Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Diagnostic Value of the Impairment of Olfaction in Parkinson's Disease

  • Swaantje Casjens,

    Affiliation Institute for Prevention and Occupational Medicine of the German Social Accident Insurance, Institute of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum (IPA), Bochum, Germany

  • Angelika Eckert,

    Affiliation Institute for Prevention and Occupational Medicine of the German Social Accident Insurance, Institute of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum (IPA), Bochum, Germany

  • Dirk Woitalla,

    Affiliation Neurological Clinic, St. Josef-Hospital, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany

  • Gisa Ellrichmann,

    Affiliation Neurological Clinic, St. Josef-Hospital, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany

  • Michael Turewicz,

    Affiliation Medizinisches Proteom-Center, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany

  • Christian Stephan,

    Affiliation Medizinisches Proteom-Center, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany

  • Martin Eisenacher,

    Affiliation Medizinisches Proteom-Center, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany

  • Caroline May,

    Affiliation Medizinisches Proteom-Center, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany

  • Helmut E. Meyer,

    Affiliation Medizinisches Proteom-Center, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany

  • Thomas Brüning,

    Affiliation Institute for Prevention and Occupational Medicine of the German Social Accident Insurance, Institute of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum (IPA), Bochum, Germany

  • Beate Pesch

    pesch@ipa-dguv.de

    Affiliation Institute for Prevention and Occupational Medicine of the German Social Accident Insurance, Institute of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum (IPA), Bochum, Germany

Abstract

Background

Olfactory impairment is increasingly recognized as an early symptom in the development of Parkinson's disease. Testing olfactory function is a non-invasive method but can be time-consuming which restricts its application in clinical settings and epidemiological studies. Here, we investigate odor identification as a supportive diagnostic tool for Parkinson's disease and estimate the performance of odor subsets to allow a more rapid testing of olfactory impairment.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Odor identification was assessed with 16 Sniffin' sticks in 148 Parkinson patients and 148 healthy controls. Risks of olfactory impairment were estimated with proportional odds models. Random forests were applied to classify Parkinson and non-Parkinson patients. Parkinson patients were rarely normosmic (identification of more than 12 odors; 16.8%) and identified on average seven odors whereas the reference group identified 12 odors and showed a higher prevalence of normosmy (31.1%). Parkinson patients with rigidity dominance had a twofold greater prevalence of olfactory impairment. Disease severity was associated with impairment of odor identification (per score point of the Hoehn and Yahr rating OR 1.87, 95% CI 1.26–2.77). Age-related impairment of olfaction showed a steeper gradient in Parkinson patients. Coffee, peppermint, and anise showed the largest difference in odor identification between Parkinson patients and controls. Random forests estimated a misclassification rate of 22.4% when comparing Parkinson patients with healthy controls using all 16 odors. A similar rate (23.8%) was observed when only the three aforementioned odors were applied.

Conclusions/Significance

Our findings indicate that testing odor identification can be a supportive diagnostic tool for Parkinson's disease. The application of only three odors performed well in discriminating Parkinson patients from controls, which can facilitate a wider application of this method as a point-of-care test.

Introduction

Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disorder characterized by movement disorders, such as tremor at rest, bradykinesia, and rigidity, which are mainly due to nigrostriatal dopamine deficiency that can be alleviated by Levodopa [1]. Although degeneration of the substantia nigra pars compacta is considered a neuropathologic hallmark of PD, the neurodegenerative process also includes extranigral structures, such as the olfactory bulb resulting in an impairment of olfaction [2]. More than 90% of PD patients are diagnosed with olfactory impairment. The high prevalence and early occurrence of the olfactory impairment suggest that the test for olfactory dysfunction can be a supportive diagnostic tool for PD [3], [4]. Furthermore, the development of symptoms like tremor or rigidity indicates that PD is a heterogeneous disease with progress in severity. Olfactory impairment may accompany disease progression [5] and vary by subtype based on specific clinical criteria [6].

An optimal diagnostic test with widespread application should be robust and easy to perform. The investigation of odor impairment is such a non-invasive tool that can be applied in research and clinical settings in addition to costly diagnostic methods. A defined set of odorants is consecutively presented to the participant in a multiple forced choice format. Most studies on olfactory impairment in PD, from both the United States and Europe, have focused on odor identification using either the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT) [7] or a test battery with Sniffin' sticks [8], which showed a good agreement with UPSIT [9].

The application of point-of-care tests should not be time consuming to ensure their wide application. Odor identification requires several minutes per odor in order to reduce carry-over effects between the provided odors. Various efforts have been made to estimate the performance of individual odors in the assessment of olfactory impairment for a quicker smell test [10][12]. Candidate odors should achieve a similar good discrimination of PD patients compared to the full sets of olfactory probes.

We used Sniffin' sticks to assess odor identification in both PD patients and controls. The present study explores the association of olfactory impairment with disease severity and with the subtype of PD. The performance of single and combined odors to discriminate PD from participants without PD is assessed in comparison to the full set of odors to establish a brief olfaction test.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by an independent ethic committee of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum (amendment of AZ3184-08) and conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were given full information regarding the study, signed to indicate their consent to take part, and could withdraw at any time. Participation, withdrawal or not, had no effect on the health care or other services provided. All comments and information were kept confidential, and patient identifying information was not recorded on the questionnaires, but kept separately from their consent forms.

Study groups

ParkCHIP was a cross-sectional study in 148 PD patients and 148 controls. The control group was unrelated to PD patients and frequency-matched to cases by gender and age. All controls were free of neurodegenerative diseases. This analysis was conducted in 296 subjects with complete information on odor identification assessed with 16 Sniffin' sticks from January 2010 to September 2011. Subjects who suffered loss of olfaction after surgery, basilar skull fracture, or head trauma were non-eligible for this analysis. Furthermore, subjects with severe cognitive impairment, drug addiction, HIV positive status, or insufficient German language skills were not enrolled for this study. A questionnaire was applied to assess socio-demographic information, medications, and other data. A physician enrolled and diagnosed eligible subjects with written informed consent.

PD patients had a validated diagnosis according to the criteria of the UK Parkinson's Disease Society Brain Bank [13]. Patients receiving dopaminergic medication had to respond to this medication as obligate inclusion criteria in order to improve diagnostic accuracy. Patients who were not under medical treatment underwent functional imaging via Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography to support the diagnosis. Disease severity was assessed using the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) [14], the Movement Disorder Society-Sponsored Revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS) [15], as well as the Hoehn and Yahr scale [16]. Movement disorders were documented using video recording with the permission of the patient. Physical examination by a neurologist (D.W. or G.E.) determined whether the patients were categorized with ‘tremor dominance’ (N = 38), ‘rigidity dominance’ (N = 90), or as ‘others’ (N = 20).

Testing odor identification

Sniffin' sticks were applied to assess the identification of 16 odors. Odors were presented in felt-tip pens. The individual pens were consecutively placed in front of both nostrils at a distance of approximately 1–2 cm. The participants could identify the odor as a multiple-choice task from a list of four potential answers [8], [17]. Subjects were classified as normosmic if more than 12 odors were identified, hyposmic if 8 to 12 odors were identified, and anosmic if less than 8 odors were identified [18].

Examination of disability, motor disorders, and cognitive skills

All participants underwent a routine neurological examination to test for signs of neurological disorders. A physician (D.W. or G.E.) assessed tremor at rest, postural tremor, rigidity, hypokinesia, as well as postural instability. The disability index was scored using the Health Assessment Questionnaire (HAQ) [19]. Cognitive skills were scored by the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the clock drawing test [20].

Statistical analysis

Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) were used to describe the distribution of continuous variables. Study groups were compared using the Kruskal-Wallis test for continuous variables and the chi-square test for categorical data. The relative risks of olfactory impairment (normosmia as reference, hyposmia, and anosmia) were estimated with proportional odds models [21] where the proportional odds ratio (OR) for a predictor can be interpreted as a summary of the risk estimates obtained from separate binary logistic regressions using all cut-points of the ordinal outcome. Age was implemented in groups (<45, 45–65, 66–79, ≥80 years) or as binary variable with 65 years as cut-off. The age-dependent risk of the impairment of olfaction was estimated for diseased and non-diseased subjects with regard to a common reference group (non-diseased subjects aged 45–65) and separately within each study group. We adjusted the association between movement disorders and age for cognitive impairment assessed with the clock drawing test. Random forests with 10-fold cross-validation were performed to evaluate the best discriminating odors between the study groups based on the permutation accuracy using the package randomForest [22], [23] in R 2.13 [23], [24]. For each tree of the random forest the misclassification rate with and without permutation of each odor was recorded. The permutation accuracy was then defined as the difference between the two misclassification rates averaged over all trees and normalized by the standard deviation of the differences. High values of the permutation accuracy indicate important variables. Other analyses were performed with SAS/STAT and SAS/IML software, version 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).

Results

Description of the study groups

Table 1 presents the characteristics of the PD patients in comparison with the controls. PD patients were on average 67 years old (IQR 59–73 years). The age distribution of controls was similar (IQR 56–72 years). Compared to the controls, a higher proportion of the PD patients were well-educated and never smokers (54.1% vs. 40.5% in controls). They showed a good performance in cognitive tests, but had a higher degree of disability compared to the control group. No significant differences, according to these characteristics, were found between the two subgroups of PD, showing either tremor or rigidity dominance (supplemental Table S1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) and controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.t001

Odor identification in PD patients and controls

Table 2 depicts the impairment of odor identification in PD patients, who identified on average 7 out of 16 odors, compared to a median of 12 in the controls. Every other PD patient (56.8%) was anosmic in contrast to 6.8% in the control group. Ten PD patients (6.8%) were normosmic and were on average 13 years younger than the other PD patients. Although the difference in odor identification was not statistically significant between the two PD subgroups (p = 0.22), 62.2% of the patients with rigidity dominance were anosmic.

thumbnail
Table 2. Odor identification in the study groups assessed with 16 Sniffin' sticks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.t002

Proportional odds models revealed that age was a strong confounder in both groups and showed a non-linear shape of the impairment of odor identification assessed as anosmia or hyposmia (Table 3). Female gender and current smoking were associated with improved odor identification; however, the association was not significant (data not shown). A lower cognitive performance assessed with the clock drawing test (OR 0.83 (95% confidence interval (CI) 0.71–0.98)) but not with the MMSE (OR 0.96 (95% CI 0.85–1.09)) was significantly related to an impairment of odor identification.

thumbnail
Table 3. Estimates of proportional odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) of Parkinson's disease and age with joint effects on impairment of olfaction (anosmia or hyposmia) adjusted by the clock drawing test result.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.t003

Table 3 presents the joint effects of study group and age on olfactory impairment, adjusted for cognitive performance using the clock drawing test. In addition, we estimated proportional odds of the age-related impairment of odor identification for each study group. PD patients aged 45 to 65 years were 12.74 (95% CI 5.81–27.94) times more likely to show anosmia or hyposmia compared to controls within the same age group, and PD patients aged ≥80 years had an OR of 74.01 (95% CI 8.17–670.68). The corresponding OR for impairment of olfaction was in controls aged ≥80 years 17.49 (95% CI 3.10–98.66) in comparison to controls aged 45 to 65 years. Comparing aged PD patients (≥80 years) with younger PD patients (45–65 years), the relative change in impairment was 5.32 (95% CI 0.59–47.73).

Table 4 shows these joint effects for the subgroups of PD using controls aged <65 years as the reference group. PD patients with rigidity dominance had higher (but not statistically significant) ORs for olfactory impairment than patients with tremor dominance, even at a younger age (<65 years: 12.72, 95% CI 5.67–28.53 vs. 6.56, 95% CI 2.12–20.35).

thumbnail
Table 4. Estimates of proportional odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) of subgroups of Parkinson's disease (PD) and age with joint effects on impairment of olfaction (anosmia and hyposmia) adjusted by the clock drawing test result.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.t004

The relative risk of olfactory impairment increased with disease severity in PD patients (Table 5). These changes were significant for the Hoehn and Yahr rating scale (per score point: OR 1.87; 95% CI 1.26–2.77) and the Clinician Global Impression of Disease Severity (per score point: OR 1.65; 95% CI 1.06–2.57). The results were marginally significant for the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) and the Movement Disorder Society-Sponsored Revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS). The association with the UPDRS-III motor scale was weaker.

thumbnail
Table 5. Estimates of proportional odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) of disease severity on impairment of olfaction (anosmia and hyposmia) in Parkinson patients with age adjustment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.t005

Table 6 depicts the group differences in identifying individual odors. Peppermint and fish ranked highest among the odors that were correctly identified by the controls. The odor of apples was rarely identified by both groups. Apart from cinnamon and garlic, PD patients showed a significant impairment in the ability to identify all other single odors when compared to the controls. Coffee, peppermint, and anise showed the largest differences between both groups. PD subgroups similarly identified most odors, including peppermint (p value 0.82), but differed in the ability to smell oranges.

thumbnail
Table 6. Correctly identified odors in the study groups sorted by the difference between controls (HC) and patients with Parkinson's disease (PD).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.t006

The misclassification rate of the full set of odors was 22.4% for PD vs. control group. Table 7 shows the misclassification rates of the study groups when using subsets of the Sniffin' sticks. Peppermint, anise, and coffee were the top-3 odors based on the highest variable importance according to random forests, and achieved a similar misclassification rate (23.8%). A 5-odor set was proposed by Mueller and colleagues and consists of orange, leather, peppermint, rose, and fish [12]. This short test yielded similar rates (24.1%). Hummel's quick olfactory test (q-Sticks) [11] containing coffee, clove, and rose led to a misclassification rate of 27.0%, and non-food odors were associated with a rate of 31.9%. The Sniffin' sticks were less successful in the classification of PD subtypes (data not shown).

thumbnail
Table 7. Classification of patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) based on Sniffin' sticks using random forests on all and selected odors with ten-fold cross-validation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.t007

Figure 1 shows the ranking of the odors with regard to their importance in achieving a good accuracy of classification based on the permutation accuracy of random forests. Peppermint and coffee ranked highest, followed by anise and fish, whereas odors with negative values like apple, cinnamon and garlic did not contribute to an accurate classification of the study groups.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Ranking of the odors with regard to their importance in achieving a good accuracy of classification based on the permutation accuracy of random forests.

High values of the permutation accuracy indicate important variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.g001

We estimated the sensitivity and specificity of the quick 3-odor test with peppermint, anise, and coffee depending on the decision rule for odor impairment. A specificity of 99% and a sensitivity of 28% were achieved for the classification of PD and controls when the decision rule for impairment was to identify none of the three odors correctly. A higher sensitivity of 61% was possible at the expense of specificity (88%) when the decision rule allows the detection of one of the three odors.

Discussion

PD is the second most common neurodegenerative disease where point-of-care tests for early detection hold great promise in adjunct to more costly investigations for an improvement of the diagnostic accuracy [1]. Screening tools for early detection of any disease require simple and non-invasive methods before symptoms actually become detectable, usually signs of more severe damage. In general, motor symptoms occur late in PD, when the nigostriatal dopaminergic system is degenerated to a larger extent, whereas an impairment of olfaction is considered an early sign of PD [4]. Odor identification can be performed with robust tests. For example, the ParkCHIP study aimed at improving the classification of PD with diagnostic markers. Here, we evaluated the performance of a brief odor-identification test based on Sniffin' sticks as a supportive diagnostic tool. The present analysis confirmed that odor impairment is highly prevalent in PD patients, for example when compared with the prevalence rates estimated in large population surveys in older adults [25][27]. Our statistical model demonstrated that subsets of three to five odors have a similar good performance to discriminate PD compared to the full set of 16 sticks. A brief examination allows a wider application of olfaction testing as a quick point-of-care test in clinical settings and large epidemiological studies.

Our results revealed that various brief test versions of odor identification are sufficiently sensitive to detect severe olfactory dysfunction. This raises the question of which odors should be selected for a quick test. Our top-3 set contained food-related odors, whereas non-food odors had a lower performance of classifying PD, which is in line with a large population survey [27]. Our statistical approach revealed peppermint, coffee, and anise as the best-performing 3-odor set for the discrimination of PD patients from all participants without PD. We also found these odors among other sets that have been previously suggested as brief tests. Usually, these sets of odors were not derived from statistical modeling. If several odors perform well like anise and fish, the statistical approach selects the best ones from the specified dataset of a defined study population. The application of the same statistical model to other study populations may reveal slightly different top-3 sets. For example, coffee together with rose and clove have been proposed as another quick test [11]. This odor set showed only a slightly different misclassification rate due to the lower identification rate of anise, compared to rose and clove among our PD patients. Anise is among the top-3 odors of another study together with cinnamon and licorice [10]. This set led to a slightly higher misclassification rate because cinnamon was identified by 44.6% of the PD patients. Peppermint was part of a 5-odor set together with fish, rose, orange, and leather [12], [27]. An odor can only perform well in a classification algorithm if a high fraction of the controls can identify this odor correctly in combination with a low proportion of PD patients that is still able to identify this odor. The strong odors of peppermint and fish were identified in about 90% of the control patients, which confirms an investigation in healthy Dutch subjects [28]. In summary, a quick 3-odor test detects a severe olfactory dysfunction rather than a selective olfactory disability in PD patients.

Sensitivity and specificity are popular measures of the performance of tests as a diagnostic classifier. A high specificity is important for screening tools to avoid invasive or cost-intensive diagnostic workups when subjects are tested positive. When applying a strong decision rule, i.e. none of the three best-classifying odors was detected, we achieved very good specificity of 99% at a sensitivity of nearly 30%. This supports that olfaction testing can serve as a good diagnostic adjunct to other examinations.

Overall, the Sniffin' sticks successfully discriminated PD patients from non-diseased subjects. Despite a generally good classification of PD patients from controls based on their impairment of olfaction, ten out of 148 PD cases were tested normosmic, of all whom were on average younger. Age was a strong confounder, which was already shown in various population surveys [25], [29]. The shape of the age-related impairment showed a non-linear trend. The estimate of the age-related impairment of odor identification increased steeply up to a very high OR in PD patients compared with controls aged 45–65 years. However, the relative change in loss of odor identification was less strong when compared with patients aged 45–65 years. These younger patients had already a significant impairment of their olfaction compared to the controls of a similar age.

We further found an association between disease severity assessed with various scales and olfactory impairment. This observation is in line with a report by Morley and coworkers [30], but contradicts the findings of others where such a relationship could not be detected [3], [31]. The characteristics of the study population, the size of the study, the applied test and scales, and an adjustment for observer or study center could influence whether such an association can be captured. In ParkCHIP, there were only two raters from the same hospital, and all patients fulfilled the criteria for a diagnosis of PD. This association was somewhat stronger for the Hoehn and Yahr scale and the Clinician Global Impression of Disease Severity scale. Both are brief scales in contrast to UPDRS or related scales. Larger scales include unspecific questions, for example on depression, which may attenuate the score. Also, Tissingh and colleagues observed a significant impact of the Hoehn and Yahr stage on the ability to discriminate odors, but not for the UPDRS motor scale [5].

Furthermore, we observed a slightly higher risk of olfactory impairment in PD patients with rigidity dominance when using the full set of Sniffin' sticks that was not statistically significant. This is in line with Stern et al. and Iijima et al. [6], [32] but in contrast to others [3], [33]. Studies with PD patients are often not very large in size; therefore when analyzing subgroups, the statistical power is limited to ensure that a small difference in olfaction is not a spurious finding.

Several other factors may also influence olfactory impairment [34]. In line with other studies, we observed an association between impairment of olfaction and cognitive function [35], [36]. Again, the choice of a sensitive test plays a role. Here, the clock drawing test but not the MMSE showed a significant association. We found some indication for lower risks of olfactory impairment in women and current smokers, supporting results from previous large population-based studies [29], [37].

Conclusions

Our results provide further support of an association between olfactory impairment as a non-motor manifestation and severity of PD. Brief odor identification tests perform well in discriminating PD and can likely be used as a point-of-care test in clinical settings, together with other examinations in PD diagnostics or in epidemiological studies exploring risk factors for neurodegeneration. A brief test of odor identification is a non-invasive, robust and cost efficient method, which might increase the acceptance among patients and healthy subjects.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Characteristics of patients with Parkinson's disease and dominance of tremor or rigidity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064735.s001

(DOC)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: DW HEM TB BP. Performed the experiments: DW GE. Analyzed the data: SC. Wrote the paper: SC AE BP. Critical review of the manuscript: DW GE MT CS ME CM HEM TB.

References

  1. 1. Brooks DJ (1998) The early diagnosis of Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol 44: S10–S18.
  2. 2. Braak H, Del TK, Rub U, de Vos RA, Jansen Steur EN, et al. (2003) Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Aging 24: 197–211.
  3. 3. Haehner A, Boesveldt S, Berendse HW, Mackay-Sim A, Fleischmann J, et al. (2009) Prevalence of smell loss in Parkinson's disease – a multicenter study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 15: 490–494.
  4. 4. Ponsen MM, Stoffers D, Booij J, van Eck-Smit BL, Wolters EC, et al. (2004) Idiopathic hyposmia as a preclinical sign of Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol 56: 173–181.
  5. 5. Tissingh G, Berendse HW, Bergmans P, DeWaard R, Drukarch B, et al. (2001) Loss of olfaction in de novo and treated Parkinson's disease: possible implications for early diagnosis. Mov Disord 16: 41–46.
  6. 6. Stern MB, Doty RL, Dotti M, Corcoran P, Crawford D, et al. (1994) Olfactory function in Parkinson's disease subtypes. Neurology 44: 266–268.
  7. 7. Doty RL, Shaman P, Dann M (1984) Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav 32: 489–502.
  8. 8. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G (1997) ‘Sniffin’ sticks': olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses 22: 39–52.
  9. 9. Silveira-Moriyama L, Carvalho MJ, Katzenschlager R, Petrie A, Ranvaud R, et al. (2008) The use of smell identification tests in the diagnosis of Parkinson's disease in Brazil. Mov Disord 23: 2328–2334.
  10. 10. Boesveldt S, Verbaan D, Knol DL, Visser M, van Rooden SM, et al. (2008) A comparative study of odor identification and odor discrimination deficits in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 23: 1984–1990.
  11. 11. Hummel T, Pfetzing U, Lotsch J (2010) A short olfactory test based on the identification of three odors. J Neurol 257: 1316–1321.
  12. 12. Mueller C, Renner B (2006) A new procedure for the short screening of olfactory function using five items from the “Sniffin' Sticks” identification test kit. Am J Rhinol 20: 113–116.
  13. 13. Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ (1992) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55: 181–184.
  14. 14. Fahn S, Elton R (1987) Unified Parkinson's disease rating scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Calne D, Goldstein M, editors. Recent developments in Parkinson's disease. Florham Park: MacMillan Health Care Information. 153–163.
  15. 15. Goetz CG, Fahn S, Martinez-Martin P, Poewe W, Sampaio C, et al. (2007) Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): Process, format, and clinimetric testing plan. Mov Disord 22: 41–47.
  16. 16. Hoehn MM, Yahr MD (1967) Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 17: 427–442.
  17. 17. Kobal G, Hummel T, Sekinger B, Barz S, Roscher S, et al. (1996) “Sniffin' sticks”: screening of olfactory performance. Rhinology 34: 222–226.
  18. 18. Kobal G, Klimek L, Wolfensberger M, Gudziol H, Temmel A, et al. (2000) Multicenter investigation of 1,036 subjects using a standardized method for the assessment of olfactory function combining tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 257: 205–211.
  19. 19. Bruce B, Fries JF (2005) The Health Assessment Questionnaire (HAQ). Clin Exp Rheumatol 23: S14–S18.
  20. 20. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12: 189–198.
  21. 21. McCullagh P (1980) Regression models for ordinal data. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B 42: 109–142.
  22. 22. Liaw A, Wiener M (2002) Classification and regression by randomForest. R News 2: 18–22.
  23. 23. Breiman L (2001) Random forests. Machine Learning 45: 5–32.
  24. 24. R Development Core Team (2011) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, version R·version·2.1.3 [computer program]. Wien, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  25. 25. Murphy C, Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, Klein BE, Klein R, et al. (2002) Prevalence of olfactory impairment in older adults. JAMA 288: 2307–2312.
  26. 26. Landis BN, Konnerth CG, Hummel T (2004) A study on the frequency of olfactory dysfunction. Laryngoscope 114: 1764–1769.
  27. 27. Boesveldt S, Lindau ST, McClintock MK, Hummel T, Lundstrom JN (2011) Gustatory and olfactory dysfunction in older adults: a national probability study. Rhinology 49: 324–330.
  28. 28. Boesveldt S, Verbaan D, Knol DL, Van Hilten JJ, Berendse HW (2008) Odour identification and discrimination in Dutch adults over 45 years. Rhinology 46: 131–136.
  29. 29. Brämerson A, Johansson L, Ek L, Nordin S, Bende M (2004) Prevalence of olfactory dysfunction: the skovde population-based study. Laryngoscope 114: 733–737.
  30. 30. Morley JF, Weintraub D, Mamikonyan E, Moberg PJ, Siderowf AD, et al. (2011) Olfactory dysfunction is associated with neuropsychiatric manifestations in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 26: 2051–2057.
  31. 31. Haehner A, Hummel T, Reichmann H (2009) Olfactory dysfunction as a diagnostic marker for Parkinson's disease. Expert Rev Neurother 9: 1773–1779.
  32. 32. Iijima M, Kobayakawa T, Saito S, Osawa M, Tsutsumi Y, et al. (2011) Differences in odor identification among clinical subtypes of Parkinson's disease. Eur J Neurol 18: 425–429.
  33. 33. Verbaan D, Boesveldt S, van Rooden SM, Visser M, Marinus J, et al. (2008) Is olfactory impairment in Parkinson disease related to phenotypic or genotypic characteristics? Neurology 71: 1877–1882.
  34. 34. Doty RL, Petersen I, Mensah N, Christensen K (2011) Genetic and environmental influences on odor identification ability in the very old. Psychol Aging 26: 864–871.
  35. 35. Damholdt MF, Borghammer P, Larsen L, Ostergaard K (2011) Odor identification deficits identify Parkinson's disease patients with poor cognitive performance. Mov Disord 26: 2045–2050.
  36. 36. Bohnen NI, Muller ML, Kotagal V, Koeppe RA, Kilbourn MA, et al. (2010) Olfactory dysfunction, central cholinergic integrity and cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Brain 133: 1747–1754.
  37. 37. Vennemann MM, Hummel T, Berger K (2008) The association between smoking and smell and taste impairment in the general population. J Neurol 255: 1121–1126.