Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Hourglass Mechanism with Temperature Compensation in the Diel Periodicity of Planulation of the Coral, Seriatopora hystrix

  • Che-Hung Lin,

    Affiliation Institute of Marine Biology, National Sun Yat-Sen University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan, R.O.C.

  • Keryea Soong,

    Affiliation Institute of Marine Biology, National Sun Yat-Sen University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan, R.O.C.

  • Tung-Yung Fan

    tyfan@nmmba.gov.tw

    Affiliations National Museum of Marine Biology and Aquarium, Pingtung, Taiwan, R.O.C., Institute of Marine Biodiversity and Evolution, National Dong Hwa University, Pingtung, Taiwan, R.O.C.

Abstract

Brooding corals often exhibit daily planulation peaks in certain phases of lunar months during their reproductive season. The mechanism controlling this diurnal phenomenon, however, remains uncertain. Seriatopora hystrix populations of Southern Taiwan exhibit a highly synchronized planulation rhythm characterized by pre-dawn peak release episodes over a period of 4–6 days per month throughout the year. In this study, controlled light-dark cycles and temperatures were used to study the mechanism of the diel planulation rhythm in the laboratory. Seriatopora hystrix did not release planulae under continuous light or continuous darkness. Thus, the lack of free-run did not support a regulatory mechanism involving an endogenous oscillator. Under some lighting conditions, planula release occurred in two peaks per day. Both peaks always occur under dark phases of various light-dark cycles in the laboratory. The first peak occurred when the dark period just started, which might be stimulated by an abrupt transition between light and darkness. The second, pre-dawn peak consistently occurred 23 hrs after application of light. We concluded that this peak of planula release of S. hystrix was cued by sunrise of the previous day. Temperature treatments at 1°C intervals from 23.5–28.5°C did not change the diel time of planula release. We suggest that the temperature compensation exhibited in the hourglass model of this species may have a common origin as that of the oscillator model of circadian clocks, due to the similar duration (23 h) and period (∼24 h) between the two. The timing mechanism of planulation discovered here represents an intermediate stage between the hourglass and the oscillator models.

Introduction

Biological activities often follow a diel pattern [1]. For example, sponges [2], [3], ascidians [4], [5] and polychaetes [6] display diel patterns in different aspects of larvae release, e.g., timing, numbers of peaks and duration of peaks. Spawning of marine organisms such as corals is no exception. Depending on species, gametes or embryos are usually released at certain hours of a day [7], [8], [9], [10], [11]. In some species, a rhythmic pattern could be detected where spawning or planulation occurred in several consecutive days [12].

Two models are often implemented to explain biological rhythms: the endogenous oscillator model and the hourglass model [13]. In the former, clock-like rhythms are autonomous and do not require a cyclic environmental trigger. Pittendrigh [14] summarized that circadian rhythms controlled by a biological clock are characterized by three major features. First, the oscillation can be sustained without external cues (free-running). Second, the rhythmic activities can be entrained by environmental cycles in processes such as light/dark cycles or feeding times (entrainment). Third, despite variations in the surrounding environment, e.g., changes in temperature, an oscillator exhibits little deviation in its period (temperature compensation).

In contrast, in the hourglass model, a rhythm is maintained only because there is oscillation of external environmental cues. The elapsed time between the cue and the expressed activities in the hourglass model corresponds to the reaction time of certain internal processes.

Because temperature controls rates of most chemical reactions, it is often essential in determining the intervals between the environmental cues and the expressed biological activities [15]. Following Van Hoff's Law an increase of 10°C doubles metabolic rates, on average, in a poikilotherm [16], and the estimation of temperature effects could be assessed by:(Q10: temperature coefficient, R: metabolic rate, T: temperature).

Hug et al. [17] showed a possibility of hourglass model with temperature compensation theoretically, but so far, no temperature compensation in the hourglass model has been published in living organism, as far as we know.

At least 74 scleractinian coral species are known to be brooders [18]. Most planulation studies focus on annual and lunar reproductive cycles. Planulation is also known to occur in certain hours of a day, most of them at night and before sunrise [11], [12], [19], [20], [21]. The diel timing of planulation may have a significant impact on post-metamorphic success. For example, the settlement rates, metamorphic success and growth rates of pre-dawn planulae were significantly higher than those of post-dusk planulae in the brooding coral, Favia fragum [20].

To date, although many studies have described diel times of planulation, which were observed under natural conditions, the mechanisms controlling such rhythmic activities remain little explored. Nevertheless, in Pocillopora damicornis, for example, the planulation time was suggested to be controlled by tide [22], [23] and was not affected by light-dark cycles.

Seriatopora hystrix is a brooder that releases planulae year round and has a lunar and diel periodicity of planulation: during new moon and 1st quarter moon for 2–3 hours before sunrise in southern Taiwan [11]. The year round reproductive activities provide an opportunity to study the mechanisms. In this research, we aimed to answer the following questions in this work:

  1. Is the cyclic planulation of S. hystrix directly due to cyclic external light conditions, or, alternatively, an endogenous clock is involved in the rhythm?
  2. What is the environmental factor, i.e. sunrise or sunset, determining the diel rhythm of planula release?
  3. Does temperature affect the diel timing of planula release?

Materials and Methods

Collection sites

All samples in this study were collected with permissions obtained from the Kenting National Park Headquarters. Colonies of S. hystrix were collected at 3–10 m depth from two fringing reefs of Southern Taiwan, Leidashyr (21°55′89″N, 120°45′69″E) and Hobihu (21°56′29″N, 120°44′70″E). Colonies collected in whole were harvested using a hammer and a chisel before new moons, with maximum diameters ranged from 7–23 cm. The average colony size was 14.7±5.4 cm (mean ± S.D, n = 50), and all corals were kept in the culturing facility in National Museum of Marine Biology and Aquarium. Each colony was placed in a separate flow-through tank, (22×15×18 cm L × W × H) in dimensions, with an overflowing outlet of 2 cm in diameter. The source seawater was pumped from a 5-m depth, filtered to 50 µm, and injected into the bottom of the tanks. The outflow of each tank passed through a collecting cup with a sieve made from a plankton net (mesh size 100 μm) to trap the released planulae. The planulae collected in each cup were counted hourly, during the experiments.

Tanks were placed in a dark room with light controls. Each treatment equipped with two OSRAM HQI-TS 70W/D metal halide lamps (photosynthetically active radiation (PAR) ∼150μE m2 s1) was subjected to different light regimes.

Free-run experiment

Twelve adult colonies of S. hystrix were collected in February, 2006 and were kept under the condition of LD (light-dark) cycle 11∶13 (light-on at 0700 hr). The LD cycle was designed in order to simulate that in the field. Planulation occurred twelve days later. After the start of planulation, four colonies were used in each of the three treatments. In the control groups, the light regime was maintained at LD 11∶13 throughout the experiment. In the LL (continual light) treatment groups, the application of continuous light sustained for 90 hours then changed to LD 11∶13. In the DD (continual dark) treatment groups, continuous darkness lasted for 97 hours, after which colonies were exposed to LD 11∶13. Data from each colony of the same treatments were pooled.

Entrainment experiment

Twenty adult colonies were collected and maintained at LD 12∶12 (light-on at 0700 hr, light-off at 1900 hr) for nine days until the start of planulation in January, 2007. Then, four colonies were used in each of the five experimental treatments. In the control groups, an LD 12∶12 was used, with light-on at 0700 hr. To examine the effect of light-on time, an LD 19∶5, with light-on at 0000 hr and light-off at 1900 hr was used in group A, and an LD 5∶19, with light-on at 1400 hr and light-off at 1900 hr was used in group B, respectively. To examine the effect of light-off time, an LD 19∶5, with light-off at 0200 hr and light-on at 0700 hr was used in group C, and an LD 5∶19, with light-off at 1200 hr and light-on at 0700 hr was used in group D, The numbers of planulae released by each colony were recorded hourly for five consecutive days.

Temperature compensation experiment

Eighteen adult colonies were collected and maintained at LD 12∶12 in March, 2007. After the colonies started releasing planulae, they were randomly divided into 6 groups. Each group was assigned to one of the water temperature treatments at 23.5, 24.5, 25.5, 26.5, 27.5 and 28.5°C. Three colonies, in separate flow-through tanks, were used in each temperature treatment. In order to manipulate inflow seawater to the corresponding designated temperatures, six 130-liter PVC tanks equipped with heaters and chillers were used. Temperature-manipulated water then was pumped to individual flow-through tanks in each treatment, respectively. HOBO Temperature Data Loggers (Onset Computer Corp., Bourne, MA) were used to record the temperature every 5 minutes in the treatment tanks to keep track of the experimental conditions. The numbers of planulae released were recorded every hour for three consecutive days. Data from each colony were transformed to percentages each day. The average times of planula release in each temperature treatment were used to calculate Q10, using the formula mentioned prevously.

The data of bleached colonies or those releasing fewer than 10 planulae per day per colony were excluded from the analyses.

Results

Free-run experiment

In the control group with LD 11∶13, the rhythmic planulation peak occurred around 0500, i.e., 22.9±0.14 hrs (mean ± S.E.) after the onset of light, every day. A few planulae (fewer than 20 planulae/colony) were released 1–2 hours after the start of light-off (Fig. 1). In LL group, no planula release was observed for 90 consecutive hours until light-off, when a high peak of planulation occurred; there was no pre-dawn peak of planulation (Fig. 1). In DD, a peak occurred at 0500 on the first day, then no planulae were released for 96 h of continuous darkness. A peak of planulation was observed toward the end of the dark phase, after LD cycle resumed (Fig. 1). Free-running was not observed in this experiment.

thumbnail
Figure 1. The distribution of planula release in the free-run experiment.

Data are expressed as mean ± S.E., n =  number of coral colonies in each treatment, N =  total number of planulae in each treatment. Note that only two colonies in the DD treatment released planulae during the experiment. The gray and white areas represent the dark and light phases, respectively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064584.g001

Entrainment experiment

In this experiment, daily synchronous planulation occurred in two main peaks, the first peaks occurred after light-off and second peaks occurred before light-on during the experimental period (Fig. 2).

thumbnail
Figure 2. The effect of light regime on planulae release in the entrainment experiment.

Data are expressed as mean ± S.E., n =  coral colonies in each treatment, N =  total number of planulae in each treatment. Note that only two colonies in the control group released planulae during the experiment. The gray and white areas represent the dark and light phases, respectively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064584.g002

The second peak of group A was about 7 hours earlier than the control group; the second peak of group B was about 7 hours later than that in the control group; the second peak of groups C and D were similar to those of the control group (Fig. 2).

The intervals between light-on and the second peak of planulation were about 23 hours in the control group. In comparing treatments, a delay in hours of light-on (X) resulted in a corresponding delay in the second peak of planulation (Y) (Y = 0.93X+23.12 (h), P<0.01, R2 = 0.95, 99% C.I. of slope  = 0.08, Fig. 3). Thus, the intervals between lights-on and the second peaks of planulation were approximately 23 hours, despite various photoperiods.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Regression analysis between planulation time and the delay of light-on in the entrainment experiment.

Numbers next to solid dots indicate numbers of observations. Data from Figure 2. Each dot represents data from one colony on each day.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064584.g003

On the other hand, there was no relationship between the second peak of planulation and the time of light-off (P = 0.31, R2 = 0.02). This result indicated the role of light-on, but not light-off, on the diel periodicity of the second peak of planulation.

Temperature compensation experiment

All of the planulation events occurred during the dark period in this experiment. The timing of the main planulation peaks was similar among treatments, that is, 23.15±0.09 hrs (Fig. 4) after application of light. The mean water temperature during the experimental period received by each treatment tank respectively were 23.62±0.2, 24.5±0.19, 25.52±0.23, 26.2±0.19, 27.55±0.33 and 28.34±0.15°C (23.5, 24.5, 25.5, 26.5, 27.5 and F treatment, mean ± SD). A regression analysis revealed no significant relationship between peaks of planulation time (Y) and water temperatures (X) (Y = −0.0017X +23.2 (h), P = 0.97); moreover, the calculated Q10 of 1.01 was not significantly different from 1.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Planulae release time under different temperature treatments.

Data from different colonies are shown in separate lines. N =  total number of planulae in each treatment, T =  actual temperature in each treatment (mean ± S.D.). There were no significant correlations between elapsed time (interval between light-on and the planulation peak) and temperature (P = 0.97, Q10 = 1.01).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064584.g004

Discussion

In this study, we found that S. hystrix colonies did not release planulae during continuous light and continuous dark conditions (Fig. 1). This result suggests the absence of free-runing which is the property of a circadian oscillator. Furthermore, the timing of planulae release corresponded with shifting in timing of light-on on the days of the treatments (Figs. 2 and 3). Thus, the hourglass model apply here with regard to the timing of larval release. It appears, on the other hand, that the onset of light phase is the trigger that initiates the processes leading to planulation in S. hystrix. Therefore, we surmise that the rhythmic planulation of S. hystrix is due to the cyclic sunrise in nature. Interestingly, the oscillator model of coral spawning was also rejected in a study of the broadcasting coral, Montastraea franksi, in which spawn timing was directly controlled by sunset time [24].

In this study, the observed pattern of the two-peak planulation strategy differed from the results of a previous study [11] in which only one planulation peak (pre-dawn) occurred in a day. We suggest that an abrupt transition from light to dark may have a role in the occurrence of these planulation peaks. In our experiment, “rectangular” light/dark stimulations (i.e., abrupt transition from light to dark and vice versa) was employed. On the other hand, Fan et al. [11] used natural light, which includes twilight and dusk periods (i.e., gradual transitions from light to dark and vice versa). Similar results were also observed in mammals. Specifically, it is evident that twilight periods increase the strength of the LD synchronizer, as compared with studies using abrupt LD transitions, in squirrel monkeys [25] and hamsters [26], [27]. However, similar activity patterns were observed in birds exposed to these two regimes [28].

The phenomenon of two-peak planulation under natural light condition was reported in P. damicornis and Stylophora pistillata, in the same family as S. hystrix. In P. damicornis, planulation occurred throughout the day with one major pre-dawn peak and one minor post-dusk peak [11]. Planulation of S. pistillata was shown to have one pre-dawn peak, but sometimes, a slight peak occurred after sunset (see figure 3 of [11]). The two-peak planulation strategy in these three species of relatively recent common ancestry suggests that the minor post-dusk peak could be a vestigial trait. This hypothesis is supported by the phenomenon that the post-dusk planulation peaks occurred only under certain artificial light condition in S. hystrix.

The two planulation peaks demonstrated by S. hystrix within the dark phase of one day brings to mind the two locomotor activity peaks in Drosophila pseudoobscura [29]. In this fruit fly, the appearance of the first peak of activity after lights were turned on was due to a masking effect, while the second peak (before light-off) was triggered by an oscillator. In S. hystrix, the post-dusk peaks and the pre-dawn peaks may be regulated by the masking effect and the hourglass model, respectively. In this case, the post-dusk peak of planulation could also be an hourglass mechanism with very short duration between dusk and planulation. On the other hand, the pre-dawn peaks could not be controlled by a masking effect which dictates an immediate response after the cues. The possible independence of the two peaks is supported by the wide ranges of the intervals between them, which were from 2 to 16 hours. On the other hand, Aschoff & von Goetz (1989) hypothesized that the degree of the masking effect was dependent on circadian phases; under LD 4∶20, the degree of masking effect was higher than under that under LD 12∶12. This hypothesis is supported in our study, as the number of planulae released of post-dusk peak in LD 5∶19 treatment was higher than that those of the LD 19∶5 and LD 12∶12 treatments (Fig 2).

Rensing et al. [13] suggested that the timer in the hourglass model is supposed to be the consumption of a critical substrate in a rate determining step. Therefore, the Van't Hoff rule, i.e. reaction rates double for every 10°C rise in temperature, should apply. Concerning the hourglass model and Van't Hoff rule, temperature compensation should not occur in hourglass model of timing mechanisms. This is obviously not supported by our study, which found that the temperature did not affect the peak larval release times.

In this study, we noticed that the interval between the cue and the planulation of S. hystrix is 23.2 hrs, very close to the periods of circadian oscillators. This finding prompt us to consider the origin of the two mechanisms. It could be a result of convergent evolution due to strong selective pressures in corals to planulate at an exact hour. On the other hand, the existence of temperature compensation, a property of the oscillator model, suggests that there is a common trait between the two rhythm models. That hourglass clock may be a special oscillator with fast damping of the cyclic activities was hypothesized in prior studies [30], [31], [32]. The results here suggest that the mechanism of planulation timing in S. hystrix could be an intermediate stage between the typical hourglass and the oscillator models.

Two hypotheses have been proposed to explain the adaptive values of diel timing of planulation. (1) Predator avoidance: planulae released at night may have a high survival rate since it is easy to avoid predators that rely on vision for foraging [9], [33], [34], [35]. (2) Reducing duration of the swimming stage of planulae: settlement of planulae may require visual cues such as light orientation. Planulae released pre-dawn may be able to find suitable substrate with minimal waiting. Goodbody-Gringley (2010) observed that Favia fragum planulae released pre-dawn had significantly higher rates of settlement than those released post-dusk. The latter hypothesis, in particular, could explain the propensity for a pre-dawn release in S. hystrix and other reef-building corals. For those corals of Taiwan in particular, it also seems likely that light, as opposed to temperature, is a larger driver of larval release timing because of the thermodynamically unstable nature of the reefs of Southern Taiwan [36], in which the local water temperature may fluctuate 10°C within 24 h due to tidally induced upwelling [36]. Additionally, the monthly average temperature differs by about 5°C in a year. Without a mechanism for temperature compensation, the time of planulation could deviate by several hours from sunrise.

Our conclusion of the mechanism of planulation rhythm of S. hystrix is that it is under regulation of a hourglass model with an involvement of temperature compensation.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks are offered to Drs. Peter Edmunds and Yoko Nozawa for helpful comments that improved the quality of this manuscript. We also thank Dr. Anderson Mayfield and Ms. Wen-Ling Liao for their careful proofing of the manuscript's English.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CHL KYS TYF. Performed the experiments: CHL. Analyzed the data: CHL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: TYF. Wrote the paper: CHL KYS TYF.

References

  1. 1. Aschoff J (1989) Temporal orientation: Circadian clocks in animals and humans. Anim Behav 37, Part 6: 881–896.
  2. 2. Amano S (1986) Larval release in response to a light signal by the intertidal sponge Halichondria panicea. Biol Bull 171: 371–378.
  3. 3. Amano S (1988) Morning release of larvae controlled by the light in an intertidal sponge, Callyspongia ramosa. Biol Bull 175: 181–184.
  4. 4. Watanabe H, Lambert CC (1973) Larva release in response to light by the compound ascidians distaplia occidentalis and metandrocarpa taylori. Biol Bull 144: 556–566.
  5. 5. Olson RR (1983) Ascidan-prochloron symbiosis: The role of larval photoadaptations in midday larval release and settlement. Biol Bull 165: 221–240.
  6. 6. Nozais C, Duchêne JC, Charles F, Mas S (2003) The effect of photoperiod on the timing of larval release in the mediterranean brood-care polychaete Eupolymnia nebulosa (Terebellida). J Mar Biol Assoc UK 83: 1227–1228.
  7. 7. Kojis BL, Quinn NJ (1981) Aspects of sexual reproduction and larval development in the shallow water hermatypic coral, Goniastrea australensis (Edwards and Haime, 1857). Bull Mar Sci 31: 558–573.
  8. 8. Harrison PL, Babcock RC, Bull GD, Oliver JK, Wallace CC, et al. (1984) Mass spawning in tropical reef corals. Science 223: 1186–1189.
  9. 9. Babcock RC, Bull GD, Harrison PL, Heyward AJ, Oliver JK, et al. (1986) Synchronous spawnings of 105 scleractinian coral species on the great barrier reef. Mar Biol 90: 379–394.
  10. 10. Szmant AM (1986) Reproductive ecology of caribbean reef corals. Coral Reefs 5: 43–53.
  11. 11. Fan T-Y, Lin K-H, Kuo F-W, Soong K, Liu L-L, et al. (2006) Diel patterns of larval release by five brooding scleractinian corals. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 321: 133–142.
  12. 12. Isomura N, Nishihira M (2001) Size variation of planulae and its effect on the lifetime of planulae in three pocilloporid corals. Coral Reefs 20: 309–315.
  13. 13. Rensing L, Meyer-Grahle U, Ruoff P (2001) Biological timing and the clock metaphor: Oscillatory and hourglass mechanisms. Chronobiol Int 18: 329–369.
  14. 14. Pittendrigh CS (1960) Circadian rhythms and the circadian organization of living systems. Cold Spring Harbor Symp Quant Biol 25: 159–184.
  15. 15. Kao CC, Chen J, Chen GF, Soong K (2010) Variable swarming time of an intertidal midge (Pontomyia oceana Tokunaga, 1964) controlled by a circadian clock and temperature. Mar Freshwat Behav Physiol 43: 1–9.
  16. 16. Rao P, Bullock T (1954) Q10 as a function of size and habitat temperature in poikilotherms. Am Nat 88: 33–44.
  17. 17. Hug DH, Hunter JK, Odonnell PS (1977) Photoactivation of urocanase in Pseudomonas putida. Temperature-compensated in vitro model of an hourglass timer. Biochemistry 16: 3995–3999.
  18. 18. Harrison PL (2011) Sexual reproduction of scleractinian corals. In: Dubinsky Z, Stambler N, editors. Coral Reefs: An Ecosystem in Transition: Springer Netherlands. 59–85.
  19. 19. Villanueva RD, Yap HT, Montaño MNE (2008) Timing of planulation by pocilloporid corals in the northwestern Philippines. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 370: 111–119.
  20. 20. Goodbody-Gringley G (2010) Diel planulation by the brooding coral Favia fragum (Esper, 1797). J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 389: 70–74.
  21. 21. Johnson KG (1992) Synchronous planulation of Manicina areolata (scleractinia) with lunar periodicity. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 87: 265–273.
  22. 22. Holloran MK, Witteman GJ (1986) Diurnal periodicity in planula release by the reef coral Pocillopora damicornis. In: Jokiel PL, Richmond RH, Rogers RA, editors. Coral Reef Population Biology: University of Hawaii, Hawaii Institute of Marine Biology, Kaneohe. 161–166.
  23. 23. Harrigan JF (1972) The planula larvae of Pocillopora damicornis: lunar periodicity of swarming and substratum selection behavior: University of Hawaii.
  24. 24. Brady AK, Hilton JD, Vize PD (2009) Coral spawn timing is a direct response to solar light cycles and is not an entrained circadian response. Coral Reefs 28: 677–680.
  25. 25. Boulos Z, Macchi MM, Terman M (1996) Effects of twilights on circadian entrainment patterns and reentrainment rates in squirrel monkeys. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 179: 687–694.
  26. 26. Boulos Z, Macchi MM (2005) Season- and latitude-dependent effects of simulated twilights on circadian entrainment. J Biol Rhythms 20: 132–144.
  27. 27. Boulos Z, Macchi MM, Terman M (2002) Twilights widen the range of photic entrainment in hamsters. J Biol Rhythms 17: 353–363.
  28. 28. Aschoff J (1966) Circadian activity pattern with two peaks. Ecology 47: 657–662.
  29. 29. Engelmann W, Mack J (1978) Different oscillators control the circadian rhythm of eclosion and activity in Drosophila. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 127: 229–237.
  30. 30. Saunders DS, Lewis RD (1987) A damped circadian oscillator model of an insect photoperiodic clock: III. Circadian and “hourglass” responses. J Theor Biol 128: 73–85.
  31. 31. Bünning E (1969) Common features of photoperiodism in plants and animals. Photochem Photobiol 9: 219–228.
  32. 32. Saunders DS (2002) Insect Clocks: Elsevier.
  33. 33. Hurlbut CJ (1988) Diel timing of larval release and settlement of a colonial tunicate in pearl-harbor, Oahu, Hawaii. Am Zool 28: 189.
  34. 34. Harrison PL, Wallace CC (1990) Reproduction, dispersal and recruitment of scleractinian corals. In: Dubinsky Z, editor. Coral Reefs. New York: Elsevier. 133–207.
  35. 35. Hobson ES, Chess JR (1978) Trophic relationships among fishes and plankton in the lagoon at Enewetak Atoll, Marshall Islands. Fish Bull 76: 133–153.
  36. 36. Lee H-J, Chao S-Y, Fan K-L (1999) Flood – ebb disparity of tidally induced recirculation eddies in a semi-enclosed basin: Nan Wan Bay. Cont Shelf Res 19: 871–890.