Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effect of Action Verbs on the Performance of a Complex Movement

  • Tahar Rabahi ,

    taharrabahi@gmail.com

    Affiliation Centre de Recherche et d’Innovation sur le Sport (CRIS, EA 647), Université de Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Villeurbanne, France

  • Patrick Fargier,

    Affiliation Centre de Recherche et d’Innovation sur le Sport (CRIS, EA 647), Université de Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Villeurbanne, France

  • Ahmad Rifai Sarraj,

    Affiliation Département de Physiothérapie, Faculté de Santé Publique-I, Université Libanaise Rafic Hariri, Hadath-Beyrouth, Lebanon

  • Cyril Clouzeau,

    Affiliation Unité de Recherche, Institut Supérieur d’Ostéopathie (ISOSTEO), Limonest-Lyon, France

  • Raphael Massarelli

    Affiliation Centre de Recherche et d’Innovation sur le Sport (CRIS, EA 647), Université de Lyon, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Villeurbanne, France

Abstract

The interaction between language and motor action has been approached by studying the effect of action verbs, kinaesthetic imagery and mental subtraction upon the performance of a complex movement, the squat vertical jump (SVJ). The time of flight gave the value of the height of the SVJ and was measured with an Optojump® and a Myotest® apparatuses. The results obtained by the effects of the cognitive stimuli showed a statistically significant improvement of the SVJ performance after either loudly or silently pronouncing, hearing or reading the verb saute (jump in French language). Action verbs specific for other motor actions (pince = pinch, lèche = lick) or non-specific (bouge = move) showed no or little effect. A meaningless verb for the French subjects (tiáo = jump in Chinese) showed no effect as did rêve (dream), tombe (fall) and stop. The verb gagne (win) improved significantly the SVJ height, as did its antonym perds (lose) suggesting a possible influence of affects in the subjects’ performance. The effect of the specific action verb jump upon the heights of SVJ was similar to that obtained after kinaesthetic imagery and after mental subtraction of two digits numbers from three digits ones; possibly, in the latter, because of the intervention of language in calculus. It appears that the effects of the specific action verb jump did seem effective but not totally exclusive for the enhancement of the SVJ performance. The results imply an interaction among language and motor brain areas in the performance of a complex movement resulting in a clear specificity of the corresponding action verb. The effect upon performance may probably be influenced by the subjects’ intention, increased attention and emotion produced by cognitive stimuli among which action verbs.

Introduction

The relation between language and motor action has been the domain of philosophy much before becoming that of neuroscience (see: [1]), but in the last decades numerous findings (e.g.: [2][6]) have reported the existence of a probable interaction among the areas involved in language and movement. This has been shown, for example, by the fMRI experiments of Hauk et al [3] showing that silent reading of action verbs lick, pick and kick might apparently activate somatotopically the cerebral motor areas corresponding, respectively, to tongue, hand and foot. Other results ([7], [8] and for review see: [9]) have supported the hypothesis that reading action verbs may create specific links between the cortical areas specialised in language, audition and motor action. For instance, during lexical decisions on words related to action, Pulvermüller et al [10] reported that these cortical areas are interconnected in a category-specific manner. In other studies it was found that reading action verbs related to the hand and the arm, facilitates the execution of a reaching movement, when compared to reading other verbs describing mouth and leg actions [11], [12]. Hearing sentences that describe hand and foot action may also modulate the corresponding effectors, as it has been verified directly by monitoring motor-evoked potentials [5]. In an fMRI experiment, listening to sentences describing actions performed with the mouth, the hand, or the leg activated a left-hemispheric fronto-parieto-temporal network, known to be activated by action execution and observation [6]. In line with these findings, Dalla Volta et al [13] found that opening the fingers of the hand in response to hearing hand-related verbs was faster than when foot-related verbs were heard; a similar effect was observed on arm velocity during reaching-grasping movements. Other experiments have confirmed that a link between action words and motor performance might indeed exist [14][17], probably through the mediation of a mental simulation process (for review see: [18], [19], vide infra). Some authors are however more cautious concerning a somatotopic, modal relationship between action verbs understanding and activation of motor areas (see: [20][23]), preferring instead a more amodal ‘grounded hypothesis’ of interaction among the concerned areas (see for review: [24], [25]).

Several hypotheses have been advanced to explain the relation of language and motor actions (see for review: [26], [27]). Some authors have suggested that gestures, in particular, may assume the role of a “primitive meaning processor” ([27], p 944) (see also: [28]), while others have advanced the possibility that the effect of words or phrases on the motor system might arise through the mental representation of a given motor action [18], [24], [29]. Mental representations may create mental images and the use of these has been well studied in the past under the term of simulation [30], [31], which was intended as the ‘mental rehearsal’ of a motor action (for reviews see: [32], [33]). Such procedure had been used by athletes and coaches in sport activities as early as the 1930’s [34], [35] and it has been renamed kinaesthetic imagery (KI) later on to clearly distinguish it from visual imagery. The latter represents the mental vision of a movement, while the former concerns the kinaesthetic sensation of the action in the absence of an actual execution [36]. The effects of KI on the amelioration of a gesture performance when executed before the actual motor action have been well documented for simple and complex motor action (for reviews see: [37], [38]). The overall research led on the subject suggests then that language, motor, audition, vision and associative areas, including Wernicke’s, form a peri-Sylvian circuitry [39] of sensory and motor neurons some of which (mirror neurons) may fulfil both functions (see for review: [9], [27], [40], [41]). This ensemble of cortical areas is partly involved also during KI (for review see: [29]).

The activation of brain functional areas, recruited when using various cognitive stimuli and found in most of the above cited studies, let suppose that the effect of language on the activation of motor areas (in particular premotor areas) may also lead to an improvement of the physical performance of an individual, i.e. having an effect upon the efficiency of a complex movement. Consequently, the experiments that will be presented were realised with the aim to observe the potential effects of action verbs upon the execution of a complex motor action commonly used in biomechanical studies, the Squat Vertical Jump (SVJ) [42]. The leading concept, in the present research, was to observe whether the reported activations solicited in the motor areas by action verbs might produce a macro-scale effect upon an action as complex as the SVJ or might instead resemble to the effect produced in simpler movements as those concerning fingers, hand or arm [11][13], [43]. The rationale was based upon results [44], [45] showing discordance in motor acquisition when actions of different complexity are executed. Wulf and Shea [44], pointing out that a ‘complex’ motor action has several degrees of freedom compared to a simpler one, have reached the expected conclusion that motor learning of simple movements is not the same as that of more complex ones, as in the case of the SVJ.

Materials and Methods

General Experimental Procedure

Action verbs were pronounced, heard or read at the second person of the imperative tense, in French, and the effects upon SVJ were studied in seven separate experiments. The possible influence of a specific action verb (saute = jump) upon the height performance of a SVJ was compared to the effect of KI, which was used here as a control for jump considering its known potentiality in improving complex motor action [46]. Possible non-specific effects were verified by: a. Mental calculus (subtractions of two digits numbers from three digits ones); b. verbs specifically describing motor actions other than jumping (lèche = lick and pince = pinch) or signifying a failing action (tombe = fall); c. verbs implying the involvement of affects (not specifying a particular movement but raising feelings and emotions in relation to the goal of the jump: gagne = win and perds = lose, or unrelated to the goal: bouge = move and rêve = dream, or contradicting the action: stop); d. Pronunciation and hearing of a Chinese verb (tiáo = jump) meaningless for French speaking subjects.

Subjects

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Claude Bernard University. Male French speaking subjects (a total of 114 students of the Faculty of Sport Sciences, University Claude Bernard Lyon 1, of the Institute of Osteopathy, ISOSTEO, Limonest-Lyon and of the Department of Physical Sciences of the Lebanese University, Beyrouth, Lebanon) were asked to participate to the experimental protocol and gave their written and informed consent. The anthropometric average characteristics of all the 114 subjects corresponded to the values for Caucasians (i.e. 104 Europeans: 23.02±2.8 years of age, 177.4±5.7 cm height and 72.3±6.4 kg weight and 10 Mediterraneans: 21.1±1.3 years, 172.9±2.8 cm height and 76.8±3.9 weight [47], [48]). The characteristics of the subjects in each experiment are shown in Table 1 and seven different groups of subjects were formed to participate to the seven experiments (one per each experiment). The subjects were briefly instructed before the execution of the experimental protocol, but they were left unaware of the expected results or of possible effects of a given action verb or of any stimulus used in the experimental protocol. The subjects performed SVJs without any previous particular expertise or training and, in this respect, they were considered naïve.

thumbnail
Table 1. Anthropometric characteristics of experimental subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068687.t001

Jumps

The jumps consisted in classical maximal SVJ, i.e.: parallel feet, heels on the ground, separated as wide as the shoulders, knees bent at 90°, hands on hips during the whole jump executed without any noticeable countermovement [42], [49].

Experimental Protocols

Before the start of each experiment the subjects were asked to perform some warm-up SVJs for about 5 minutes, to obtain a correct execution of the movement. Afterwards the participants were asked to perform a series of 6 SVJs (each series being called block) with a 3 min rest between blocks (see Figure 1). In a typical block the first 3 jumps were carried without any cognitive conditions (they were defined Baseline Jumps, BJ). The ensuing 3 jumps were executed after the cognitive conditions (lasting 10 s each; Figure 1) as specified below for each experiment. The blocks, comprising BJ and cognitive stimuli, were randomly presented to each subject.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Protocol design.

Before the start of the experiment the subjects were asked to perform some warm-up jumps, for about 5 minutes, to obtain a correct execution of the movement. The jumps consisted in classical maximal squat vertical jumps (SVJ). The heights of the jumps were measured as described in Material and Methods (section Jump). In a typical block the first 3 jumps were carried without cognitive conditions (to be called baseline jumps, BJ). The following 3 jumps were executed separately after the cognitive stimulus (repeated three times for 10 s before each jump). A rest period of 3 min was observed before a following block.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068687.g001

Note that all subjects were asked to draw a particular attention to reach, during the warm-up period, the execution of correct SVJs and the maximal height in order to avoid improper variations in the height of the motor performance in the experimental follow-up. Moreover each subject was told not to communicate with other subjects about the experimental protocol.

Apparatus and Measurement

The heights of the jumps were calculated, by measuring the time of flight, either by an OPTOJUMP® apparatus (Microgate France, 38330 St-Ismier; the apparatus was connected to a Laptop), in experiments 2 and 4, or by an accelerometer Myotest® Pro (Myotest France SAS, 84 210 Pernes les Fontaines), which was used for all other experiments.

A comparison experiment, performed on 8 subjects and using simultaneously both instruments, showed that the absolute heights measured with Myotest® might be higher (6.6 cm in the average) compared to OPTOJUMP®, a value that corresponds well to those found in the literature (Casartelli et al [50] reported values ranging from 5.7 to 6.9 cm). Insignificant differences were instead found between the two instruments in the measures of the effect of the action verb jump upon the height of the SVJ when compared, per cent wise, to BJ (when normalised to BJ, Optojump: 106.3±6.9% of height increase after pronouncing jump, n = 44, experiments 2 and 4; Myotest: 105.9±6.7%, n = 60, experiments 3, 5, 6 and 7 group effect: p = 0.76, t = - 0.30; the statistics was performed as specified below).

Experiment 1: Reference Subjects

A control experiment was performed on 10 reference subjects (see Table 1) to check the presence of a possible effect of training. This was done according to the same protocol described in Figure 1 with the exception that no cognitive task was demanded to the subjects.

Experiment 2: Pronouncing the Specific Action Verb and Comparison to Non-specific Stimuli

Twenty eight subjects (see Table 1) executed the following cognitive conditions: a. loud pronunciation of the specific action verb jump (the verb was repeated 9–10 times, during 10 s, with a steady, monotone, low voice), b. silent pronunciation (S-jump) was repeated in a similar way as when loudly pronounced, c. mental subtraction (MS) that was used as a control measure of a non-specific stimulus for the jump effect (the subjects were asked to mentally subtract two digit numbers from three digit ones) and d. kinaesthetic imagery (KI; the subjects were asked to feel the movement, without realising it) also used as a possible control for the effect of the action verb jump. Prior to the start of the experiment the subjects completed the revised version of the Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ, [51]) to measure the individual ability in kinaesthetic imagery. No significant correlation was found between the values of MIQ and the performance after KI (r2 = 0.09; Pearson’s test).

Experiment 3: Reading the Specific Action Verb

The action verb jump was projected on a black screen (the image was 1.3 m long, in diagonal) and the participants (see Table 1) stood at a distance of 3.5 m from the wall (as specified by the manufacturer, Optoma/ThemeScene® projector, 92100 Boulogne-Billancourt, France). The action verb was written in white (Police: Times New Roman, font 110) on a black background. The subjects carried out 6 cognitive conditions, each lasting 10 s: a. reading loudly jump (R-jump) written on the middle of the screen; b. reading silently the action verb (RS-jump); c. reading loudly jump that was moving bottom-up on the screen (Rm-jump; the movement speed of the verb was set on medium mode, Microsoft PowerPoint 2007® software); d. reading silently jump as in c (RSm-jump); e. pronouncing jump without visual support as in experiment 2, used as a first specific control; f. looking at the blank unlighted screen as a second control condition.

Experiment 4: Pronouncing Non-specific Action Verbs

Sixteen participants (see Table 1) were asked to perform experimental blocks where the selected verbs either described the specific action (jump was used as the standard specific action verb) or actions concerning the fingers (pinch) or the tongue (lick), while a meaningless verb for all subjects (tiáo = jump, in Pinjing Chinese) was used as a semantic control.

Experiment 5: Hearing Non-specific Action Verbs

The same experimental protocol used in the previous experiment was here performed with the difference that verbs were not pronounced but were heard by 16 subjects (see Table 1). The action verbs were previously recorded by an experimenter and pronounced once per second with the same tone and pace employed when the subjects pronounced them. The recording was listened for 10s before the execution of the SVJ.

Experiment 6: Pronouncing Emotion and Feeling Verbs

Twenty subjects (see Table 1) took part in this experiment and five verbs were selected as cognitive stimuli: gagne (win), perds (lose, considered as the antonym of the previous), rêve (dream), bouge (move) as a general action verb and jump as the control specific action verb.

Experiment 7: Hearing Actions Verbs Contradicting SVJs

Ten francophone subjects (see table 1) heard the verbs saute (jump), tombe (fall) and stop before the execution of the SVJ. The verbs were heard and not pronounced in order to produce a surprise in the subjects; the verbs were thus registered and listened by the subjects as in experiment 5.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using the software R2.11.1® (www.r-project.org). Analysis of variance for repeated measures (rANOVA) was used to examine the effect of the various conditions followed by the post-hoc Tukey’s test. Both were combined with the linear Mixed Effects Model [52], which was used to compare different conditions and taking into consideration the inter subjects’ variability. Statistical significance was set at 2-tail p<0.05.

Results

Experiment 1

This experiment was performed to show whether the repetition of SVJ might have a training effect upon the performance of reference subjects. The results (Table 2; group effect: p = 0.55, F = 0.76) showed that this was not the case. Moreover when the data were normalized to BJ (in per cent of BJ height) they showed a non-significant decrease in the height values (from 96.4±5.7% for J1 to 99.0±8.9% for J4; Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 2. Effect of action and other verbs upon the height of the SVJ.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068687.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Normalized Data: Influence of different cognitive stimuli upon SVJ performance.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068687.t003

Experiment 2

The experiment showed a significant group effect (p<0.001, F = 7.32) and an improvement in SVJs was observed when the subjects pronounced loudly the verb jump (p<0.001, z = 4.66; Table 2) or when they did so silently (without moving lips or tongue) prior to the jump (S-jump; p<0.001, z = 4.68). Kinaesthetic Imagery produced an increase in height (p<0.01, z = 3.49), thus confirming the potentiality of KI upon the motor execution of even a complex motor task as the SVJ. A height improvement was also obtained when the performed cognitive stimulus was a mental subtraction (MS; p = 0.02, z = 3.13). The effect of each cognitive condition revealed that the height of SVJ was significantly improved also when the data were normalized to BJ (Table 3) (from 104.4±7.8% for MS, p = .014, z = 3.16 up to 106.5±7.5%, p<.001, z = 4.69 for jump).

Experiment 3

The results of the third experiment (Table 2) revealed the effect of adding a visual stimulus to the loud or silent pronunciation of jump. Reading (R) the action verb shown on a screen was effective in increasing the performance of SVJs either if read and loudly pronounced (R-jump; p<0.01, z = −3.97) or if silently read (RS-jump; p<0.01, z = 4.04), but it was not significantly different from the loud pronunciation of the verb without reading (jump; p<0.001, z = −5.0; Table 2). When the action verb was moving bottom-up on the screen, no additional effect was observed (Rm-jump: p<0.001, z = −4.55; RSm-jump: p<0.01, z = 3.8). The control condition, looking at the blank screen before jumping, did not affect the height of the jump (p = 1.0, z = 0.05). In any case there was no observable significant difference between pronouncing jump and reading it fixed or moving bottom-up. The normalization towards BJ gave results comparable to the absolute values (from 106.9±8.9% for RSm-jump up to 108.9±11.0 for jump; see Table 3 for statistical significance) indicating that reading and pronouncing were similarly effective.

Experiment 4

The pronunciation of verbs such as lick and pinch did not show statistically significant effects (Table 2; p = 0.78, z = 1.14 and p = 0.13, z = 2.34, respectively). In semantic coherence, the meaningless verb tiáo did not affect the height performance (p = 0.99, z = 0.84) and the normalisation of the values with respect to BJ (Table 3) gave similar results, excepted for pinch, which showed a statistically significant improvement in the height of the SVJ (104.4±8.7%, p = 0.043, z = 2.78), when normalized to BJ (Table 3). However, note that hearing pinch (H-pinch) did not show any significant improvement (see next experiment).

Experiment 5

The possible effect of hearing (H) these action verbs (Table 2 and 3) gave similar results when compared to their pronunciation. H-jump ameliorated significantly the SVJ height (p<0.001, z = 4.13) but neither did H-lick (p = 0.30, z = 1.94) nor H-pinch (p = 0.83, z = 1.04). The height of SVJs after hearing the meaningless verb tiáo was almost identical to that observed in BJs (p = 0.99, z = 0.21). The comparison of the normalized values between pronounced and heard action verbs gave similar results (Table 3).

Experiment 6

This experiment (Table 2) showed that the height of the jumps was enhanced by the action verb jump (p = 0.006, z = 3.49) and significantly affected by the loud pronunciation of win, a verb which may have an emotional association to the physical performance (p<0.001, z = −4.1). However a significant effect was also found with the antonym lose (p = 0.018, z = 3.19) and this was confirmed when the values were normalized to BJ (table 3; p = 0.014, z = 3.25). The loud pronunciation of dream did not affect the performance of the jump (p = 0.64, z = 1.54) and the same was observed with the non-specific action verb move (p = 0.36, z = −1.96). The meaningless verb tiáo was not included in this experiment in order not to burden the task of the subjects with too many stimuli and risk an unwanted sense of fatigue in the subjects.

Experiment 7

The last experiment of the study (Table 2 and 3) was performed to further control the effect of verbs signifying actions opposing the SVJ. To further test the reproducibility of the protocol with other subjects that French nationals, the experiment was performed at the Department of Physiotherapy, Lebanese University (the subjects were students at the Department of Physical Education). The results show a significant group effect (p<0.001, F = 31.86) with the expected increase of about 2 cm after hearing the verb jump (31.7±0.4 vs 29.8±0.9 for BJ, p<0.001, z = 7.97) and no effect after fall (29.8±0.7, p<0.99, z = - 013) or stop (29.9±0.7, p<1, z = 0.1).

General Comments Concerning the Experimental Setting

It is important to note that all the subjects participating to the study were asked, at the end of each experiment, if they had carefully understood the meaning of the verbs and this was confirmed; moreover the subjects stated that they kept obeying, throughout the experiment, to the instruction given at the beginning of the experiment to execute maximum SVJ (this was necessary to avoid inevitable large variations in the values of the heights).

Furthermore, when all the values obtained with the stimulus jump, as delivered in experiments 2 to 7 (Table 2), were summed up the results gave an improvement of 2.0 cm (32.8±4.2 cm vs 30.8±4.2 cm for BJ, p<.001, z = 9.43, n = 114) representing an average increase of 106.8±7.5% of the total jump height (p<.001, z = 8.99). Moreover, when all the values obtained, in each experiment, by the action verb jump were compared one to the other no statistically significant difference was observed among them (the group effect gave p = 0.88, F = 0.35 when analysed by a one factor ANOVA). Improvements were also obtained with MS (104.4±7.8%), KI (104.6±5.8%) and with the verbs pinch (104.4±8.7%; but not H-pinch, 101.2±4.4%), win (104.9±5.8%) and lose (103.6±5.5%) (see Table 3). The meaningless verb tiáo did not produce any significant increase in height and its value was quite similar to that of BJ, much as the observation of a blank screen in experiment 3, and verbs lick, dream, move, fall and stop showed also no statistically significant effect.

Discussion

The relationship between language and action is based upon experimental evidence reporting the involvement of pre-central frontal cortical areas (premotor, primary motor and Broca’s areas) in the expression of language ([10], for a review see: [9], [53]) and in action understanding [54]. Conversely, it has also been found that language, and in particular action verbs, may stimulate motion. This was done with kinematic studies, usually after reading action words, and by measuring the velocity in grasping or reaching [11], [12]. The observed results showed functional activations in corresponding cortical motor areas [3], [8]. The present findings confirm the relation between language and motion and show that the pronunciation, the enunciation, the hearing and the reading of a specific action verb (jump) improve subjects’ performance of a complex movement as a SVJ. Furthermore, the height improvement produced by this action verb is comparable to the effects produced by KI, which is a well-known stimulus used to enhance physical performance (see for review: [55]).

One of the findings arising from studies reported in the literature shows the possibility that the cerebral cortex, organised in specialised areas, may not appear as clearly structured as it was represented in Talairach and Tornoux’s [56] atlas of the brain cortex (see for example: [40], [41]). It is a common observation among neuro-anatomists and, particularly, neurosurgeons that the brain circumvolutions are as complex and varied as human fingerprints and, consequently, that each brain is hardly similar to another one. In fact, already Penfield and Boldrey, in their seminal article on intra-cortical electrical stimulation in epileptic patients [57], observed that the brain cortical differences are not only anatomo-morphologic but also functional. Other findings have shown that the relations among word listening, pronunciation and comprehension, and the motor areas imply their participation and interaction into peri-Sylvian circuitries that modulate these functions (see: [9], [58], [59]). As a consequence the possibility that the brain functional activity corresponds to the regulation of compartmentalised, highly specialised cortical areas does not seem to be confirmed by recent findings as more complex interactions correlate one area to the other through very intricate circuitries (for review see: [18], [20], [21]).

The present findings have led to the main following results: 1. The four types of stimuli that were given to the naïve subjects: loudly and silently pronouncing, hearing and reading a significant and specific action verb (jump; at the second person of the French imperative), ameliorate the height of SVJs; 2. Kinaesthetic Imagery, known to stimulate motor action, improved the performance and so did mental subtraction (MS, vide infra); 3. When the SVJ was preceded by the pronunciation of action verbs having no specificity in relation to SVJs (lick and pinch) a statistically not significant increase in the SVJ height was usually observed. The only exceptions appeared with pinch after normalization of the data to BJ (although at the limit of significance, p = 0.043, z = 2.78); 4. Verbs capable of raising an affect but not specific to SVJ, as gagne (win) and its antonym perds (lose), increased the height of the SVJ, while a verb having no relation to motor action but a peculiar emotionality (dream) led to a not significant effect; similarly, a verb having a relation to movement (move) was not effective; 5. The addition of a visual stimulus, i.e.: when the specific action verb jump was read on a screen, immobile or moving from bottom up at a steady pace, it led to an improvement of the jump height. The effect of reading jump was not different to pronouncing it (without visual stimulus); 6. All verbs used in the study, however, increased, even if not significantly, the SVJ height (with the exception of the senseless tiáo and blank screen when reading in experiment 3; see Tables 2 and 3), possibly implying that verbs having a poor specificity and an affective touch, may increase the height, as it was observed in the case of win and lose (Experiment 6, Tables 2 and 3) (it should be kept in mind though that these two verbs may be related to a physical effort, contrary to dream and move, which in fact showed no statistically significant influence on the jumps; emotions might then be important in influencing the motor action [60], [61], particularly if related to the motion); 7. In an attempt to assure the specificity of hearing the verb jump a seventh experiment was performed to compare its effect to that of fall and stop (Table 2 and 3). The two verbs opposing to the motor action did not show any effect, much as it was observed with tiáo. It might have been expected a decrease in the value of the jumps, but the results are in line with the instruction given at the beginning of each trial, to each subject individually, to execute a maximum SVJ. This confirms that the not significant effects observed in table 3 were possibly the consequence of the intention and attention of each individual to the given task, i.e.: to perform a maximum SVJ regardless the given stimulus (see below).

An interesting result concerned the effect of simple calculus. Arithmetic is known to activate motor brain areas when an exact answer is demanded [62], [63]. Such correlation between simple calculus and the activation of motor areas was the reason that brought to the use of the mental subtraction in the present study, even if the main purpose was to use it as a control for jump; the results showed a significant improvement of the SVJ heights after mental subtraction (Table 2 and 3). A possible explanation for this effect may be the reported activation of language areas while calculating in mother tongue (French in this case) [64] and of motor areas found during exact arithmetic [65]. Moreover, and contrary to tiáo (senseless verb for whoever is not a sinophone), arithmetic has a significance for all humans representing, much as movement and gestures, a particular form of communication [66]. If this is the answer for the observed effect of calculus upon SVJ height, it may be understandable that a form of communication might improve motor action, probably because of the primitive information qualities of motor actions (gestures), quite common in several cultures in support to language (see: [23]).

The effects observed after KI and after visual observation (when reading) of the action verb require some further comments. As it has been mentioned in the introduction, KI is the kinaesthetic feeling of the motor action that, in the present experiments, was used as a control for the action verb jump. The fact that, under the present experimental conditions, KI might also improve the performance of the SVJ gives a further impact of kinaesthetic imagery upon a complex motion.

The improvement in performance induced by some of the stimuli used in the study did not completely follow the specificity usually described in the literature. One possible reason in the discordance may be related to the nature of the motor action itself. A Squat Vertical Jump is a rigorous well-known movement used in biomechanical studies for its reproducibility and the number of information that may be derived from a congruous analysis of the motor action. Compared to a grasping or reaching movement, in neuro-functional terms, a SVJ is a more complex movement as it includes the individual’s intention, attention, learning, automatisms, perceptions, understanding, conceptualisation, etc.; in other terms, the SVJ movement involves the functional activation of most part of the central sensory-motor systems, including the partial participation of the peripheral nervous systems ([67], [68] and for review [21], [24]). From this point of view, the data are in line with the finding that a difference exists in learning and executing movements of different complexity (see [44], [45]).

In an attempt to reach a tentative conclusion for the present results it seems possible that the intention of the individual, the attention that she/he gives to the motor act [67] and the emotional status produced by words [60], [61] may lead to an interaction of language areas with motor areas to eventually improve performance. This may be particularly so in the case of a complex movement as SVJ.

Conclusion

The present results indicate that specific cognitive stimuli, such as the verb jump or KI, improved the performance of SVJs when related to the complex motor action. However, non-specific cognitive stimuli (verbs win, lose, pinch and MS) might also implement the jump (see Table 3 and for review see: [21]). Emotions, represented by verbs win and lose, may play an important role as it has been recently suggested [60] (especially during a physical effort) and the attention of the individual has also been suggested in the interaction between language and motor action [67], which may possibly happen through a mental simulation process [24], [69].

The present data indicate an improving effect of the action verb specific for the SVJ, but they do not seem to indicate an exclusive specificity probably because of the complexity of SVJ. It might also be suggested that it is not only the specificity of the verb that is important but also the intentionality raised in the subject when pronouncing or expressing or hearing any meaningful action verb (for the role of the fronto-parietal cortex in the intention, planning and decision making in sensory-motor actions, see: [68]). This may include other cognitive means such as KI and calculus.

Acknowledgments

Our warm thanks to Dr Stéphane Champely for his valuable help in the statistical analysis of the data.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: TR PF ARS RM. Performed the experiments: TR CC RM. Analyzed the data: TR PF RM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: CC RM. Wrote the paper: TR PF RM.

References

  1. 1. Gentilucci M, Corballis MC (2006) From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30: 949–960.
  2. 2. Gentilucci M, Cangitano M (1998) Influence of automatic word reading on motor control. Eur J Neurosci 10: 752–756.
  3. 3. Hauk O, Johnsrude I, Pulvermüller F (2004) Somatopic representation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex. Neuron 41: 301–307.
  4. 4. Aziz-Zadeh L, Wilson SM, Rizzolatti G, Iacoboni M (2006) Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Curr Biol 16: 1818–1823.
  5. 5. Buccino G, Riggio L, Melli G, Binkofski F, Gallese V, et al. (2005) Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24: 355–363.
  6. 6. Tettamanti M, Buccino G, Saccuman MC, Gallese V, Danna M, et al. (2005) Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J Cogn Neurosci 17: 273–281.
  7. 7. van Dam WO, Rueschemeyer SA, Bekkering H (2010) How specifically are action verbs represented in the neural motor system: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 53: 1318–1325.
  8. 8. Kemmerer D, Castillo JG, Talavage T, Patterson S, Wiley C (2008) Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: Evidence from fMRI. Brain Lang 116: 16–43.
  9. 9. Pulvermüller F, Fadiga L (2010) Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Rev 11: 351–360.
  10. 10. Pulvermüller F, Hauk O, Nikulin VV, Ilmoniemi RJ (2005) Functional links between motor and language systems. Eur J Neurosci 21: 793–797.
  11. 11. Boulenger V, Roy AC, Paulignan Y, Deprez V, Jeannerod M, et al. (2006) Crosstalk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200 msec of processing. J Cogn Neurosci 18: 1607–1615.
  12. 12. Nazir TA, Boulenger V, Roy A, Silber B, Jeannerod M, et al. (2008) language-induced motor perturbations during the execution of a reaching movement. Q J Exp Psychol 61: 933–943.
  13. 13. Dalla Volta R, Gianelli C, Campione GC, Gentilucci M (2009) Action word understanding and overt motor behavior. Exp Brain Res 196: 403–412.
  14. 14. Péran P, Démonet JF, Cherubini A, Carbebat D, Caltagirone C, et al. (2010) Mental representations of action: The neural correlates of the verbal and motor components. Brain Res 1328: 89–103.
  15. 15. Boulenger V, Hauk O, Pulvermüller F (2009) Grasping ideas with the motor system: semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension. Cereb Cortex 19: 1905–1914.
  16. 16. Gentilucci M, Campione GC, De Stefani E, Innocenti A (2012) Is the coupled control of hand and mouth postures precursor of reciprocal relations between gestures and words? Behav Brain Res 233: 130–140.
  17. 17. Hauk O, Pulwermüller F (2011) The lateralization of motor cortex activation to action-words. Front Human Neurosci 5: 149
  18. 18. Willems RM, Casasanto D (2011) Flexibility in embodied language understanding. Front Psychol 2: 1–11.
  19. 19. Fischer MH, Zwaan RA (2008) Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Quart J Exper Psychol 61: 825–850.
  20. 20. Fernandino L, Iacoboni M (2010) Are cortical motor maps based on body parts or coordinated actions? Implications for embodied semantics. Brain Lang 112: 44–53.
  21. 21. Bedny M, Caramazza A (2011) Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: the case from action verbs. Ann NY Acad Sci 1224: 81–95.
  22. 22. Postle N, McMahon K, Ashton R, Meredith M, de Zubicaray GI (2008) Action word meaning representations in cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cortices. Neuroimage 43: 634–644.
  23. 23. Willems RM, Hagoort P (2007) Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review. Brain Lang 101: 278–289.
  24. 24. Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain 22: 557–560.
  25. 25. Mahon BZ, Caramazza A (2008) A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. J Physiol Paris 102: 59–70.
  26. 26. Gentilucci M, Dalla Volta R (2008) Spoken language and arm gestures are controlled by the same control system. Quart J Exper Psychol 61: 944–957.
  27. 27. Cappa SF, Pulwermüller F (2012) Cortex special issue: Language and motor system. Cortex 48: 785–944.
  28. 28. Pulvermüller F (2005) Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 576–582.
  29. 29. Gallese V, Lakoff G (2005) The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cogn Neuropsychol 21: 455–479.
  30. 30. Ingvar D, Phillipson L (1977) Distribution of cerebral blood flow in the dominant hemisphere during motor ideation and motor performance. Ann Neurol 2: 230–237.
  31. 31. Driskell JE, Copper C, Moran A (1994) Does Mental Practice Enhance Performance? J Appl Physiol 79: 481–492.
  32. 32. Jeannerod M (1995) Mental imagery in the motor context. Neuropsychologia 33: 1419–1432.
  33. 33. Decety J, Grèzes J (2006) The power of simulation: Imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Res 1079: 4–14.
  34. 34. Sackett RS (1935) The relationship between amount of symbolic rehearsal and retention in maze habit. J Gen Psychol 13: 113–130.
  35. 35. Perry HM (1939) The relative efficiency of actual and imaginary practice in 5 selected tasks. Arch Psychol 4: 5–75.
  36. 36. Schuster C, Hilfiker R, Amft O, Scheidhauer A, Butler J, et al. (2011) Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training element in five different disciplines. BMC Med 17: 9–75.
  37. 37. Guillot A, Di Rienzo F, Macintyre T, Moran A, Collet C (2012) Imagining is not doing but involves specific motor commands: a review of experimental data related to motor inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci 6: 1–22.
  38. 38. Lotze L, Halsband U (2006) Motor imagery. J Physiol Paris 99: 386–395.
  39. 39. Catani M, Jones DK, ffytche DH (2005) Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Ann Neurol 57: 9–16.
  40. 40. Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C (2010) The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Rev 11: 264–274.
  41. 41. Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C (2010) So quel che fai. Il cervello che agisce e i neuroni specchio. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore.
  42. 42. Hubley CL, Wells RP (1983) A Work-Energy approach to determine individual joint contributions to vertical jump performance. Eur J Appl Physiol 50: 247–254.
  43. 43. Frak V, Nazir T, Goyette M, Cohen H, Jeannerod M (2010) Grip force is part of the semantic representation of manual action verbs. PloS One 5: e9728.
  44. 44. Wulf G, Shea CH (2002) Principles derived from the study of simples skills do not generalize to complex skill learning. Psychon Bull Rev 9: 185–211.
  45. 45. Fink PW, Foo P, Jirsa V (2000) Local and global stabilization of coordination by sensory information. Exp Brain Res 134: 9–20.
  46. 46. Hoyek N, Champely S, Collet C, Fargier P, Guyot A (2009) Age and gender-related differences in the temporal congruence development between motor imagery and motor performance. Learn Individ Differ 19: 555–560.
  47. 47. Zatziorsky V, Seluyanov V (1983) The mass and inertia characteristics of the main segments of the human body. In: Matsui H, Kobayashi K, editors. Biomecanics VIII-B., Champaign: Human Kinetics. 1152–1159.
  48. 48. Li JZ, Absher DM, Tang H, Southwick AM, Casto AM, et al. (2008) Worldwide human relationships inferred from genome-wide patterns of variations. Science 319: 1100–1104.
  49. 49. Van Soest AJ, Bobbert MF (1993) The contribution of muscle properties in the control of explosive movements. Biol Cybern 69: 195–204.
  50. 50. Casartelli N, Muller R, Maffiuletti NA (2010) Validity and reliability of the Myotest accelerometer system for the assessment of vertical jump height. J Strength Cond Res 24: 3186–3193.
  51. 51. Hall CR, Martin KA (1997) Measuring movement imagery abilities: A revision of the movement imagery questionnaire. J Mental Imagery 21: 143–154.
  52. 52. Pinheiro JC, Bates DM (2000) Mixed-effects models in S and S-Plus. New York: Springer-Verlag.
  53. 53. Pulvermüller F (2001) Brain reflection of words and their meaning. Trends Cogn Sci 5: S17–S24.
  54. 54. Tranel D, Kemmerer D, Adolphs R, Damasio H, Damasio AR (2003) Neural correlates of conceptual knowledge for actions. Cogn Neuropsychol 20: 409–432.
  55. 55. Vogt S (1996) Imagery and perception-action mediation in imitative actions. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 3: 79–86.
  56. 56. Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human Brain. Stuttgart: Thieme.
  57. 57. Penfield W, Boldrey E (1937) Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain 60: 389–443.
  58. 58. Garagnani M, Wennekers T, Pulvermuller F (2008) A neuroanatomically grounded Hebbian-learning model of attention-language interactions in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 27: 492–513.
  59. 59. Hickok G (2009) The functional neuroanatomy of language. Phys Life Rev 6: 121–143.
  60. 60. Moseley R, Carota F, Hauk O, Mohr B, Pulwermuller F (2012) A role for the motor system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cereb Cortex 22: 1634–1647.
  61. 61. Havas DA, Glenberg AM (2007) Emotion stimulation during language comprehension. Psychon Bull Rev 14: 436–441.
  62. 62. Kazui H, Kitagaki H, Mori E (2000) Cortical activation during retrieval of arithmetical facts and actual calculation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 54: 479–485.
  63. 63. Tschentscher N, Hauk O, Fischer MH, Pulwerüller F (2012) You can count on the motor cortex: finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. Neuroimage 59: 3139–3148.
  64. 64. Dehaene S, Spelke E, Pinel P, Stanescu R, Tsivkin S (1999) Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain imaging evidence. Science 284: 970–974.
  65. 65. Lin JFL, Imada T, Kuhl PK (2011) Mental addition in bilinguals: an fMRI study of task-related and performance-related activation. Cereb Cortex 22: 1851–1861.
  66. 66. Gentilucci M, Dalla Volta R, Giannelli C (2008) When the hands speak. J Physiol Paris 102: 21–30.
  67. 67. Shtyrov Y, Smith ML, Horner AJ, Henson R, Nathan PJ, et al. (2012) Attention to language: Novel MEG paradigm for registering involuntary language processing in the brain. Neuropsychol 50: 2605–2616.
  68. 68. Andersen RA, Cui H (2009) Intention, Action Planning, and Decision Making in Parietal-Frontal Circuits. Neuron 63: 568–583.
  69. 69. Zwaan RA, Madden CJ, Yaxley RH, Aveyard ME (2004) Moving words: Dynamic mental representations in language comprehension. Cogn Sci 28: 611–619.