presence and correlates of hoarding symptoms among children and adolescents with OCD [13][15]. Hoarding in children with OCD is associated with lower insight and higher levels of externalizing disorders compared to children with OCD who do not hoard [16]. Hoarding symptoms have also been described in clinical samples of youth and adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) [17] and ADHD [18]. However, it seems important to establish to what extent hoarding symptoms in childhood/adolescence are associated with these common neuropsychiatric disorders in the general population, free from the selection biases involved with clinically ascertained samples.

Research into the etiology of HD is still in its infancy. Uncontrolled clinical studies suggest that hoarding runs in families, [19][21] and twin studies indicate that approximately 50% of the liability to hoarding symptoms can be attributed to genetic factors whereas unique (or non-shared) environmental factors explain the remaining variance [10], [22], [23]. While the largest twin study was conducted in adult females [10], [22], Taylor et al. [23] found no evidence of gender differences in the heritability of hoarding. However, it is well known that heritability estimates of behavioral traits can change substantially with age, and that gender differences in heritability patterns may be present in adolescents but not in adults [24][27]. Thus, data on the heritability of hoarding symptoms in younger people could potentially increase our knowledge of the dynamic etiological factors that may be at play across the lifespan.

The first aim of this study was to elucidate prevalence and comorbidity of hoarding symptoms in a large twin sample of adolescents. The second aim was to estimate the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors to hoarding symptoms and to test whether sex-differences in heritability patterns exist in this age group.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 15-year old monozygotic (MZ) or dizygotic (DZ) twins enrolled in the Swedish Twin Registry and taking part in the ongoing Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS). CATSS is a prospective, longitudinal study of all twins born in Sweden since 1992 whose parents were first contacted and interviewed when twins reached the age of 9 or 12 years [28]. We used data on hoarding symptoms from the follow-up at age 15, when twins themselves were contacted and asked to fill out a measure of hoarding symptoms. This age group is ideal to study the development of hoarding symptoms given the available retrospective accounts of adolescent symptom onset given by adult patients with HD [8], [29].

The current study sample is comprised of (1) twins who, according to parental interviews, screened negative for neurodevelopmental disorders at age 9/12 (n = 3,713) and (2) twins from the same cohort, who screened positive for a neurodevelopmental disorder and age-matched random healthy twins (n = 261). Twins from the latter group were invited to participate in a clinical face-to-face examination at age 15 and hoarding symptoms were captured with the same questionnaire but in connection to the clinical examination. Twin zygosity was determined either by using single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) [30] or (if DNA was unavailable) an algorithm based on twin similarity with predictive value greater than 95% compared to DNA testing [31].

Ethical approval was obtained from the Regional Ethics Review Board in Stockholm under contracts Dnr 02–289, 2009–793 and 03–672. In accordance with the board’s decision and Swedish regulations, written informed consent was obtained directly from the 15-year olds involved in this study and not from their parents. However, parents were informed about the study and what was being collected. Parents provided written informed consent regarding their own participation in the parental interviews when twins were 9 and 12 years old.

A total of 3,974 participants (response rate = 47% of all Swedish twins in the age group) were included in the current study. Of these, 45% (n = 1,805) were boys and 55% (n = 2,169) girls. Furthermore, 30% (n = 1,210) were monozygotic (MZ) twins, 30% (n = 1,119) dizygotic same-sex (DZ), 26% (n = 1,022) dizygotic opposite sex (DZOS) twins and 14% (n = 543) had unknown zygosity. There were no significant differences between participants who responded to the hoarding questionnaire in the CATSS-battery compared to those who did not (n = 218) in terms of dwelling area (in both groups, 69% lived in a city rather than a rural area), having a mother born abroad (9% vs.10%, respectively), a father born abroad (8% vs. 11%), parental income, or paternal educational level. However, a larger proportion of mothers to responders had a university education compared to non-responders (47 vs. 39%, z = −2.12, df = 1, p<0.05). Co-occurring OCD at age 9/12 was also equally common in both groups (1% among responders vs. 3% among non-responders; χ2 = , 2.00, df = 1, p = 0.20), whereas more non-responders screened for ASD (2% vs. 6%; χ2 = 8.62, df = 1, p<0.01) and ADHD (7% versus 17%; χ2 = 22.28, df = 1, p<0.001). Further, a larger proportion of the responders were girls (55% vs. 45%; χ2 = 5.05, df = 1, p<0.05), and monozygotic (35% vs. 27%; χ2 = 4.71, df = 1, p<0.05) than non-responders.

Measures

All study participants filled out the Hoarding Rating Scale-Self Report (HRS-SR) [32]. The HRS-SR consists of five items measured on a 9-point Likert type scale ranging from 0 (none) to 8 (extreme). The HRS-SR items reflect the proposed DSM-5 criteria for HD: clutter in the rooms of one’s home, difficulty discarding possessions, excessive acquisition and perceived distress and impairment. To address adolescents’ limited control over their entire homes and parental control, we rephrased the clutter item in the questionnaire to refer solely to clutter in the young person’s own room (rather than the entire home).

In the current study, the HRS-SR displayed acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.70) and a principal component analysis revealed a single factor structure explaining 46.2% of the variance, with factor loadings ranging from 0.65 (clutter in room) to 0.71 (emotional distress).

Presence of clinically significant hoarding symptoms was defined as scoring at least moderately (4 or more out of 8) on items measuring clutter, difficulties discarding and distress and/or impairment. This has previously been described as criteria for clinically significant hoarding [33], [34] and approximately corresponds to the proposed core criteria for DSM-5 [3]. Although it is currently unclear whether the DSM-5 diagnostic criteria are suitable for adolescents [35], a recent taxometric exploration of the latent structure of hoarding symptoms [36] supported a similar dimensional structure of hoarding in adults and young adults (mean age = 19). However, since the phenomenology of hoarding in adolescence is largely unknown, we also calculated an additional prevalence estimate. For this, we used the above-described criteria but excluded the clutter criterion since clutter, or the lack thereof, could be heavily influenced by familial factors, such as parents or siblings discarding items. Additionally, since excessive acquisition is a prominent feature of hoarding (but not a core criterion in DSM-5) we also calculated the proportion of participants reporting excessive acquisition among those endorsing clinically significant hoarding symptoms.

Co-occurring neurodevelopmental disorders were assessed at age 9/12 with the ‘Autism-Tics, AD/HD and other Comorbidities inventory’ (A-TAC) [37], [38] specifically developed for parental interviewing in CATSS. Parents to twins underwent telephone interviews answering questions regarding lifetime symptoms of ADHD and ASD closely mapped after the corresponding DSM-IV-TR-criteria [39]. The A-TAC is structured in different modules, which in turn consist of several items: Language (6 items), Social interaction (6 items), and Flexibility (5 items) for ASD and Concentration/Attention (9 items) and Impulsiveness/Activity (10 items) for ADHD. In each module, parents are first reminded that the items refer to a life-time perspective and subsequently asked to answer either by stating No (Score 0), Yes, to some extent (Score 0.5) or Yes (Score 1). Additionally, parents are given the alternatives Do not know or Do not wish to answer. To be defined as screening positive for ASD in this study, a cut-off score of 4.5 was required. Conversely, for ADHD, a cut-off score of 6 was used. These cut-offs have been validated and provide high sensitivity/specificity (0.91/0.80) for ASD and (0.91/0.73) ADHD as well as excellent inter-rater agreement (ICC>0.90) [38]. Furthermore, predictive validity, measured by receiver operating characteristics (ROC) is excellent with areas under the curve (AUC) of 0.96 for ASD and 0.94 for ADHD. OCD symptoms were ascertained with two A-TAC items, modeled after DSM-IV-TR-criteria for OCD, addressing obsessions and compulsions. Examples of typical obsessions concerning dirt, contamination or catastrophes and compulsions consisting of washing, touching, repeating, arranging or counting were provided. The cut-off score for OCD was determined to 1.

Statistical Analyses

Internal consistency and factor analysis of the HRS-SR as well as prevalence estimates and demographic differences were estimated using SAS version 9.2 [40]. For continuous variables we used t-tests, whereas ordinal and nominal data were analyzed with χ2-tests (Fischer’s exact test) and Wilcoxon’s test. In the prevalence estimates, we controlled for the clustering of twins within families. All parameter estimates in the twin analyses were obtained from maximum-likelihood estimates in MX [41].

Twin Analyses

Twin analyses are based on the differing genetic relatedness of MZ twins, sharing all of their genes and DZ twins, which share on average 50% of their co-segregating genes. The assumption that both twin types grow up under equal environments leads to the conclusion that an accentuated resemblance within MZ compared to DZ pairs on a given trait is due to a genetic influence on that trait. To estimate the magnitude of this within-twin pair resemblance, we calculated intraclass correlations for HRS-SR total scores by zygosity. Since total scores were positively skewed (skewness = 1.55), they were successfully logarithmically transformed using the natural logarithm (skewness = −0.14). Next, we undertook maximum-likelihood univariate model-fitting [42] and fitted our raw data to different models to identify that with the best fit by comparing the chi-square statistic of the −2 Log likelihood (−2LL) statistics. Twin models decompose the variance of phenotypes to additive genetic factors (A), shared environmental factors (C; which make twins within a pair alike), and non-shared environmental factors (E; which make twins within a pair dissimilar). The correlational pattern showed that the MZ twins were more alike compared to DZ twins among males but not among females, suggesting possible sex differences in genetic and environmental influences on hoarding symptoms. Therefore we fitted several sex-limitation models to our data. Initially, however, data was fitted to the saturated model where variances and means of the first and second born MZ and DZ twin were allowed to vary freely. Next, we fitted data to the General Sex Limitation Model (GSM) [42] which allows for qualitative and quantitative sex effects on a trait (i.e., we tested whether the same genes influenced hoarding symptoms in both sexes and if the magnitude of this genetic effect was equal across sexes). Additionally, the genetic correlation between DZOS twins was allowed to vary freely and so were the effects of A, C and E on males and females. We also tested a similar model where the shared environmental correlation in DZOS twins was allowed to vary freely to test if qualitative differences in the shared environment could be influential across sexes. In striving for parsimony by using as few parameters in a model as possible, we subsequently tested the more restrained Common Effects Sex Limitation Model (CESM) [42] and compared this to the full GSM. In CESM only the magnitude of the genetic effect is allowed to vary whereas the genes affecting the trait are common to both sexes by restraining the DZOS genetic correlation to 0.5. Finally, we also tested a No Sex Effects Model (NESM) [42] in which A, C and E effects are all restrained to be equally influential on both sexes and compared this to the CESM.

Results

Prevalence

A total of 2% (N = 79; 95% CI 1.6%−2.5%; Mean HRS-SR score = 19.7; min = 12, max = 40; SD = 4.9) of the entire sample of 15-year olds had clinically significant hoarding symptomatology based on the above-described criteria. The prevalence was significantly higher in girls (2.6%; 95% CI 2.0%−3.5%) than in boys (1.2%; 95% CI 0.8%−1.8%) (χ2 = 15.5, df = 1, p<0.01). After excluding the clutter criterion, the prevalence rate increased to 3.7% (N = 145; 95% CI 3.1%−4.3%; Mean HRS-SR score = 17.0; min = 8, max = 40; SD = 5.3).

Excessive acquisition was endorsed by 38% (n = 30) of participants with clinically significant hoarding symptoms (including clutter) and by 28% (n = 41) of participants when the clutter criterion was excluded from the prevalence estimates.

Demographic Characteristics and Comorbidity

Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of hoarding and non-hoarding groups are shown in Table 1. Adolescents with hoarding symptoms were more likely to be female (72% vs. 54%). The two groups were similar regarding urban area of living, parental education, income and immigrant status. Among hoarders, ADHD was the most common co-occurring disorder at age 9/12 (10.0%) whereas OCD and ASD were each comorbid in 2.9% of the cases. However, no significant differences emerged between the hoarding and the non-hoarding groups in terms of comorbidity.

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of 15-year old twins in Sweden fulfilling criteria for clinically significant hoarding symptoms according to the HRS-SR and those who did not.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069140.t001

Heritability

In total, 1555 twin pairs were included in the model-fitting analyses. The intraclass correlations for HRS-SR hoarding symptoms are provided in Table 2. In MZ boys, the correlation (0.44; 95% CI 0.33–54) was significantly higher than the DZ correlation (0.17; 95% CI 0.06–0.29), indicating that genes had an influence on hoarding among males. In contrast, we found no significant difference in MZ (0.35; 95% CI 0.25–0.44) and DZ (0.41; 95% CI 0.31–0.51) correlations among girls, suggesting that familial similarity regarding could be explained by shared environment rather than genes.

thumbnail
Table 2. Intraclass correlations for hoarding symptoms according to the HRS-SR in Swedish twins at age 15 years.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069140.t002

Table 3 provides model-fitting results. Compared to the initial saturated model, the GSM did not provide a significantly worse fit (p = 0.88). Similarly, testing the more parsimonious CESM did not result in a worsening of fit compared to the GSM (p = 0.99), suggesting no qualitative genetic/shared environmental sex differences (i.e., the same genes/shared environments are important for boys and girls). However, in comparison to the CESM the NESM, in which genetic and environmental influences are equated across sexes, resulted in a significant deterioration of the fit (p = 0.02). Thus, the model with the best fit was the CESM, indicating that the same genes/shared environments accounted for the variation in hoarding symptoms in both adolescent males and females, but that the magnitude of this influence varied across sexes.

thumbnail
Table 3. Model-fitting results for hoarding symptoms in Swedish twins at age 15 years.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069140.t003

Table 4 summarizes the standardized parameter estimates of the best-fitting model (i.e. the CESM). The figures indicated that in boys, genes accounted for 32% (95% CI 13–44%) of the phenotypic variance in hoarding symptoms whereas shared environmental factors (4%; 95% CI 0–17%) only had a negligible effect. Non-shared environmental effects and measurement error (64%; 95% CI 55–75%) accounted for the largest proportion of the variance. By contrast, genes only accounted for a non-significant 2% (95% CI 0–24%) of the variance in girls, whereas the shared environment (32%; 95% CI 14–41%) and the non-shared environment plus measurement error (65%; 95% CI 58–73%) accounted for most of the phenotypic variance.

thumbnail
Table 4. Explained variance by additive genetic and environmental factors to hoarding symptoms in Swedish twins at age 15 years according to best fitting model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069140.t004

Discussion

As proposed by retrospective studies of adults [8], [9] and studies of children with OCD exhibiting hoarding symptoms [13][15] our study suggests that hoarding symptoms are relatively prevalent (approximately 2%) among adolescents although somewhat less prevalent than in similar adult studies [11], [43]. Mean scores on HRS-SR among young hoarders (mean = 19.7; sd = 4.9, range = 12–40) in the current study were also similar to scores reported in adults (mean = 21.1; sd = 3.9) [11]. However, a notable difference between hoarding symptoms in adolescence and adulthood is that they seem to affect females to a larger extent in the younger age group, a finding that also has been reported earlier [15]. An additional difference emerged when examining the frequency of excessive acquisition amongst individuals classed as having clinically significant hoarding difficulties. Approximately 30–40% of these individuals also reported at least moderate problems with acquisition, indicating that excessive acquisition may be less prominent in adolescents than it is in adults (about 85%; [44]). This lends further support to the inclusion of excessive acquisition as a diagnostic specifier rather than a core criterion in DSM−5. It is possible that excessive acquisition difficulties develop later in life when individuals with hoarding tendencies become financially independent and also have more control over their acquisitive behavior and space.

The obtained prevalence estimate of hoarding symptoms also indicates that these are at least as common as OCD during adolescence [45], [46], a finding that contradicts the notion that hoarding is merely is a subtype of OCD.

Given the lack of published data on the hoarding phenotype in adolescence and the potential of familial effects on clutter we also estimated a prevalence rate of 3.7% when excluding the requirement to have significant clutter in the participant’s room. It is still unknown whether the phenomenology of hoarding changes across the lifespan and whether the proposed diagnostic criteria for HD can be applied to adolescents without adaptation. Further exploration of the phenomenology of hoarding during adolescence involving face-to-face interviews and home visits will be needed in order to determine which features of hoarding are most prominent and problematic during this age period and whether the diagnostic criteria require adapting for their use in adolescents. Nevertheless, our estimates provide an important first step towards estimating the possible prevalence of hoarding difficulties in this age group.

Our findings also showed that OCD, to which HD has been linked traditionally, co-occurred in only 2.9% of adolescents who fulfilled hoarding criteria and at a similar rate among those who did not. This finding from a non-clinical sample suggests that the link between the two disorders might be especially tenuous during adolescence. Previous findings of high comorbidity between HD and ADHD in clinically ascertained samples of adults [12], [47] and youth [18] were not confirmed. We also found comparable rates of ASD in the hoarding and non-hoarding groups. Similarly, research in adults suggests that symptoms of autism are not more prevalent in subjects with HD compared to psychiatric controls [17]. Taken together, our findings suggest that in the majority of cases at the population level, hoarding symptoms are frequently present in the absence of other neurodevelopmental disorders, lending further support to the notion that HD might be a distinct nosological entity.

Finally, the twin analyses indicated that hoarding symptoms were moderately heritable in adolescence, although the magnitude of the genetic influence was much stronger in boys than in girls. This somewhat unexpected finding – hoarding symptoms are clearly heritable in adult men and women [10], [23] – was related to considerable shared environmental influences on hoarding symptoms amongst girls. However, similar findings of sex differences during adolescence have been reported previously regarding genetic and environmental influences for OCD, [48], [49] weight [50], and pubertal development [51]. Furthermore, dynamic developmental genetic effects from childhood to young adulthood, expressed in genetic attenuation and innovation (i.e. the decline in impact of certain genes and the activation of genes which previously had no effect) in symptoms of anxiety and depression are well documented [26]. It seems plausible that hoarding may become progressively more heritable over time as the influence of shared familial environment decreases when young people leave their parents’ home and have stronger control over their own living space. Longitudinal studies of the heritability of hoarding symptoms in young adulthood and beyond are needed to elucidate developmental trajectories of hoarding in both sexes.

The results should be interpreted considering several limitations. First, we based our classification of clinically significant hoarding symptoms on a measure not specifically validated in an adolescent population. Thus, the possibility that it did not capture hoarding symptoms equally well as in adults cannot be ruled out. Hopefully, the modification of the clutter item increased its relevance for this age group. Second, prevalence estimates should be seen as indicative rather than definitive because we could not conclusively rule out other medical or psychiatric conditions that are known to lead to hoarding behavior [52], [53]. Third, although ADHD, ASD and OCD are relatively stable prevalence-wise in the age span from 9/12 to 15 years, more precise estimates of their co-occurrence with hoarding symptoms would have been obtained had they been assessed at age 15 years and not at age 9/12; hence, we cannot totally exclude an under- or possibly overestimation of ADHD, ASD and OCD comorbidity. Fourth, albeit modeled closely after DSM-IV-TR-criteria [39], comorbid OCD was not determined using a validated measure, and was based solely on parental report. Thus, the OCD comorbidity rate might have been underestimated by parents and should therefore be interpreted cautiously. Fifth, and finally, since ASD and ADHD were significantly more common among non-responders, we cannot rule out that the true comorbidity of hoarding symptoms and neurodevelopmental disorders might be higher.

Conclusion

This study is the first to investigate the occurrence of hoarding symptoms in a large population based sample of adolescents. Hoarding symptoms were prevalent among adolescents and usually appeared without co-occurring ADHD, ASD and OCD. Furthermore, the same etiological factors seemed to influence hoarding symptoms in both sexes although the genetic effect was much stronger in boys. Longitudinal studies are required to elucidate the developmental trajectories of hoarding symptoms and their heritability from adolescence onto adulthood.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Camilla Palm at the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet for assistance in data management and preparation.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: DMC PL HA ZC CHG SL NL CR ES. Analyzed the data: VZI DMC PL ZC CR. Wrote the paper: VZI DMC ES PL HA ZC CHG SL NL CR.

References

  1. 1. Frost RO, Hartl TL (1996) A cognitive-behavioral model of compulsive hoarding. Behav Res Ther 34: 341–350.
  2. 2. Steketee G, Frost R (2003) Compulsive hoarding: current status of the research. Clin Psychol Rev 23: 905–927.
  3. 3. Mataix-Cols D, Frost RO, Pertusa A, Clark LA, Saxena S, et al. (2010) Hoarding disorder: a new diagnosis for DSM-V? Depress Anxiety 27: 556–572.
  4. 4. American Psychiatric Association, DSM−5 Development. Available: http://www.dsm5.org/.
  5. 5. Mataix-Cols D, de la Cruz LF, Nakao T, Pertusa A (2011) Testing the validity and acceptability of the diagnostic criteria for Hoarding Disorder: a DSM−5 survey. Psychol Med: 1–10.
  6. 6. Mataix-Cols D, Billotti D, Fernandez de la Cruz L, Nordsletten AE (2012) The London field trial for hoarding disorder. Psychol Med: 1–11.
  7. 7. Grisham JR, Frost RO, Steketee G, Kim HJ, Hood S (2006) Age of onset of compulsive hoarding. J Anxiety Disord 20: 675–686.
  8. 8. Tolin DF, Meunier SA, Frost RO, Steketee G (2010) Course of compulsive hoarding and its relationship to life events. Depress Anxiety 27: 829–838.
  9. 9. Landau D, Iervolino AC, Pertusa A, Santo S, Singh S, et al. (2011) Stressful life events and material deprivation in hoarding disorder. J Anxiety Disord 25: 192–202.
  10. 10. Iervolino AC, Perroud N, Fullana MA, Guipponi M, Cherkas L, et al. (2009) Prevalence and heritability of compulsive hoarding: a twin study. Am J Psychiatry 166: 1156–1161.
  11. 11. Timpano KR, Exner C, Glaesmer H, Rief W, Keshaviah A, et al. (2011) The epidemiology of the proposed DSM-5 hoarding disorder: exploration of the acquisition specifier, associated features, and distress. J Clin Psychiatry 72: 780–786; quiz 878–789.
  12. 12. Frost RO, Steketee G, Tolin DF (2011) Comorbidity in hoarding disorder. Depress Anxiety 28: 876–884.
  13. 13. Leonard HL, Goldberger EL, Rapoport JL, Cheslow DL, Swedo SE (1990) Childhood rituals: normal development or obsessive-compulsive symptoms? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29: 17–23.
  14. 14. Hanna GL (1995) Demographic and clinical features of obsessive-compulsive disorder in children and adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 34: 19–27.
  15. 15. Mataix-Cols D, Nakatani E, Micali N, Heyman I (2008) Structure of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in pediatric OCD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47: 773–778.
  16. 16. Storch EA, Lack CW, Merlo LJ, Geffken GR, Jacob ML, et al. (2007) Clinical features of children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder and hoarding symptoms. Compr Psychiatry 48: 313–318.
  17. 17. Pertusa A, Bejerot S, Eriksson J, Fernandez de la Cruz L, Bonde S, et al. (2012) Do patients with hoarding disorder have autistic traits? Depress Anxiety 29: 210–218.
  18. 18. Hacker LE, Park JM, Timpano KR, Cavitt MA, Alvaro JL, et al. (2012) Hoarding in Children With ADHD. J Atten Disord.
  19. 19. Pertusa A, Fullana MA, Singh S, Alonso P, Menchon JM, et al. (2008) Compulsive hoarding: OCD symptom, distinct clinical syndrome, or both? Am J Psychiatry 165: 1289–1298.
  20. 20. Seedat S, Stein DJ (2002) Hoarding in obsessive-compulsive disorder and related disorders: a preliminary report of 15 cases. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 56: 17–23.
  21. 21. Winsberg ME, Cassic KS, Koran LM (1999) Hoarding in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a report of 20 cases. J Clin Psychiatry 60: 591–597.
  22. 22. Iervolino AC, Rijsdijk FV, Cherkas L, Fullana MA, Mataix-Cols D (2011) A multivariate twin study of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68: 637–644.
  23. 23. Taylor S, Jang KL, Asmundson GJ (2010) Etiology of obsessions and compulsions: a behavioral-genetic analysis. J Abnorm Psychol 119: 672–682.
  24. 24. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Annas P, Neale MC, Eaves LJ, et al. (2008) A longitudinal twin study of fears from middle childhood to early adulthood: evidence for a developmentally dynamic genome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65: 421–429.
  25. 25. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Annas P, Lichtenstein P (2008) The development of fears from early adolesence to young adulthood: a multivariate study. Psychol Med 38: 1759–1769.
  26. 26. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Lichtenstein P (2008) A developmental twin study of symptoms of anxiety and depression: evidence for genetic innovation and attenuation. Psychol Med 38: 1567–1575.
  27. 27. Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (2002) Sex differences in the genetic and environmental influences on the development of antisocial behavior. Dev Psychopathol 14: 395–416.
  28. 28. Anckarsater H, Lundstrom S, Kollberg L, Kerekes N, Palm C, et al. (2011) The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS). Twin Res Hum Genet 14: 495–508.
  29. 29. Ayers CR, Saxena S, Golshan S, Wetherell JL (2010) Age at onset and clinical features of late life compulsive hoarding. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 25: 142–149.
  30. 30. Hannelius U, Gherman L, Makela VV, Lindstedt A, Zucchelli M, et al. (2007) Large-scale zygosity testing using single nucleotide polymorphisms. Twin Res Hum Genet 10: 604–625.
  31. 31. Lichtenstein P, Sullivan PF, Cnattingius S, Gatz M, Johansson S, et al. (2006) The Swedish Twin Registry in the third millennium: an update. Twin Res Hum Genet 9: 875–882.
  32. 32. Tolin DF, Frost RO, Steketee G (2010) A brief interview for assessing compulsive hoarding: the Hoarding Rating Scale-Interview. Psychiatry Res 178: 147–152.
  33. 33. Tolin DF, Frost RO, Steketee G, Gray KD, Fitch KE (2008) The economic and social burden of compulsive hoarding. Psychiatry Res 160: 200–211.
  34. 34. Frost RO, Hristova V (2011) Assessment of hoarding. J Clin Psychol 67: 456–466.
  35. 35. Mataix-Cols D, Pertusa A (2012) Annual Research Review: Hoarding disorder: potential benefits and pitfalls of a new mental disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 53: 608–618.
  36. 36. Timpano KR, Broman-Fulks JJ, Glaesmer H, Exner C, Rief W, et al. (2012) A Taxometric Exploration of the Latent Structure of Hoarding. Psychol Assess.
  37. 37. Hansson SL, Svanstrom Rojvall A, Rastam M, Gillberg C, Anckarsater H (2005) Psychiatric telephone interview with parents for screening of childhood autism - tics, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and other comorbidities (A-TAC): preliminary reliability and validity. Br J Psychiatry 187: 262–267.
  38. 38. Larson T, Anckarsater H, Gillberg C, Stahlberg O, Carlstrom E, et al. (2010) The autism–tics, AD/HD and other comorbidities inventory (A-TAC): further validation of a telephone interview for epidemiological research. BMC Psychiatry 10: 1.
  39. 39. American Psyciatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press.
  40. 40. SAS Institute I, Cary, North Carolina SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, North Carolina. 9.2 ed.
  41. 41. Available: http://www.vcu.edu/mx/.
  42. 42. Neale MC, Cardon LR, North Atlantic Treaty Organization. Scientific Affairs Division (1992) Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht; Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. xxv, 496 p.
  43. 43. Mueller A, Mitchell JE, Crosby RD, Glaesmer H, de Zwaan M (2009) The prevalence of compulsive hoarding and its association with compulsive buying in a German population-based sample. Behav Res Ther 47: 705–709.
  44. 44. Frost RO, Tolin DF, Steketee G, Fitch KE, Selbo-Bruns A (2009) Excessive acquisition in hoarding. J Anxiety Disord 23: 632–639.
  45. 45. Flament MF, Whitaker A, Rapoport JL, Davies M, Berg CZ, et al. (1988) Obsessive compulsive disorder in adolescence: an epidemiological study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 27: 764–771.
  46. 46. Valleni-Basile LA, Garrison CZ, Jackson KL, Waller JL, McKeown RE, et al. (1994) Frequency of obsessive-compulsive disorder in a community sample of young adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 33: 782–791.
  47. 47. Tolin DF, Villavicencio A (2011) Inattention, but not OCD, predicts the core features of hoarding disorder. Behav Res Ther 49: 120–125.
  48. 48. Hur YM, Jeong HU (2008) Sex differences in genetic and environmental influences on obsessive-compulsive symptoms in South Korean adolescent and young adult twins. Twin Res Hum Genet 11: 314–320.
  49. 49. Moore J, Smith GW, Shevlin M, O’Neill FA (2010) Alternative factor models and heritability of the Short Leyton Obsessional Inventory-Children’s Version. J Abnorm Child Psychol 38: 921–934.
  50. 50. Cornes BK, Zhu G, Martin NG (2007) Sex differences in genetic variation in weight: a longitudinal study of body mass index in adolescent twins. Behav Genet 37: 648–660.
  51. 51. Mustanski BS, Viken RJ, Kaprio J, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ (2004) Genetic and environmental influences on pubertal development: longitudinal data from Finnish twins at ages 11 and 14. Dev Psychol 40: 1188–1198.
  52. 52. Chong SA, Tan CH, Lee HS (1996) Hoarding and clozapine-risperidone combination. Can J Psychiatry 41: 315–316.
  53. 53. O’Sullivan SS, Djamshidian A, Evans AH, Loane CM, Lees AJ, et al. (2010) Excessive hoarding in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 25: 1026–1033.