Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Behaviours Associated with Acoustic Communication in Nile Tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus)

  • Nicolas Longrie,

    Affiliation Laboratory of Functional and Evolutionary Morphology, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium

  • Pascal Poncin,

    Affiliation Laboratory of Fish and Amphibian Ethology, Behavioural Biology Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium

  • Mathieu Denoël,

    Affiliation Laboratory of Fish and Amphibian Ethology, Behavioural Biology Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium

  • Vincent Gennotte,

    Affiliation Aquaculture Research and Education Center (CEFRA), University of Liège, Tihange, Belgium

  • Johann Delcourt,

    Affiliation Laboratory of Fish and Amphibian Ethology, Behavioural Biology Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium

  • Eric Parmentier

    E.Parmentier@ulg.ac.be

    Affiliation Laboratory of Functional and Evolutionary Morphology, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium

Abstract

Background

Sound production is widespread among fishes and accompanies many social interactions. The literature reports twenty-nine cichlid species known to produce sounds during aggressive and courtship displays, but the precise range in behavioural contexts is unclear. This study aims to describe the various Oreochromis niloticus behaviours that are associated with sound production in order to delimit the role of sound during different activities, including agonistic behaviours, pit activities, and reproduction and parental care by males and females of the species.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Sounds mostly occur during the day. The sounds recorded during this study accompany previously known behaviours, and no particular behaviour is systematically associated with sound production. Males and females make sounds during territorial defence but not during courtship and mating. Sounds support visual behaviours but are not used alone. During agonistic interactions, a calling Oreochromis niloticus does not bite after producing sounds, and more sounds are produced in defence of territory than for dominating individuals. Females produce sounds to defend eggs but not larvae.

Conclusion/Significance

Sounds are produced to reinforce visual behaviours. Moreover, comparisons with O. mossambicus indicate two sister species can differ in their use of sound, their acoustic characteristics, and the function of sound production. These findings support the role of sounds in differentiating species and promoting speciation. They also make clear that the association of sounds with specific life-cycle roles cannot be generalized to the entire taxa.

Introduction

Many teleosts are able to emit sounds associated with different behaviours such as feeding competition [1], courtship [2], [3], [4] or agonistic behaviour [5], [6], [7]. More precisely, sound production can be used to deter intruders [8], [9], [10], to identify conspecifics [11], [12], [13], [14], or to attract and choose partners [15]. Moreover, sound characteristics can also inform conspecifics about the motivation [16], [17] or the size [18] of the caller.

In cichlids, sexual selection plays a central role in mate choice, meaning their complex courtship behaviours are considered to have a key role in their diversification process [19], [20]. In several species, courtship and agonistic interactions are closely associated with sound production [3], [21]. Moreover, these sounds are species-specific and allow species distinction based on acoustic characteristics such as trill duration, number of pulses per trill, pulse period, pulse duration, and interpulse interval [11], [12], [22], [23], [24]. There are today at least 29 species of Cichlidae described as sound producers, of which 25 are from Africa and 4 from the South American continent (Table 1). For some cichlid species, such as for Archocentrus multispinosus, several different types of sounds have been recorded and associated to specific contexts: burst and thump sounds are produced in agonistic situations and growls may help to synchronise partners during spawning. The less frequent “whoof sounds” have not yet been related to a specific behaviour [25].

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of Cichlid species producing sounds referenced in the literature.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t001

Although most occurrences of the production of sounds are reported among males, as for example in Tramitichromis intermedius [26] or Maylandia spp [11], females are also capable of emissions, as in Archocentrus centrarchus [10] or Hemichromis bimaculatus [27], [28].

Among the Cichlidae, the “Tilapia” group comprises species with oral incubation by females. In the breeding season, females brood the eggs until they hatch and the yolk sac is resorbed. The fry are then released and aggregate near their mother for ca. 21 days, re-entering her mouth in times of danger [29]. The agonistic behaviours [30], [31], [32], [33] and sexual behaviours [30], [34] of Oreochromis niloticus are well described: males defend their territories (arenas or leks) where they attract females. At the end of the mating sequence, the male quivers while circling the nest and is followed by the female, who takes both eggs and sperm into her mouth, where the eggs are fertilized [35].

The Nile Tilapia’s production of sounds has received little attention [36], [37], [38]. Males produce short-duration (250–400 ms), often double-pulse sounds. Most energy is below 200 Hz and includes three main low-frequency peaks [37]. The sound producing mechanism has been studied in adults [38]. It is made during a backward movement of the pelvic and pectoral girdles and a forward movement of the second pterygiophore of the anal fin. Muscle contractions should result in compression of the rib cage and the swim bladder.

Despite these studies, it has so far been unknown whether the female can emit sounds and whether there are links between specific visual behaviours and sound emission. The aim of this study was to test these hypotheses by an in-depth analysis of acoustic behaviour of Oreochromis niloticus. This work primarily relied on the known ethogram of O. niloticus males to determine which parts of this cichlid behaviour are related to sound production. In addition, we investigated different aspects of the behaviours of O. niloticus females and their ability to produce sounds.

Methods

Ethics Statement

Throughout the experiment, mortality was very low and did not exceed the average rate that we have observed in the species in captivity. The experiments were carried out under the approval of the Animal Care Committee of the University of Liège (forms 564 and 738) in accredited experimental rooms (LA 1610429 and LA 1610430).

Fish

Nile tilapia O. niloticus (lake Manzala strain) used in the experiments were raised in the Behavioural Biology Unit (Laboratory of Fish and Amphibian Ethology), University of Liège (Liège, Belgium). They came from the Aquaculture Research and Education Center of the University of Liège (CEFRA-ULg, Tihange, Belgium). Adults (Total Length: 7 to 30 cm, sex ratio 1∶1) were stocked in a 4.5 m3 aquarium (375 cm×100 cm×125 cm) with coarse sand on the bottom where the males would dig their nests, removing the sand with their mouths [30]. Fish were fed ad libitum three times a week with commercial pellets (the excess was removed to avoid any pollution) and aquaria were maintained at 28°C with a photoperiod of 12L:12D (7 am –7 pm). The recordings of behaviours were performed in three 650L aquaria (200 cm×55 cm×60 cm) with the same environmental conditions. Fish stayed in these experimentation aquaria for 1 to 6 days, for the time of the recordings.

Recording and Analysis of Sound Production

The first sound recordings were conducted in the stock aquarium, using a Digital Spectrogram Recorder (DSG, Loggerhead Instruments Inc, Sarasota, FL). A DSG is a low-power acoustic recorder designed to sample at different rates, continuously or on a duty cycle to a Secure Digital High Capacity (SDHC) memory. It makes possible the recording of scheduled periods at regular intervals. The rate of main sampling was 40 kHz, and the hydrophone (HTI) sensitivity was 186 dBV lPa−1. Prior to the recording sessions, all electric devices (ventilation, pumps and heating system) were unplugged.

Two recording series were conducted with the DSG:

  1. Nycthemeral variations: the number of sounds was recorded in the stock aquarium for 15 minutes every hour for four days, meaning 96 recordings were made for one group. The number of individuals in the stock tank was 94.
  2. Variations during “light” phase: another set of automatic recordings (15 minutes every hour during the light periods) was conducted for 8 days (N = 1, n = 8) in a 650L aquarium containing 15 males and 10 females.

Recordings were analysed with Avisoft-SASLab Pro version 4.38 (Berlin, Germany). Recording in small tanks induces potential hazards because of reflections and tank resonance [39]. A relevant equation [39] was thus used to calculate the resonant frequency of the stock and experimental tanks, and a low pass filter of 938 Hz (Stock) and 1.8 kHz (experimental) respectively was then applied to all sound recordings.

The sets of recordings were then automatically analysed with the function « Pulse Train Analyses » of Avisoft (Threshold = 0.1 V, Group Time 250 ms, Hysteresis 30 dB). A manual count was performed for several portions of the sound ranges to verify the validity of the automatic analysis.

Recording of the Video Sequences

In order to observe a possible association between the sounds produced and visual behaviour, sounds were recorded with a hydrophone HTI (sensitivity 186 dB re. 1 V µPa−1) connected to a digital video camera Canon FS100. Prior to the recording sessions, all electric devices (ventilation, pumps and heating system) were unplugged, and the hydrophone was placed vertical to the nest of a territorial male at least 20 min before recording in order to minimise any impact on behaviour.

1. Males vs. Males - Agonistic interactions.

Oreochromis niloticus territorial males were observed for 6 days in two different types of territorial situations. The aim was to learn in what ways the presence of other males induced sound production by focal territorial males.

  1. First observations were made to study the behaviour of a territorial male towards non-territorial males and to seek information about the influence of the population density on the emission of sounds by the territorial males. This experiment was carried out with 7 different territorial males. A male (SL mean ± SD, 28.6±6 cm, n = 7) was first placed in an aquarium two days before starting the recordings. This period was necessary to give it time to delimit its territory. Intruders (SL mean ± SD, 24.4±6 cm, n = 28) were then added one after the other in the tank at the rate of one fish per day. A total of 4 intruders were introduced in each of the sessions. Each day, the sounds and the behaviours were recorded by sessions of 15 minutes that started 5 minutes after the introduction of the intruder. A total of 28 (7 males×4 intruders) recording sessions were carried out.
  2. The second set of observations examined confrontations between territorial males. First five males were simultaneously introduced into the experimental tank. Three of them (SL mean ± SD,11.7±0.6 cm) took possession of a zone and then dug and defended an arena. The two remaining males were then removed from the tank. The three territorial males were acclimated for two days before starting the recording sessions. During recording sessions, the hydrophone (connected to the video camera) was placed over each male’s arena successively. Each recording session occurred between 11 am and 3 pm and lasted 15 minutes. The elapsed time between two adjacent sessions was at least one hour. A total of 4 sessions per male were recorded and analysed.

2. Males vs. Females – Courtship interactions.

First one male (n = 4, SL mean ± SD,18±8 cm) was placed in the experimental tank for two days before recording in order to allow him to acquire the status of territorial male. Three females (SL mean ± SD, 13.7±7 cm, n = 12) were then added to provoke courtship behaviour. Females (n = 12) were chosen according to the swelling of their genital papilla. Experiments were conducted four times. In each experiment, sounds and behaviours were recorded as long as the courtship behaviour and the mating lasted (from 20 to 60 min). At the end of the experiment, 12 complete reproduction cycles were recorded.

3. Females vs. Females - Territoriality and Oral incubation.

Experiments with “female vs. female” interactions were carried out to determine if females are capable of sound production. Five females (SL mean ± SD, 12.1±1 cm) were placed in each of three experimental aquaria. Sounds and associated behaviours were then recorded during 3 sessions lasting 10 minutes each. Female behaviours associated with sound production were listed.

4. Females – Parental care.

This set of observations was devoted to looking for sound production during females’ oral incubation. For this experiment, a female incubating eggs or fry was exposed to intruders such as Amphilophus citrinellus (South American cichlid) and to females of the same species. Five different females were tested, allowing 55 experimental encounters (n = 55) that each lasted 10 minutes. Twenty-three encounters were arranged between O.niloticus females and 32 between female O. niloticus and female Amphilophus citrinellus.

Analysis of the Video Files

The various behaviours analysed are listed in Table 2. The association between sounds and broadcasting fish can be made visually. Sounds are made during a backward movement of the pelvic and pectoral girdles [38], but it was not possible to observe the calling fish in every experimental test.

thumbnail
Table 2. Description of Oreochromis niloticus behaviours according to [2], [30], [34], [35], [57], [73].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t002

Video films were analysed with Noldus Observer Video Pro 4.1 software [40], [41]. This software made possible the analysis and recording of events (instantaneous behaviour such as sound production or bites) or states (behaviours with appreciable duration such as arena digging or frontal display) by typing keys from the keyboard in synchronization with video-files. The different behavioural patterns were consistent with previous descriptions of O. niloticus (Table 2).

Analysis of the Sound Productions of Females

Sounds were digitized at 44.1 kHz (16-bit resolution) and analysed with the Avisoft-SASLAB Pro 4.33 software [1024-point Hanning window fast Fourier transform (FFT)]. Only sounds with a high signal-to-noise ratio were used in the analysis. Temporal features were measured by oscillograms and frequency variables from power spectra (filter bandwidth 300 Hz, FFT size 256 points, time overlap 96.87% overlap and a flat top window). The sound parameters measured were: sound duration (ms); number of pulses in a sound; pulse period (measured as the average peak-to-peak interval between consecutive pulses in the entire sound, ms); pulse length (measured as the time from the beginning of one pulse to its end, ms); and dominant (or main) frequency, representing the most intense frequency (in Hz).

Statistical Analysis

The statistical analysis was made with Statistica 9.1. The Shapiro-Wilk W-test was used to test the normality of the data. A Wilcoxon’s non-parametric paired test was used to determine if the fishes’ visual behaviours were significantly more often accompanied by sounds or lack of sounds. We used this paired test because the data of behaviours with and without sounds are dependent. A Spearman rank correlation coefficient (RS) was used to test the relation between the number of intruders and the number of sound produced. The chi-square test was used to determine if sounds were more frequently associated with some behavioural patterns than others. The non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA by ranks with subsequent Dunn's test for pairwise comparisons was used to compare the rate of bites and production of sound in three contexts (territorial males ♂♂ vs. intruders ♂; ♂♂ vs. ♂♂; ♂♂ vs. ♀). The Mann-Whitney U-test was used to compare the sounds/behaviour co-occurrence between the various contexts; and the Student t-test was used to compare the duration of the sound, the number of pulses, the pulse duration and the main frequency of sounds in the experimental conditions.

Results

Nycthemeral Rhythm of the Production of Sounds

The sound production cycle of male Oreochromis niloticus coincided with the nycthemeral cycle: the sound production took place mainly during the diurnal phase (Fig. 1), but some sounds were also recorded during the night. However, the rate of sound production is not uniform at all stages of the light phase (Fig. 2). Sound production regularly increased throughout the first 7 hours of illumination (until, essentially, the beginning of the artificial afternoon) and then decreased until the illumination ceased (nightfall).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Nycthemeral production of sounds in Oreochromis niloticus.

Number of fishes = 94.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Diurnal production of sounds in Oreochromis niloticus, mean ± SD (n = 8 days, N = 25 fishes including 15♂ and 10♀).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.g002

Association of Behaviours with the Production of Sounds

a) Acoustic behaviours between males.

The confrontation between territorial males and non-territorial intruders resulted in a range of behaviours that were sometimes associated with sounds (Table 3). Other behaviours (see Table 2) also resulted which were not accompanied by sound production.

thumbnail
Table 3. Mean ± SE of the occurrence of a behaviour appearing on a 15 min recording, for Oreochromis niloticus territorial males.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t003

There was no relation between the number of sounds and the number of intruders present in the tank (Spearman correlation, RS = 0.34; p>0.05) (Fig. 3). In confrontations between a territorial male and intruders, the different behaviours were usually performed without sound production (Table 2), indicating that sounds do not constitute the essential part of these various behaviours. Comparisons were realised at two levels. 1) First we compared co-occurrence of sounds and behaviours between the different kinds of behaviour within each kind of meeting to know which behavior is more commonly associated with sounds. During the first set of experiments (Territorial Male ♂♂ vs intruders males ♂), some behavioural patterns (mainly Lateral display and Nest display) were more associated with sounds than others (see Table 4).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Oreochromis niloticus - Number of sounds produced during 28 recording sessions of 15 minutes with 1 to 4 male intruders in the tank.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.g003

thumbnail
Table 4. Proportions of behavioural patterns exhibited in association with sounds in two contexts: ♂♂ vs. ♂ (1 Territorial Male x Intruders; above the diagonal) and ♂♂ vs ♂♂ (3 Territorial Males x Intruders; under the diagonal).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t004

In the second set of experiments (Territorial Male ♂♂ vs Territorial Male ♂♂), the confrontation between a focal individual and other territorial males elicited an additional threatening behaviour: the frontal display during which two opponents approach each other frontally while swelling the branchiostegal membrane and spreading their opercles. Although mouth fighting is more often associated with sounds (14.3%) in the second set of experiment, the co-occurrence remains significantly less important than in other behaviours (Table 4).

Faced with non-territorial intruders, the territorial males mostly produced their threats from the nest. Conversely, confrontations with other territorial males mostly resulted in chases and frontal displays, and lateral displays became rare (Table 3). All these behaviours seem to correspond to a more aggressive response to other territorial males. However territorial males bite non-territorial intruders more often than other territorial males (Table 5). In comparison with encountering between territorial male and non-territorial intruder, the sound production of a territorial male encountering other territorial males was associated with a greater number of different behaviours, and the sound production rate was six times greater (Table 5). 2) In the second set of comparisons, we compared the co-occurence of behaviour and sounds between males placed in two different kinds of meeting (Table 6). In the presence of other territorial males, sounds were emitted simultaneously with 19%–63% of the occurrences of the different behaviours (Table 6, Fig. 4). More precisely, sounds were more associated with Frontal display, Nest hover and Nest display (Table 6). This co-occurrence between sound production and behaviour display was less than 20% when the territorial male was confronted with non-territorial males (Table 6, Fig. 4). During meetings between territorial males, more sounds were significantly realised except during Lateral display (Table 6).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Histogram of the average percentage (mean ± SE) of sound production during the display of different behaviours in Oreochromis niloticus males during different kinds of encounters.

* p<0.05, *** p<0.001: significant differences between light and dark grey bars (Mann-Whithney test). ♂♂ = territorial male; ♂ = intruder male. The statistical test cannot cannot be applied to Frontal display and Mouth fighting because these behaviours were not recorded in ♂♂ vs ♂.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.g004

thumbnail
Table 5. Mean rate (± SE) per minute of event-behaviour during confrontation of territorial males against intruders (♂♂ vs. ♂, N = 7, n = 28), between territorial males (♂♂ vs. ♂♂, N = 3, n = 12) and territorial males against females (♂♂ vs. ♀, N = 6, n = 24).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t005

thumbnail
Table 6. Mean percentage (± SE) of co-occurrence between behaviour and production of sounds.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t006

b) Acoustic behaviour between males and females.

Observation of the confrontations staged between a territorial male and one or several females showed there were no sounds associated with reproductive behaviours (such as tilting, leading, lateral display, tail wagging and the wedding dance, see Table 2) and spawning. However, males emitted sounds towards females in the context of territorial defence.

During the three kinds of meetings (Table 5), call rates appeared to be significantly different (Kruskal-Wallis, H = 19.4; N = 79, p>0.05). However, pair-wise comparisons indicated the global rate ( = 0.5 sound/min) of sound production of territorial males towards females corresponded to the rate of sound production of territorial males ( = 0.9 sound/min) when facing non-territorial male intruders (Dunn’s test, p>0.05). These rates were both significantly lower (Dunn’s test, p<0.05) than the rate of 6 sounds per minute when facing other territorial males (Table 5). The difference in biting rates towards the two groups was also tested (Kruskal-Wallis Test, H = 27.3; N = 79). Territorial males bite females (0.6 bites/min) significantly less (Dunn’s test, p<0.05) than non-territorial intruders (1.7 bites/min). However, the biting rate toward females and other territorial males was similar (Dunn’s test, p>0.05).

c) Acoustic behaviour of females.

Females were recorded in three situations: Territorial (the female digs and defends an arena); Incubation (eggs in the mouth); and Protection (free alevins that can be taken back in the mouth in case of danger). In Oreochromis niloticus, females emitted sounds in two contexts: Territorial and Incubation.

The 315 sounds (recorded from 7 different territorial females) lasted for 320 ms ±101 (Χ ± SD) and consisted of 3.1±1 pulses (Table 7, Fig. 5). Pulse periods had an average length of 105±23 ms, and the sounds had a frequency of 49±14 Hz (N = 7, n = 963). For females in incubation, the sound duration was 264±84 ms with 2.5±0.6 pulses (N = 4, n = 141, Table 7). Pulse periods had an average length of 107±21 ms, and the sounds had a frequency of 48±15 Hz (N = 4, n = 348). The comparison between the data obtained in the two behavioural contexts showed the sounds were significantly different in their duration and their number of pulses. However, pulse length was not significantly different between the two contexts; the difference between sounds was only due to the number of pulses (Table 7).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Oscillogram (A) and spectrogram (B) of a sound having four pulses in Oreochromis niloticus female.

Frequency spectrum of a pulse (C). (*) = Main frequency. Spaces between dotted lines correspond to pulse period.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.g005

thumbnail
Table 7. Means (± SE) of the sonic parameters of Oreochromis niloticus females, recorded in situation of territoriality (Nest) and of oral incubation (Eggs).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t007

The female acoustic behaviour associated with territory defence was very similar to that of the males. Females were also able to produce sounds during behaviours associated with territorial defence, except during mouth fighting (Table 8). Moreover, as with the males, sounds were not made systematically during the different behaviours (Table 8). They mostly occurred during nest digging and frontal display, with sounds accompanying 45% and 30% of the behaviours respectively, whereas fewer than 20% of other behaviours were associated with sounds (Table 9, Fig. 6).

thumbnail
Figure 6. Mean percentage (% ± SE) of sound production during the display of different behaviours in Oreochromis niloticus females.

Nest = territorial female; eggs = female incubating eggs. * p<0.05, *** p<0.001: significant differences between light and dark grey bars (Mann-Whithney test). The statistical test cannot be applied to Mouth fighting because this behaviour was not recorded during “Nest” experiments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.g006

thumbnail
Table 8. Mean (±SE) of the occurrence of a behaviour appearing on a 15 min recording, for Oreochromis niloticus females.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t008

thumbnail
Table 9. Mean percentage (± SE) of co-occurrence between a behaviour and a production of sounds.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t009

Females were also able to make sounds during the oral incubation of their eggs (Table 10). In this case, sound productions co-occurred more often during Frontal Display than during the other behaviours (77%, Table 10). In addition, sounds were associated with 45% of Mouth Fighting by incubating females, but territorial females did not use this behaviour (Tables 9 and 10).

thumbnail
Table 10. Proportions of behavioural patterns exhibited in association with sounds in two contexts: Nest = Territorial female (above the diagonal); Eggs = Female incubating eggs (under the diagonal).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.t010

Comparisons between territorial and incubating females showed other differences in behaviours as well. Although the same behaviours were realised during nest defence and egg defence, more sounds co-occurred with behaviours when the female was incubating (Table 8). The digging behaviour (Digging Nest) reported for incubating females was actually observed shortly after spawning and then disappeared rapidly.

d) Acoustic behaviour during female parental care.

During fry guard, no sound was heard either to confront the intruder (Amphilophus citrinellus or Oreochromis niloticus) or to call fry to shelter inside the oral cavity. Faced with an intruder, females chose to attack rather than to discourage it by threats, quickly eliminating the danger. After the attack, the female retrieved her fry into her mouth.

Discussion

Producing signals represents an economic way to solve disputes over resources. According to Ladich and Myrberg [6], defence and offence begin with signals, before physical interactions occur. In O. niloticus, meetings always include visual displays, but acoustic displays are only produced during escalated contests. Acoustic display appears to be only a part of a complex signalling system during interactions.

Rate of Sound Production

Sound productions in Oreochromis niloticus take place mostly during the day (Fig. 1). All the sounds recorded during this study accompanied previously known behaviours, and no particular behaviour was systematically associated with sound production. This means that sounds are mostly produced to emphasize or reinforce visual behaviours that are firstly mainly based on visual stimuli [42], [43].

In O. niloticus, reproduction usually occurs in the afternoon and evening [31], while the early hours of the day are assigned to nest making/improvement and to defence of territory against other males attempting to settle down. Falter [31] showed that the territorial behaviour of O. niloticus was subject to variation during the day. In a study of 200 males in an aquarium (4.85 m3), the number of defended arenas increased during the day from about 56 between 8 and 11 am to 123 between 2 and 5 pm [31]. These data are in complete accord with the rates of sound production observed in the present study, which also increased gradually, with a peak in emissions at around 1 pm (or 7 hours post-lighting). Early in the day, the establishment and defence of a first arena do not face hostility from congeners. However, those males subsequently establishing arenas meet aggression from the territorial males already installed, increasing the global sound rate of the community. Territorial behaviours are accompanied by sounds, and the rate of sound production is more frequent in an aquarium where several territorial males are present. The increase in sound production during the day could therefore result from the increasing number of individuals who acquire or seek to acquire a territorial status as spawning time approaches.

The difference between vocalizing activity during the day and at night could possibly be simply a side-effect of the lack of visual stimuli during the night. In cichlids visual cues are important in mate choice, in male-male interactions and in non sexually motivated communication such as parent-offspring communication or species recognition [44], [45]. This would reinforce the hypothesis that acoustic displays are mainly used to support visual cues; sounds seem to carry messages only when they are associated with visual displays.

As in Amphiprion species, no sound production is involved in reproductive behaviour [46]. We have tested 4 different males in a total of 12 reproduction cycles. The number of tested males could seem low but in other cichlid species such as O. mossambicus [2], all the territorial males made sound during reproduction, and not only some of them. Moreover, males made sounds during other behaviours meaning they were able to produce sounds. Finding in the future some O. niloticus males are able to produce sounds during reproductive behaviour could in fact reinforce the main results of this study: sounds are produced to reinforce visual behaviours in O. niloticus. However, the number of sounds in the altercations between males might be a criterion for mate choice. Indeed, the simplest message that can be conveyed by the sounds is the location of a breeding area and the motivation of its owner [47]. In the cichlids Haplochromis nyererei, H. omnicaeruleus and Pundamilia pundamilia, females significantly prefer males who are associated with sound production [15].

Sound Production by Territorial Males

In the cichlid Archocentrus centrarchus, acoustic communication was considered an inhibitor of aggressiveness because the sounds of territorial males reduced the number of physical injuries during aggressive encounters related to territory defence and progeny protection [48]. In Maylandia zebra (Cichlidae), aggressive behaviour is based on visual stimuli, and playback experiments have showed that acoustic signals alone never trigger aggression. Furthermore, when fish can only interact visually the association between visual and acoustic channels seems to lower the level of aggression observed [43]. However, as summarised by Ladich and Myrberg [6], sounds can also repel an intruder, increase an opponent’s aggressiveness or be used to assess a competitor’s fighting ability.

Sound production in Oreochromis niloticus territorial males is associated with behaviours of threat (frontal display, nest display, etc.) and chase (Table 2). When the territorial male is opposing dominated intruders, simple assault or threatening movements seems to be enough to keep them away from the arena. However, faced with other dominant males, more sounds are associated with threatening behaviours (Tables 3, 4 and 5). In O. niloticus, there are simultaneously more sounds and fewer bites between territorial males having a high status than between territorial males and females or non-territorial males. Once again, the major role of the sounds in Oreochromis niloticus seems to be to reinforce the messages of the visual stimuli.

In the sister species O. mossambicus [49], [50], sounds are emitted in all phases of courtship, but especially during late stages of courtship including spawning. In the late stage of courtship, the display called “Tail Wagging” (tail-flick) is associated with sounds in 89.7% of the behaviours. Amorim et al. [2] suggested that the acoustic emissions in this species may play a role in signalling the presence and reproductive readiness of males and in synchronizing gamete release, as in the cichlid Tramitichromis intermedius [26]. No sound production is associated with aggressive behaviours in O. mossambicus [2]. In O. niloticus, sounds are principally associated with territory defence and threatening purposes but not with courtship/mating behaviours per se. Nevertheless, because emissions start during nest building, it may be the case that females use sounds to gain information on the reproductive status of the males. Oerochromis niloticus also has a lower number of pulses (2–5 vs. at least 10), a longer pulse duration (150 vs. 10 ms) and a lower peak frequency than O. mossambicus [2], [37], allowing the two acoustic signatures to be easily differentiated. Yet while the pulse duration between the two species is quite different, the strong frequency component peaking at ca. 40 Hz in both is otherwise not common in fish sounds, which suggests they have similar sound producing mechanisms [38].

Although in an artificial environment hybridizations are always possible between these two sister-species, they are rare (or poorly documented) in the wild [47], [51]. According to a study by N’Gokaka [34], mating between O. niloticus and O. mossambicus can only occur shortly after individuals meet because there is confusion of sexual calls. Indeed, the divergences in their reproductive patterns make reproduction even less likely as the heterospecific partners may recognise each other easily. These matings are advanced by a high state of sexual motivation in females and only a brief encounter before spawning, two conditions making the partners more likely to respond to inappropriate stimuli. The presence (O. mossambicus) or the absence (O. niloticus) of mating sounds increases the differences in their behavioural patterns. The difference between these two species might be a factor that has contributed to their specific isolation by reducing errors in reproduction between heterospecific partners.

Production of Sounds Among Females

This is the first study to highlight the ability of O. niloticus females to produce sounds. However, this ability has already been noted in other Cichlidae species (Table 1) where females produce sounds in aggressive behaviours and protection of spawn. The characteristics of the female calls are similar to those observed in males [37]: sounds are composed of few pulses (generally <5), for which the period is ca.100 ms and the main frequency between 30 and 100 Hz (Fig. 5). Because sexual dimorphism of morphological characteristics is extremely low in this species, both sexes should use the same sound-producing mechanism. Moreover, the ability of the incubating females to make sounds reinforces the hypothesis that pharyngeal jaws are not involved in sound production in this species [38] because having eggs in the mouth might preclude sound production by this mechanism.

In O. niloticus territorial females sound productions are associated with similar behaviours to those reported in territorial males (♂♂ vs. ♂♂) but are less frequent, with the exception of Nest digging. The digging of an arena by a female is usually observed before spawning in aid of the territorial male [30], but females may also dig an arena alone [42], [52], [53]. The female that digs an arena (while emitting sounds) usually spawns within 2–3 days and then incubates the eggs. During this incubation, the female may continue to issue sounds, probably to protect her eggs. Similar observations have been made of females of other Cichlidae species, such as Archocentrus centrarchus and Hemichromis bimaculatus, that emit sounds to protect their eggs, but also while preparing the nest for spawning [10], [28]. In Maylandia zebra, Simões et al. [23] showed that the sounds were produced by females when they were sexually receptive (visible ovipositor) or in oral incubation. Similar observations of sound productions in females in the state of oral incubation have been documented in Oreochromis mossambicus [54].

Comparison of the rate of sounds/behaviour co-occurrence between a territorial female and an incubating one indicates a significantly higher number of sound productions during frontal threats and mouth fighting in the incubating female. This difference may reflect the changing role of the mouth between a territorial female and an incubating one. For the territorial female, the mouth is available to take part in defence and combat. The incubating female uses her mouth to protect her eggs. When she is doing so, mouth fighting behaviour is hazardous because she risks losing the spawn. Sound emissions may therefore be used in order to curtail a potentially dangerous situation for the eggs. In this context, the production of sounds may be being used to inhibit the aggressiveness of the opponent. This hypothesis is reinforced by studies of Amphilophus centrarchus, a substrate incubator, which report that the females emit more sounds against threats after spawning (during incubation) [10].

Parental Guard by Females

In the animal kingdom the degree of aggressiveness of females increases during the phase of parenting for animals as diverse as rodents [55] and crustaceans [56]. This increasing aggressiveness in females helps in protecting their offspring from predators [57]. In teleosts, the quickness and intensity of aggressions are influenced by both size and age of the progeny. In the Mozambique tilapia Oreochromis mossambicus, Oliveira and Almada [57] reported an increase in maternal aggressions during the development of the eggs and fry, becoming most intense when the young fish were capable of swimming freely outside the mouth of their mother. Data concerning sound production during this behaviour are unfortunately lacking for this species.

Female O. niloticus incubating eggs and embryos are unable to fight properly as a consequence of their mouth-breeding activity. During this period, female aggressiveness towards other individuals is reinforced by production of sounds. When the larvae are able to swim and feed by themselves, the female releases them while remaining able to take them back in the mouth in times of danger. When an intruder approaches, the first reaction of the female is not to recover its young but to directly attack the dangerous individual without any prior warning, threat or sound emission. This result also matches what is observed in O. mossambicus, where attack will prevail over control during this early phase in the development of the larvae [57]. In A. centrarchus, where the couple continues to emit sounds during the stage of free swimming larvae, the production of sounds by the parents does not seem to affect the behaviour of the young; the young themselves emit no sounds [10].

Conclusion

This thorough approach to studying the acoustic behaviour of a cichlid species (O. niloticus) gave significant insight into their biology. Sounds may support some visual behaviours but are not systematically associated with any given behaviour. They are made by males and females in the context of territorial defence but do not seem to be related to courtship and mating. During agonistic interactions, sounds appear to be used to postpone or to lower aggressiveness: a calling O. niloticus does not simultaneously bite. In females, sounds may be made to defend the eggs but not the larvae. Comparisons with O. mossambicus highlight the fact that sounds in these two sister species can differ in usage, signature and function. This finding suggests that sounds may help in differentiating species and promoting speciation. It also makes clear that the functions of the sounds cannot be generalized to the entire taxa.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Christian Michel and his team from the Aquarium – Museum of Liège for providing facilities and helping with fish care. MD is Research Associate and JD is Postdoctoral Researcher at F.R.S.-FNRS (Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique, Belgium). Michael Fine, Clara Amorim and an anonymous referees greatly helped in reviewing the first versions of this MS. This is the AFFISH (Applied and Fish Research Center) publication n°1.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: EP NL PP. Performed the experiments: NL. Analyzed the data: NL EP. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: PP MD VG JD. Wrote the paper: NL EP. Corrected and improved the manuscript: PP MD VG JD.

References

  1. 1. Amorim MCP, Hawkins AD (2005) Ontogeny of Acoustic and Feeding Behaviour in the Grey Gurnard, Eutrigla gurnardus. Ethology 111: 255–269.
  2. 2. Amorim MCP, Fonseca PJ, Almada VC (2003) Sound production during courtship and spawning of Oreochromis mossambicus: male–female and male–male interactions. Journal of Fish Biology 62: 658–672.
  3. 3. Lobel PS (1998) Possible species specific courtship sounds by two sympatric cichlid fishes in Lake Malawi, Africa. Environmental Biology of Fishes 52: 443–452.
  4. 4. Parmentier E, Kéver L, Casadevall M, Lecchini D (2010) Diversity and complexity in the acoustic behaviour of Dacyllus flavicaudus (Pomacentridae). Marine Biology 157: 2317–2327.
  5. 5. Ladich F (1997) Agonistic behaviour and significance of sounds in vocalizing fish. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 29: 87–108.
  6. 6. Ladich F, Myrberg AAJ (2006) Agonistic behavior and acoustic communication. In: Ladich F, Collin S, Moller P, Kapoor B, editors. Communication in fishes. Enfield, NH: Science Publishers. pp.122–148.
  7. 7. Millot S, Vandewalle P, Parmentier E (2011) Sound production in red-bellied piranhas (Pygocentrus nattereri, Kner): an acoustical, behavioural and morphofunctional study. Journal of Experimental Biology 214: 3613–3618.
  8. 8. Lagardère JP, Fonteneau G, Mariani A, Morinière P (2003) Les émissions sonores du poisson-clown mouffette Amphiprion akallopisos, Bleeker 1853 (Pomacentridae), enregistrées dans l’aquarium de La Rochelle. Ann Soc Sci nat Charente-Maritime 9: 281–288.
  9. 9. Santiago JA, Castro JJ (1997) Acoustic behaviour of Abudefduf luridus. Journal of Fish Biology 51: 952–959.
  10. 10. Schwarz AL (1980) Sound production and associated behavior in a cichlid fish, Cichlasoma centrarchus. II. Breeding pairs. Environmental Biology of Fishes 5: 335–342.
  11. 11. Amorim MCP, Simões JM, Fonseca PJ, Turner GF (2008) Species differences in courtship acoustic signals among five Lake Malawi cichlid species (Pseudotropheus spp.). Journal of Fish Biology 72: 1355–1368.
  12. 12. Simões JM, Duarte IG, Fonseca PJ, Turner GF, Amorim MCP (2006) Acoustic behaviour in Malawian Cichlids (Pseudotropheus, Cichlidae): Potential cues for species recognition and intraspecific communication. Razprave IV razreda SAZU, Advances in Bioacoustics II 47: 229–236.
  13. 13. Lobel PS (1998) Possible species specific courtship sounds by two sympatric cichlid fishes in Lake Malawi, Africa. Environmental Biology of Fishes 52: 443–452.
  14. 14. Parmentier E, Lagardère JP, Vandewalle P, Fine ML (2005) Geographical variation in sound production in the anemonefish Amphiprion akallopisos. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences 272: 1697–1703.
  15. 15. Verzijden MN, van Heusden J, Bouton N, Witte F, ten Cate C, et al. (2010) Sounds of male Lake Victoria cichlids vary within and between species and affect female mate preferences. Behavioral Ecology 21: 548–555.
  16. 16. Lugli M, Torricelli P (1999) Prespawning sound production in mediterranean sand-gobies. Journal of Fish Biology 54: 691–694.
  17. 17. Lobel PS, Kerr LM (1999) Courtship sounds of the Pacific Damselfish, Abudefduf sordidus (Pomacentridae). Biol Bull 197: 242–244.
  18. 18. Colleye O, Frederich B, Vandewalle P, Casadevall M, Parmentier E (2009) Agonistic sounds in the skunk clownfish Amphiprion akallopisos: size-related variation in acoustic features. Journal of Fish Biology 75: 908–916.
  19. 19. Streelman JT, Danley PD (2003) The stages of vertebrate evolutionary radiation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 18: 126–131.
  20. 20. van Staaden MJ, Smith AR (2011) Cutting the Gordian knot: Complex signaling in African cichlids is more than multimodal. Current Zoology 57: 237–252.
  21. 21. Lobel PS (2001) Acoustic Behaviour of Cichlid Fishes. Journal of Aquariculture & Aquatic Sciences 9: 167–186.
  22. 22. Amorim MCP, Knight ME, Stratoudakis Y, Turner GF (2004) Differences in sounds made by courting males of three closely related Lake Malawi cichlid species. Journal of Fish Biology 65: 1358–1371.
  23. 23. Simões JM, Duarte IG, Fonseca PJ, Turner GF, Amorim MCP (2008) Courtship and agonistic sounds by the cichlid fish Pseudotropheus zebra. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 124: 1332–1338.
  24. 24. Parmentier E, Vandewalle P, Lagardère JP (2003) Sound-producing mechanisms and recordings in Carapini species (Teleostei, Pisces). Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 189: 283–292.
  25. 25. Brown DH, Marshall JA (1978) Reproductive behaviour of the rainbow cichlid, Herotilapia multispinosa (Pisces, Cichlidae). Behaviour 67: 297–321.
  26. 26. Ripley JL, Lobel PS (2004) Correlation of acoustic and visual signals in the cichlid fish, Tramitichromis intermedius. Environmental Biology of Fishes 71: 389–394.
  27. 27. Myrberg AAJ, Kramer E, Heinecke P (1965) Sound production by cichlid fishes. Science 149: 555–558.
  28. 28. Rowland WJ (1978) Sound production and associated behavior in the jewel fish, Hemichromis bimaculatus. Behaviour 64: 125–136.
  29. 29. Russock H (1999) Filial social bond formation in fry of the maternal mouthbrooding tilapia (Pisces : Cichlidae) : a comparative study. Behaviour 136: 567–594.
  30. 30. Falter U (1983) Les comportements agonistiques de Sarotherodon niloticus (Pisces : Cichlidae) et la signification évolutive de l’incubation buccale. Bulletin de l’Academie Royale de Belgique (Classe des Sciences) LXIX: 566–594.
  31. 31. Falter U (1986) Fluctuations journalières dans le comportement territorial chez Oreochromis niloticus (Teleostei: Cichlidae). Annales de la Société Royale de Zoologie de Belgique 116: 175–190.
  32. 32. Falter U (1987) Description des patrons de coloration chez Oreochromis niloticus (L.) (Teleostei: Cichlidae). Annales de la Société Royale de Zoologie Belge 117: 201–219.
  33. 33. Falter U, Dolisy D (1989) The effect of female sexual pheromones on the behaviour of Oreochromis niloticus, Oreochromis mossambicus and hybrid males (Pisces, Cichlidae). Annales du Musée Royal de l’Afrique Centrale des Sciences Zoologique 257: 35–38.
  34. 34. N’Gokaka C (1983) Ethologie et hybridation des Tilapias. Contribution a l’etude des mecanismes ethologiques de l’isolement reproducteur des poissons cichlides africains: analyse du comportment de parade sexuelle chez Sarotherodon niloticus (L.) et Sarotherodon mossambicus (Peters). Cahiers d’Ethologie Appliquée 3: 1–195.
  35. 35. Oliveira RF, Almada VC (1998) Mating tactics and male–male courtship in the lek-breeding cichlid Oreochromis mossambicus. Journal of Fish Biology 52: 1115–1129.
  36. 36. Bauer J (1963) Lautäusserungen bei Tilapia nilotica. Aquarium Terrarium Zeitschrift 16: 171–172.
  37. 37. Longrie N, Fine ML, Parmentier E (2008) Innate sound production in the cichlid Oreochromis niloticus. Journal of Zoology 275: 413–417.
  38. 38. Longrie N, Van Wassenbergh S, Vandewalle P, Mauguit Q, Parmentier E (2009) Potential mechanism of sound production in Oreochromis niloticus (Cichlidae). Journal of Experimental Biology 212: 3395–3402.
  39. 39. Akamatsu T, Okumura T, Novarini N, Yan HY (2002) Empirical refinements applicable to the recording of fish sounds in small tanks. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 112: 3073–3082.
  40. 40. Noldus LPJJ (1991) The Observer: A software system for collection and analysis of observational data. Behavior Research Methods 23: 415–429.
  41. 41. Denoël M (2002) Paedomorphosis in the Alpine newt (Triturus alpestris): decoupling behavioural and morphological change. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 52: 394–399.
  42. 42. Castro ALS, Gonçalves-de-Freitas E, Volpato GL, Oliveira C (2009) Visual communication stimulates reproduction in Nile tilapia, Oreochromis niloticus (L.). Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research 42: 368–374.
  43. 43. Bertucci F, Beauchaud M, Attia J, Mathevon N (2010) Sounds Modulate Males’ Aggressiveness in a Cichlid Fish. Ethology 116: 1179–1188.
  44. 44. Seehausen O, Mayhew PJ, Van Alphen JJM (1999) Evolution of colour patterns in East African cichlid fish. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 12: 514–534.
  45. 45. Kocher T (2004) Adaptive evolution and explosive speciation: the cichlid fish model. Nature Reviews Genetics 5: 288–298.
  46. 46. Colleye O, Parmentier E (2012) Overview on the Diversity of Sounds Produced by Clownfishes (Pomacentridae): Importance of Acoustic Signals in Their Peculiar Way of Life. PLoS ONE 7: e49179.
  47. 47. Fryer G, Iles TD (1972) The cichlid fishes of the great lakes of Africa: their biology and evolution. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.
  48. 48. Schwarz A (1974) The inhibition of agressive behaviour by sound in a cichlid fish, Cichlasoma centrarchus. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie 35: 508–517.
  49. 49. Klett V, Meyer A (2002) What, if Anything, is a Tilapia?–Mitochondrial ND2 Phylogeny of Tilapiines and the Evolution of Parental Care Systems in the African Cichlid Fishes. Molecular Biology and Evolution 19: 865–883.
  50. 50. He A, Luo Y, Yang H, Liu L, Li S, et al. (2011) Complete mitochondrial DNA sequences of the Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) and Blue tilapia (Oreochromis aureus): genome characterization and phylogeny applications. Molecular Biology Reports 38: 2015–2021.
  51. 51. D’Amato M, Esterhuyse M, van der Waal B, Brink D, Volckaert F (2007) Hybridization and phylogeography of the Mozambique tilapia Oreochromis mossambicus in southern Africa evidenced by mitochondrial and microsatellite DNA genotyping. Conservation Genetics 8: 475–488.
  52. 52. Duponchelle F, Legendre M (2001) Rapid phenotypic changes of reproductive traits in response to experimental modifications of spatial structure in Nile tilapia, Oreochromis niloticus. Aquatic Living Resources 14: 145–152.
  53. 53. Silverman HI (1978) Effects of different levels of sensory contact upon reproductive activity of adult male and female Sarotherodon (Tilapia) mossambicus (Peters); Pisces: Cichlidae. Animal Behaviour 26: 1081–1090.
  54. 54. Marshall JA (1971) Sound production by Tilapia mossambica (Pisces, Cichlidae). American Zoologist 11: 632.
  55. 55. Parmigiani S, Brain PF, Mainardi D, Brunoni V (1988) Different patterns of biting attack employed by lactating female mice (Mus domesticus) in encounters with male and female conspecific intruders. Journal of Comparative Psychology 102: 287–293.
  56. 56. Figler MH, Twum M, Finkelstein JE, Peeke HVS (1995) Maternal Aggression in Red Swamp Crayfish (Procambarus clarkii, Girard): The Relation between Reproductive Status and Outcome of Aggressive Encounters with Male and Female Conspecifics. Behaviour 132: 107–125.
  57. 57. Oliveira RF, Almada VC (1998) Maternal aggression during the mouthbrooding cycle in the cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. Aggressive Behavior 24: 187–196.
  58. 58. Schwarz A (1974) Sound production and associated behaviour in a cichlid fish, Cichlasoma centrarchus. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie 35: 147–156.
  59. 59. Nelissen MHJ (1977) Sound production by Haplochromis burtoni (Günther) and Tropheus moorii Boulanger (Pisces, Cichlidae). Annales de la Societe Royale Zoologique de Belgique 106: 155–166.
  60. 60. Nelissen MHJ (1978) De taal van de Cichliden. Vakbl Biol 21: 373–377.
  61. 61. Smith AR (2007) Is accoustic communication a candidate signal for sexual selection in Malawian Cichlid? Master’s thesis. Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green State University.
  62. 62. Simões JM, Fonseca PJ, Turner GF, Amorim MCP (2008) African cichlid Pseudotropheus spp. males moan to females during foreplay. Journal of Fish Biology 72: 2689–2694.
  63. 63. Bertucci F, Scaion D, Beauchaud M, Attia J, Mathevon N (2012) Ontogenesis of agonistic vocalizations in the cichlid fish Metriaclima zebra. Comptes Rendus Biologies 529–534: 529–534.
  64. 64. Konstantinova NG, Nikol’skii ID, Termen LS (1979) Acoustic and electric activity of Tilapia mossambica (Cichlidae: Pisces). Moscow University Biological Science Bulletin 34: 35–39.
  65. 65. Lanzing WJR (1974) Sound production in the cichlid Tilapia mossambica Peters. Journal of Fish Biology 6: 341–347.
  66. 66. Rodman DT (1966) Sound production by the African cichlid Tilapia mossambica. Ichthyol Aquar J 38: 279–280.
  67. 67. Amorim MCP, Almada VC (2005) The outcome of male–male encounters affects subsequent sound production during courtship in the cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. Animal Behaviour 69: 595–601.
  68. 68. Marshall JA (1972) Influence of male sound production of oviposition in female Tilapia mossambica (Pisces, Cichlidae). American Zoologist 12: 663–664.
  69. 69. Nelissen MHJ (1976) Contribution to the Ethology of Simochromis babaulti Pellegrin (Pisces, Cichlidae). Annales de la Societe Royale Zoologique de Belgique 106: 167–175.
  70. 70. Nelissen MHJ (1975) Sound production by Simochromis diagrama (Günther) (Pisces, Cichlidae). Acta Zoologica et Pathologica Antverpiensia 61: 19–24.
  71. 71. Nelissen MHJ (1978) Sound production by some Tanganyikan cichlid fishes and a hypothesis for the evolution of their communication mechanisms. Behaviour 64: 137–147.
  72. 72. Albrecht H (1966) Tilapia mariae (Cichlidae) – Kampf zweier männchen, text for Encyclopedia cinematographica - film E603.
  73. 73. Baerends GP, Baerends-van Roon JM (1950) An introduction to the study of the ethology of Cichlid Fishes. Behaviour 1: 1–242.