Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Smokers’ Decision Making: More than Mere Risk Taking

  • Eyal Ert ,

    eyal.ert@mail.huji.ac.il

    Affiliation Department of Agricultural Economics and Management, Faculty of Agriculture Food and Environment, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Rehovot, Israel

  • Eldad Yechiam,

    Affiliation Faculty of Industrial Engineering and Management, Technion, Haifa, Israel

  • Olga Arshavsky

    Affiliation Faculty of Industrial Engineering and Management, Technion, Haifa, Israel

Abstract

The fact that smoking is bad for people’s health has become common knowledge, yet a substantial amount of people still smoke. Previous studies that sought to better understand this phenomenon have found that smoking is associated with the tendency to take risk in other areas of life as well. The current paper explores factors that may underlie this tendency. An experimental analysis shows that smokers are more easily tempted by immediate high rewards compared to nonsmokers. Thus the salience of risky alternatives that produce large rewards most of the time can direct smokers to make bad choices even in an abstract situation such as the Iowa Gambling Task. These findings suggest that the risk taking behavior associated with smoking is not related to the mere pursuit of rewards but rather reflects a tendency to yield to immediate temptation.

Introduction

Tobacco use, particularly cigarette smoking, is a major preventable cause of premature death and disease. Globally, nearly five million persons die every year from tobacco-related illnesses. In the United States alone smoking is responsible for nearly 443,000 deaths each year (about 18% of total US deaths, [1]). Nevertheless, a substantial amount of people are still smoking. The 2011 National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) indicated that approximately 19.3% of US adults are cigarette smokers [2]. In Israel, where the current study was conducted, about 23% of the adult population smokes cigarettes [3].

Given the clear negative consequences of smoking, the substantial number of smokers is puzzling. This puzzle has led scholars to search for potential traits associated with smoking, in the hope of better understanding the decision to smoke. A natural “suspect” is smokers’ attitude towards risk. There is evidence that smokers are well aware of the risks of smoking ([4]; though Slovic [5] suggests that they do not interpret them correctly). In fact, some studies showed that smokers are even more informed of the risks of smoking than nonsmokers [6]. These findings may suggest that smokers are generally risk insensitive. In line with this reasoning, studies have indicated that compared to nonsmokers, smokers are more likely to partake in a variety of risky behaviors other than tobacco use. For example, smokers tend to be more involved in traffic accidents [7], are less likely to wear seatbelts [8], [9], and are more likely to engage in risky sexual behavior [10]. In addition, women smokers report 12–15% lower rates of mammography checks than nonsmokers [11].

These surprising findings have motivated more controlled examinations for possible differences between smokers and nonsmokers. One interesting area of research focuses on differences in personality, and has shown that smoking is associated with impulsivity [12], [13], psychoticism [13], defensive optimism [14], [15], and sensation seeking [16].

A complementary line of research focuses on behavioral measures of risk taking in controlled laboratory tasks. A commonly used task in this context is the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; see [17], [18]) in which individuals choose repeatedly between four decks of cards, associated with different payoff distributions, for an unspecified number of trials. Two of these payoff distributions are characterized by relatively higher risk (larger gains and losses) compared to the other two decks, as well as by greater immediate benefits but lower long term outcomes (see Table 1). Typically, individuals with brain lesions in areas affecting decision making were found to take more risk and perform more poorly in this task [17], [18]. The IGT was also found to distinguish chronic cannabis and cocaine abusers from non drug-abusing controls (e.g., [19], [20]). However, in a recent study, Lejuez et al. [21] used the IGT to compare smokers to nonsmokers, but did not find significant differences between the two groups (see also Aklin et al. [22]). These results suggest that smokers’ ability to learn the possible harmful consequences of risky behavior is not impaired [4]. Yet they still leave open the question of why smokers exhibit risky behavior in different domains as noted above.

thumbnail
Table 1. The schedule of rewards and penalties in the four decks of cards of the IGT, as introduced in [20]: The first block of 10 selections from each of the decks (out of 6 blocks).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068064.t001

The main objective of this paper is to better understand the psychological constructs that contribute to smokers’ risk taking. We suggest that it is not risk taking per se that drives the difference between smokers and nonsmokers’ decision making. Rather, we argue that the potential link between smoking and risk taking lies in the person’s ability to resist temptations. Indeed, previous research has suggested a close relation between visceral urges and smoking [23]. In a series of studies, Baumeister and his colleagues have shown that smokers tend to exhibit poorer self control than nonsmokers [24], [25]. Additionally, training of self-control seemed to improve people’s ability to give up smoking [26].

In the context of decision making, we propose a condition where risk taking can be facilitated by poor self-control. Selecting alternatives that result in poor immediate outcomes but high long term outcomes requires the exercise of self control [27]. Hence, when all rewards are transparent poor self control facilitates the preference for a risky alternative that frequently produces the best reward, even if its expected value is negative. Using two modified versions of the IGT, we predicted that smokers’ risk taking is driven by their tendency to be more easily tempted by common favorable outcomes, rather than by a general tendency towards risk taking.

Methods

Ethics Statement

Before the beginning of the study the participants were provided with a written statement, which specified the research procedure. Participants gave their informed consent both verbally and by clicking a response button that asked for their approval to participate in the experiment. As anonymity was vital for the success of the study, this procedure ensured that no record would link their identity to this research. The experiment and its consent procedure were approved by the Technion Ethics committee for Human Studies in the Behavioral Sciences.

Task Description

As presented in Table 1, our modified version of the IGT involves choices between four different alternatives in each of 60 trials. The IGT includes two disadvantageous decks that yield negative return in the long run: Dis50 and Dis90. The latter deck (Dis90), however, also produces the highest payoff in most trials, and is associated with the smallest probability of losing. Thus, it presumably takes a certain amount of self-control to consistently make the decision of not selecting this option (indeed, most individuals are attracted to options with high common payoffs and rare negative outcomes [28][30]). Additionally, in our version of the IGT, the outcomes of all (chosen and unchosen) decks are revealed after each trial. The use of such “foregone payoffs” was expected to further increase the self control challenge of avoiding deck Dis90, as it highlights its relative advantage over all other decks in a typical trial (e.g., [28], [31], [32]). Previously, the administration of foregone payoffs was found to increase risk taking among high functioning drug abusers [28].

In order to further evaluate this assertion we administered a second task where risky choices were not associated with common (or uncommon) events. This task, referred to as EGT (Equal-probability Gambling Task) is described in Table 2 (it is a variant of the task used by Lane et al. [33]). Since there is no alternative associated with the best common outcome in this task, the tendency to select the best recent outcome should not affect risk taking. Thus, we expected no differences in risk taking between smokers and nonsmokers in the EGT.

thumbnail
Table 2. The schedule of rewards and penalties in the four decks of cards of the EGT.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068064.t002

Participants

One hundred Technion students (51 males and 49 females) participated in the experiment. Participants were paid NIS 30 (about $7.50) for showing-up plus the monetary bonus they had earned in association with their performance. They earned on average about NIS 55.

Apparatus and Procedure

Before the beginning of the study participants were instructed about the research procedure and gave their informed consent. Then, each participant was presented with two behavioral tasks: (a) the modified IGT and (b) the EGT (presented in Table 2). The modified IGT involved choices between four decks of cards. The task included 60 trials. Payoffs were drawn from six decks of 10 cards, as described in Table 1. In each consecutive block the differences between advantageous and disadvantageous decks increased linearly. Participants were given written instructions identical to those provided in [28]. Briefly, participants were told that some decks are worse than others, and they should avoid those decks to win the game. After each selection participants were presented with foregone payoffs, which were displayed on the unselected decks for an unlimited time. Participants transitioned to the next trial by pressing a button labeled “Move to the next choice.” The EGT used the same procedure as the IGT except that its alternatives were associated with different outcomes (see Table 2). The ordering of the two tasks was counterbalanced. Participants were told that their accumulated task payoff would be added to their show-up fee.

After the completion of the behavioral tasks, we used several additional measures. We administered the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire – Revised (EPQ-R-S [34]; Hebrew version, see [35], [36]) to identify possible differences between smokers and nonsmokers’ personality traits. We also ran a computerized version of Set 1 of the Raven Progressive Matrices test [37] to verify that there were no differences in IQ between smokers and the control group.

Participants’ last task was to answer a demographic questionnaire whose main objective was to identify smokers. Specifically, participants were asked whether they smoked, or used to smoke in the past. Those who responded positively were asked how many cigarettes they had smoked per day in the last six months. Thirty three participants (15 males and 17 females) were classified as smokers. Initially, out of 87 people only 20 were identified as smokers. To increase the proportion of smokers in our sample, we added the last 13 people by first delivering a phone interview asking about various habits (including smoking) and then inviting smokers only to the lab. We did not identify any significant differences in behavior between these 13 smokers and the other 20 smokers. The median number of cigarettes smoked per day was 5 and the mean was about 7, so the smokers in our samples can be considered as relatively light smokers. Additionally, seven participants (5 males and 2 females) were identified as past smokers. These past smokers were treated in our main analysis as nonsmokers but we also conducted a secondary analysis to see whether the results change when past smokers are classified as smokers. The data is available at the first author’s website: http://departments.agri.huji.ac.il/economics/teachers/ert_eyal/ publications.htm.

Results

The choice patterns in the IGT are summarized in Figure 1 which presents the choice proportion from each deck, as well as the proportion of disadvantageous choices across trials. The results show a weak (and insignificant) tendency of smokers to choose more disadvantageously than nonsmokers (Smokers: 0.54, SD  = 0.19; nonsmokers: 0.46, SD  = 0.25; t(98)  = 1.49, NS).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Smokers and nonsmokers’ choices from each of the decks of the modified IGT (average proportions and standard errors).

The right column (Disadv) shows the proportion of choices from the disadvantageous decks (Dis50+Dis90).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068064.g001

We proceeded to examine whether differences would be found in the learning patterns for individual decks. Figure 2 presents the learning patterns of smokers and nonsmokers for each deck. These patterns were evaluated with a repeated measures analysis of variance with trial block (3 blocks of 20 trials) as a within subject variable, and smoking as a between subject variable. The results revealed that, with experience, smokers increased their selections from Deck Dis90, whereas nonsmokers learned to avoid it, F(2, 180)  = 2.99, p  = .05. This pattern of choices led nonsmokers to choose more advantageously with experience, whereas for smokers the learning curve was milder, F(2, 180)  = 2.20, p  = .09.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Smokers’ and nonsmokers’ choices as a function of time, by blocks of 20 trials, in the modified IGT (average proportions).

The right column (Disadv) shows the proportion of choices from the disadvantageous decks (Dis50+Dis90).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068064.g002

In line with these differences in mean performance, an individual level analysis [36] showed that only 33% of the smokers learned from their experience and improved their advantageous selections in each block (of 20 trials) compared to 54% of the nonsmokers, Z  = 1.97, p  = .048.

These results demonstrate that, similarly to high-functioning drug abusers [28], smokers could not resist the temptation of the constantly presented high rewards associated with deck Dis90. Thus, smokers’ preference of high common outcomes has led them to make bad choices in the task. In order to examine a potential mechanism leading to this pattern of selections, we ran a trial by trial analysis that focused on the proportion of choices from the alternative that yielded the best outcome in the most recent trial (a pattern called “chasing” [38]). The analysis revealed that smokers chased the best recent outcome 39% (SD  = 17%) of the time, while nonsmokers chased it only 30% of the time (SD  = 26%). This difference was significant, t(98)  = 2.12, p  = .04.

The results of the EGT, presented in Figure 3, showed that, similar to the IGT, there was a weak tendency among smokers to choose more disadvantageously than nonsmokers (smokers  = 0.30, SD  = 0.22; nonsmokers  = 0.23, SD  = 0.25; t(98)  = 3.11, p  = .08). Yet an analysis of the learning patterns, depicted in Figure 4, reveals that, unlike in the IGT, smokers have not become more attracted to the disadvantageous decks in the course of their experience. Both smokers and non- smokers learned to choose advantageously with experience, F(2,196)  = 26.81, p<.001, and there were no significant differences in the learning patterns of the two groups, F(2,98)  = 0.33, NS. An individual level analysis showed that 52% of the smokers learned from their experience and improved their advantageous selections in each block compared to 60% of the nonsmokers, Z  = 0.76, NS.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Smokers and nonsmokers choices from each of the decks of the EGT (average proportions and standard errors).

The right column (Disadv) shows the proportion of choices from the disadvantageous decks (Dis50a+Dis50b).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068064.g003

thumbnail
Figure 4. Smokers’ and nonsmokers’ choices as a function of time, by blocks of 20 trials, in the modified EGT (average proportions).

The right column (Disadv) shows the proportion of choices from the disadvantageous decks (Dis50a+Dis50b).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068064.g004

Interestingly, the trial by trial analysis of chasing suggests that in the EGT as well smokers were more sensitive than nonsmokers to attractive recent outcomes. Smokers chose the alternative that yielded the best outcome in the most recent trial in 40% (SD  = 25%) of the cases while nonsmokers chased the best outcome only in 24% (SD  = 11%) of the trials, t(98)  = 4.55, p<.0001. These results replicate the findings from the IGT: smokers were more attracted to alternatives that produced the best recent outcome. Unlike the IGT, however, no specific alternative is associated with the best typical outcome in the EGT. Thus, no specific alternative benefited from smokers’ tendency to chase the best recent outcomes.

A Pearson correlation analysis between the proportion of advantageous choices in the IGT and the EGT suggested that performance was mildly related in these tasks, r  = .17, p  = .09. Interestingly, the correlation among nonsmokers was somewhat higher (r  = 0.18) than among smokers (r  = 0.06), though the difference was insignificant, Z  = 0.85, NS.

Table 3 presents the data from the demographic questionnaire, the Raven test, and the personality questionnaire. Almost no differences were found between the two groups in the EPQ-R-S. The only significant difference was in the Lie scale, t(98)  = 3.32, p  = .002, in which smokers’ scores were somewhat lower (Mean  = 1.74, SD  = 3.07) than those of nonsmokers (Mean  = 3.52, SD  = 2.86), suggesting that smokers may be less inclined to make a good impression. The Raven test also indicated similar scores for smokers (Mean  = 85.2%, SD  = 15.0%) and nonsmokers (Mean  = 88.8%, SD  = 12.7%). Thus, the disadvantageous choices of smokers could not be attributed to IQ.

thumbnail
Table 3. Demographic and personality questionnaire data for smokers and nonsmokers, and the average number of cigarettes smoked per day in the last six months in the smokers group (standard deviations appear in parenthesis).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068064.t003

In all the analyses reported above we considered past smokers (7 people) as nonsmokers. We also repeated our analysis by treating past smokers as part of the smokers group. Interestingly, the group differences in the IGT were strengthened. This observation gives further support to the idea that the decision to smoke might be associated, at least to some degree, with certain behavioral traits.

Discussion

The objective of the current paper was to better understand the constructs that drive smokers’ risk taking behavior. In the context of decision making, we proposed a condition where risk taking can be facilitated by temptation. Specifically, we referred to the case where a risky alternative yields the best common outcomes. The results supported the assertion that, similarly to high-functioning drug abusers [28], smokers could not resist selecting from an alternative that yielded highly noticeable positive rewards, even though it also produced large infrequent penalties resulting in an overall loss. The results further showed that the exclusion of such alternatives (as in the EGT task) did not eliminate smokers’ tendency to chase the best recent payoffs but did reduce the differences in risk taking between smokers and nonsmokers, particularly after gaining experience with the alternatives.

Note that this pattern was observed even though the typical smokers in our sample were light smokers. A potentially related finding is that smokers in our sample did not differ from their peers in the personality traits of the EPQ-R-S. This latter observation differs from that found in other studies which have observed differences in the P and E scales [13], [39]. Possibly, the difference results from the fact that the current study included mostly (74%) light smokers (<10 cigarettes a day), while previous studies focused on heavier smokers. For example, light smokers accounted for only 39% of the smokers sample in [13]. This assertion is supported by the finding that psychoticism and impulsivity were found to be predictive of the number of cigarettes smoked per day [13].

We believe that the current findings shed new light on previous studies that have demonstrated smokers’ tendency to take risk in various naturally occurring circumstances, such as in driving behavior [9], sexual conduct [10], and preventative care [11]. It is interesting to note that all of these past findings focused on situations where risky behavior implies under-sensitivity to a rare disaster, such as catching a sexually transmitted disease, being involved in an accident, or having an undiagnosed fatal illness. Moreover, in most of these analyses the risky option can be easily compared to the safer one, and appears to be significantly more attractive in the short run.

The results of the current paper suggest that it is not the mere inclination to take risk that drives smokers’ behavior in such situations, but rather their tendency not to engage in choices that are inconvenient (even though they protect the person from a small likelihood of a catastrophe), and to be more easily tempted by the typically pleasurable alternative.

The results also clarify one potential cognitive mechanism leading to the tendency of smokers to focus on typically favorable outcomes: The tendency to respond to the most recent events and to improperly integrate outcomes in the more distant past. This tendency has been argued and shown to be implicated in cocaine and marijuana abuse [19], [20], [40] and the current study is the first to demonstrate it in light tobacco smokers. The existence of this shared cognitive mechanism may suggest commonalities in self control impairments between tobacco smokers and other psychoactive substance abusers. Because our study focused on light smokers, we believe that these observed characteristics of the smokers’ population were associated with the predisposition to smoke, rather than the effect of Nicotine use and dependence. In line with this interpretation, in previous studies light smokers or even individuals who just tried smoking a couple of times had lower self control in a behavioral task than never-smokers [41], [42]. Nevertheless, future studies should use controlled periods of abstinence in order to rule out potential effects of Nicotine addiction on decision style and self control.

In light of this self control account, it is interesting to return to the results of Lejuez et al. [21], [22], who found no differences between smokers and nonsmokers in the simple IGT. The simple IGT does not include forgone payoff information, and thus the favorable outcomes from the risky alternatives are less tempting, which may explain the null results in these studies. Lejuez et al. [21], [22] also found that smokers took more risk than nonsmokers in the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). In this task the participants choose in several trials between inflating a virtual balloon or moving to the next balloon. Each inflation results in an increase of money in a temporary cache, and upon moving to the next balloon the participant earns all the money in this cache. However, the probability of explosion (that leads to losing all temporary earnings) increases exponentially with each inflation. Notice that in this setting taking risk (choosing to inflate) is typically more favorable than acting safely (choosing to stop), and this distinction is quite salient (the decision maker is continually exposed to the positive outcomes of the “inflate” strategy). The current analysis therefore suggests that smokers’ attraction to the typically favorable outcome might drive their risk taking in this task.

The present findings are thus instrumental in clarifying the link between studies of decision making and self control in smokers. The different decision making behavior exhibited by smokers in the current study, as well as in other studies reported above, can be interpreted by the assertion that smokers are consistently exhibiting less self-control; which leads them to make choices that are commonly rewarding but may be risky on occasion.

Our findings also have some implications to policies directed at reducing smoking. Specifically, they imply that manipulations aimed at reducing the convenience of smoking are likely to be useful because they reduce the immediate temptation to which smokers are highly sensitive. An example is the policy of banning smoking not only inside buildings but also at an external perimeter [43]. While originally proposed to address secondary smoking hazards, this policy also reduces the immediate convenience of smoking, and it was found to decrease the rate of adolescent cigarette use [44]. In a similar vein, making smoking areas less observable to non-smokers (e.g., by using non-transparent partitions) is expected to lower the immediate foregone pleasure of smoking. Similar strategies of making smoking less convenient could be enacted at the individual level as part of treatment. For example, the individual could be instructed to make changes to his/her smoking environment that increase the effort implicated in a typical smoking session (e.g., by wrapping cigarette packets in several layers of adhesive tape). Future studies should examine whether manipulations of this type are particularly effective for individuals who are chronically low on self-control.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: EE EY OA. Performed the experiments: EE EY OA. Analyzed the data: EE EY OA. Wrote the paper: EE EY OA.

References

  1. 1. US Department of Health and Human Services (2010) How tobacco smoke causes disease: the biology and behavioral basis for smoking-attributable disease: a report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health and Human Services, CDC. http://www.cdc.gov/tobacco/data_statistics/sgr/2010/index.htm. Accessed 2013 June 4.
  2. 2. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2011) Vital signs: Current cigarette smoking among adults aged ≥18 years–United States, 2005–2010. Morbidity and mortality weekly report 60(33): 1207–12.
  3. 3. Israeli Ministry of Health. Minister of Heath report on smoking in Israel in 2006–2007. Israeli Ministry of Health (in Hebrew).
  4. 4. Viscusi WK (1999) Do smokers underestimate risks? J Polit Econ 98: 1253–69.
  5. 5. Slovic P (2000) What does it mean to know a cumulative risk? Adolescents' perceptions of short-term and long-term consequences of smoking. J Behav Decis Making 13: 259–66.
  6. 6. Steptoe A, Wardle J, Weiwei C, Baban A, Glass K, et al. (2002) An international comparison of tobacco smoking, beliefs, and risk awareness in university students from 23 countries. Addiction 97: 1561–71.
  7. 7. DiFranza JR, Winters TH, Goldberg RJ, Cirillio L, Bilioris T (1986) The relationship of smoking to motor vehicle accidents and traffic violations. New York State J Med 86: 464–7.
  8. 8. Dillow I, Swann C, Cliff KS (1981) A study of the effect of a health education programme in promoting seat-belt wearing. Health Educ J 40: 14–8.
  9. 9. Eiser JR, Sutton SR, Wober M (1997) Smoking, seatbelts, and beliefs about health. Addict Behav 4: 331–8.
  10. 10. Valois RF, Oeltmann JE, Waller J, Hussey JR (1999) Relationship between number of sexual intercourse partners and selected health risk behaviors among public high school adolescents. J Adol Health 25: 328–35.
  11. 11. Rakowski W, Clark MA, Truchil R, Schneider K, Meersman S (1981) Smoking status and mammography among women aged 50–75 in the 2002 behavioral risk factor surveillance system. Women Health 41: 1–21.
  12. 12. Mitchell SH (1981) Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and nonsmokers. Psychopharmacology 146: 455–64.
  13. 13. Glicksohn J, Nahari G (2007) Interacting personality traits? Smoking as a test case. Eur J of Personality 41: 225–234.
  14. 14. Hahn A, Renner B, Schwarzer R (1998) Perception of health risks: How smokers status affects defensive optimism. Anxiety Stress Coping 11: 93–112.
  15. 15. McKenna FP, Warburton DM, Winwood M (1993) Exploring the limits of optimism: The case of smokers’ decision making. Brit J Psychol 84: 389–94.
  16. 16. Carton S, Jouvent R, Widlocher D (1994) Sensation seeking, nicotine dependence, and smoking motivation in female and male smokers. Addict Behav 19: 219–27.
  17. 17. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson S (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50: 7–15.
  18. 18. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP (1999) Different contributions of the human amygdala and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neuroscience 19: 5473–81.
  19. 19. Yechiam E, Busemeyer JR, Stout JC, Bechara A (2005) Using cognitive models to map relations between neuropsychological disorders and human decision-making deficits. Psychol Sci 16: 973–8.
  20. 20. Verdejo-Garcia A, Benbrook A, Funderburk F, David P, Cadet JL, et al. (2007) The differential relationship between cocaine use and marijuana use on decision-making performance over repeat testing with the Iowa Gambling Task. Drug Alcohol Depend 90: 2–11.
  21. 21. Lejuez CW, Aklin WM, Richards JB, Strong DR, Kahler CW, et al. (2003) The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Exp Clin Psychopharm 11: 26–33.
  22. 22. Aklin WM, Lejuez CW, Zvolensky MJ, Kahler CW, Gwadz M (2005) Evaluation of behavioral measures of risk taking propensity with inner city adolescents. Behav Res Ther 43: 215–28.
  23. 23. West R, Schneider N (1987) Craving for cigarettes. Brit J Addict 82: 407–15.
  24. 24. Baumeister RF, Heatherton TF, Tice DM (1994) Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  25. 25. Muraven M, Baumeister RF (2000) Self regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self control resembles a muscle? Psychol Bull 126: 247–59.
  26. 26. Baumeister RF, Gailliot M, DeWall CN, Oaten M (2006) Self-regulation and personality: How interventions increase regulatory success, and how depletion moderates the effects of traits on behavior. J Pers 74: 1773–802.
  27. 27. Lerman DC, Addison LR, Kodak T (2006) A preliminary analysis of self-control with aversive events: The effects of task magnitude and delay on the choices of children with autism. J Appl Behav Anal 39: 227–232.
  28. 28. Yechiam E, Stout JC, Busemeyer JR, Rock SL, Finn PR (2005) Individual differences in the response to forgone payoffs: An examination of high functioning drug abusers. J Behav Decis Making 18: 97–110.
  29. 29. Caroselli JS, Hiscock M, Scheibel RS, Ingram F (2006) The simulated gambling paradigm applied to young adults: An examination of university students’ performance. Appl Neuropsychol 13: 203–12.
  30. 30. Hertwig R, Barron G, Weber EU, Erev I (2004) Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choices. Psychol Sci 15: 534–9.
  31. 31. Grosskopf B, Erev I, Yechiam E (2006) Foregone with the wind: Indirect payoff information and its implications for choice. Inter J Game Theory 34: 285–302.
  32. 32. Yechiam E, Busemeyer JR (2006) The effect of foregone payoffs on underweighting small probability events. J Behav Decis Making 19: 1–16.
  33. 33. Lane SD, Cherek DR, Pietras CJ, Tcheremissine OV (2004) Alcohol effects on human risk taking. Psychopharmacology 172: 68–77.
  34. 34. Eysenck SBG, Eysenck HJ, Barrett P (1985) A revised version of the psychoticism scale. Pers Indiv Differ 6: 21–9.
  35. 35. Glicksohn J, Abulafia J (1998) Embedding sensation seeking within the big three. Pers Indiv Differ 25: 1085–99.
  36. 36. Glicksohn J, Zilberman N (2010) Gambling on individual differences in decision making. Pers Indiv Differ 48: 557–562.
  37. 37. Raven J (1989) The Raven progressive matrices: An overview of international norming studies. Psychol Test Bull 2: 7–16.
  38. 38. Ert E, Erev I (2007) Replicated alternatives and the role of confusion, chasing, and regret in decisions from experience. J Behav Decis Making 20: 305–22.
  39. 39. McManus IC, Weeks SJ (1982) Smoking, personality, and reasons for smoking. Psych Med 12: 349–56.
  40. 40. Bechara A (2005) Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci 8: 1458–63.
  41. 41. Johnson MW, Bickel WK, Baker F (2007) Moderate drug use and delay discounting: a comparison of heavy, light, and never smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 15: 187–94.
  42. 42. Reynolds B, Karraker K, Horn K, Richards JB (2003) Delay and probability discounting as related to different stages of adolescent smoking and non-smoking. Behav Process 64: 333–44.
  43. 43. World Healthy Organization. (2003). Framework Convention on Tobacco Control (Article 8). Available from: http://www.who.int/fctc/en/
  44. 44. Wakefield M, Chaloupka F, Kaufman N, Orleans C, Barker D, et al. (2000) Effect of restrictions on smoking at home, at school and in public places on teenage smoking: cross sectional study. Brit Med J 321: 333–7.