Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Stress Triangle: Do Introduced Predators Exert Indirect Costs on Native Predators and Prey?

Abstract

Non-consumptive effects of predators on each other and on prey populations often exceed the effects of direct predation. These effects can arise from fear responses elevating glucocorticoid (GC) hormone levels (predator stress hypothesis) or from increased vigilance that reduces foraging efficiency and body condition (predator sensitive foraging hypothesis); both responses can lead to immunosuppression and increased parasite loads. Non-consumptive effects of invasive predators have been little studied, even though their direct impacts on local species are usually greater than those of their native counterparts. To address this issue, we explored the non-consumptive effects of the invasive red fox Vulpes vulpes on two native species in eastern Australia: a reptilian predator, the lace monitor Varanus varius and a marsupial, the ringtail possum Pseudocheirus peregrinus. In particular, we tested predictions derived from the above two hypotheses by comparing the basal glucocorticoid levels, foraging behaviour, body condition and haemoparasite loads of both native species in areas with and without fox suppression. Lace monitors showed no GC response or differences in haemoparasite loads but were more likely to trade safety for higher food rewards, and had higher body condition, in areas of fox suppression than in areas where foxes remained abundant. In contrast, ringtails showed no physiological or behavioural differences between fox-suppressed and control areas. Predator sensitive foraging is a non-consumptive cost for lace monitors in the presence of the fox and most likely represents a response to competition. The ringtail’s lack of response to the fox potentially represents complete naiveté or strong and rapid selection to the invasive predator. We suggest evolutionary responses are often overlooked in interactions between native and introduced species, but must be incorporated if we are to understand the suite of forces that shape community assembly and function in the wake of biological invasions.

Introduction

Increasing evidence suggests that the indirect or non-consumptive effects of predators on prey populations can often exceed the effects of direct predation [1]. In the short term such effects may include reduced access by prey to preferred habitats and food resources as well as constraints on their growth and reproduction, while longer term effects include depressed fitness and reduced abundance, site occupancy and distribution at the population level [2][4]. Predators may also have non-consumptive effects on each other via competition for food, especially if they are members of the same foraging guild [5].

Most research on non-consumptive interactions has focused on situations where predators have co-evolved with prey and other predators. This is not surprising; the selective pressure exerted by predators is a key determinant of the structure and function of many natural communities [6], [7]. However, the effects of introduced predators often exceed those of native predators on prey populations, dramatically reducing prey reproductive output, survival and abundance [8], [9]. These impacts appear to be wrought largely by direct predation; evidence for non-consumptive effects is limited [10], perhaps because naïve local species are often extirpated before anti-predator responses evolve [11], [12]. Despite this, recent studies predict that where local prey have large ranges or populations and survive the initial impact, a novel predator can act as a strong selective agent on prey to develop anti-predator responses [13], [14]. We test this prediction here by evaluating the non-consumptive effects of the invasive red fox (Vulpes vulpes) on two common native species in eastern Australia.

Two general hypotheses have been proposed to account for how predators cause non-consumptive effects [15], [16]. Firstly, the ‘predator stress hypothesis’ predicts that physiological consequences for prey of interactions with predators occur through an adrenocortical fear response [17]. This response activates the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and results in elevated production of glucocorticoids (GC). These hormones increase an individual’s ability to cope with a predator attack, protecting the body throughout the stress response and promoting anti-predator responses that benefit survival such as avoidance and vigilance behaviours [17][20]. However, anti-predator responses can be energetically costly. Persistent exposure to environmental stressors or exposure to novel stressors can result in chronic elevation of GC levels, immunosuppression, increased parasite loads and reproductive inhibition [18], [21], [22]. Predator-induced stress occurs in a range of mammal species including snowshoe hares (Lepus americanus) and Arctic ground squirrels (Spermophilus parryii plesius), with a high correlation between predation risk and plasma corticosterone levels [17], [23], [24].

Secondly, the ‘predator sensitive foraging hypothesis’ predicts that predator presence will increase prey vigilance or restrict foraging efficiency and hence result in energetic or nutritional costs [25], [26]. For example, elk (Cervus elaphus) in Yellowstone National Park increase their vigilance and shift from high-value foraging habitat to reduce their risk of predation by wolves (Canis lupus), but experience poorer nutrition as a consequence [27][29]. Increasing vigilance in areas dominated by raptors is beneficial also to the short term survival of partridge (Perdix perdix); however, the associated nutritional trade-off leads to decreased reproductive output and other long term fitness consequences [30]. Animals may show risky behaviour to obtain food if hungry [31], but increase the probability of encountering predators by doing so. The non-lethal nutritional costs of predation risk include, but are not limited to, reduced birth rate [4], [32], decline in body weight, and immunosuppression [33].

As with prey species, predators also show behavioural shifts when confronted with more dominant counterparts [34], and often suffer negative effects on body condition. For example, spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta) in the Masai Mara Natural Reserve have a lower reproductive rate and food intake than in Amboseli owing to the effects of interspecific resource competition with high density lion (Panthera leo) populations [35]. Likewise, the red fox (Vulpes vulpes) is more vigilant and spends less time feeding in the presence of the larger European badger (Meles meles), and shows marked increases in population density when released from competition with the badger [36], [37].

The two hypotheses are not mutually exclusive; in the presence of a predator, physiological and behavioural changes can combine to reduce the fitness of prey [38] and perhaps of competing predators [39]. Indeed, in song birds, there is some evidence that these non-consumptive effects are synergistic: Clinchy et al. [40] showed that while reduced access to nutritional resources and increased predation pressure both result in chronic stress, the level is not additive but multiplicative.

In this study we investigate the responses of native Australian prey and predator species to the invasive red fox. We derived three predictions based on the predator stress and predator sensitive foraging hypotheses (Fig. 1), and used a landscape-scale manipulation of fox abundance to test them. Specifically, we predicted that the native species would exhibit:

  1. higher plasma glucocorticoid concentrations,
  2. increased vigilance and reduced risk during foraging, and
  3. lower body condition and higher parasite loads
in areas where fox abundance was high compared to where it was low. We predicted that these differences would be greater in the native predator than in the prey species because, in areas with low fox abundance the native predator would be minimally exposed to the non-consumptive effects of predation but the prey would continue to be at risk from the native predator. These expectations are represented in the conceptual ‘stress triangle’ of Fig. 1. Because of mechanistic links between the two hypotheses (Fig. 1) we have not attempted to derive contrasting predictions that allow us to distinguish between the two hypotheses. However, we note that evidence for predictions 1) and 2) would support the predator stress and predator sensitive foraging hypotheses, respectively, while evidence for prediction 3) could be taken as support for both.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Stress triangle: Non-consumptive costs of introduced predators on native predators and prey, and the potential pathways of these costs.

The ‘predator stress hypothesis’ predicts that fear will drive a chronic glucocorticoid (GC) response that leads to immunosuppression and consequent increases in parasite load. The ‘predator sensitive foraging hypothesis’ predicts that animals will reduce their foraging efficiency and hence lose body condition. This may lead directly to immunosuppression and an increase in parasite load, or indirectly by stimulating a GC response. These pathways can be set in train by an introduced predator via competition or the risk of predation on native predators and prey, respectively, forming the base of the triangle. Arrows show direction of pathway, with symbols representing an increase (+) or decrease (−) in an individual’s response.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060916.g001

Methods

Study Species

The common ringtail possum (Pseudocheirus peregrinus), hereafter referred to as ringtail, and the lace monitor (Varanus varius) were chosen as the native study species and the red fox as the invasive predator. The fox has had pronounced effects on native species since establishment in Australia in the 1870s. It has been linked to local losses and regional extinctions of several species of vertebrates on the continental mainland, in particular small- to medium-sized mammals [41]. It affects native fauna through both predatory and competitive interactions [10].

The lace monitor is a diurnal reptile that hunts on the ground and in trees. Weighing up to 14 kg, it is the second largest native carnivore in eastern Australia [42], [43]. Its activity is seasonal, with home range varying from 185 ha in summer to zero in winter when animals are inactive [44].

The ringtail is a folivorous marsupial that feeds by night on eucalyptus leaves. Adults weigh 700–900 g and occupy home ranges of 0.02–0.05 ha [45], [46]. Animals spend some time on the ground; they utilize dreys, enclosed nest-like constructions, in addition to tree hollows [47]. The choice of dreys over hollows for nests may be a response to predation risk, as dreys more successfully facilitate escape from arboreal predators [48], [49]. Ringtails form the major prey of the fox and the lace monitor in the study region [50], [51], providing potential for competition between the two predators over this shared resource [52].

Experimental Design

The study region, covering 42,000 ha, centred on the Cape Conran Coastal Park (CCCP) and adjacent Murrungowar state forest in East Gippsland, Victoria (37°48′ S, 148°52′ E). This region was selected because its mosaic of banksia woodland, heath and lowland eucalypt forest is representative of coastal wooded habitats over much of south-eastern Australia, and also because it provides access to a regional and ongoing fox management program [53]. Initiated in 1998, this program aims to suppress fox populations and reduce their impacts on native fauna using intensive baiting with toxic ‘1080’ (sodium monofluoroacetate) baits. We used two baited ‘fox suppression’ areas and two unbaited control areas (Fig. 2), sampling predators and prey throughout to encompass the spatial variation in habitats [54], [55]. Baits are buried 15 cm below ground at ∼1 km intervals along forest management tracks in all areas; those in the fox suppression areas contain 1080 and those in the controls do not.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Map of the Cape Conran Coastal Park and Murrungowar state forest study region, situated in Far East Gippsland, Victoria (37°48′S 148°52′E).

Dots indicate the locations of the 76 transect lines used for trap and sand pads to sample the lace monitor (Varanus varius). Colour of dots indicates treatment area; black represents fox (Vulpes vulpes) suppression areas (baited with 1080 poison) with low densities of V. vulpes, white represents control areas (baited with non-toxic baits) with high densities of V. vulpes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060916.g002

Independent estimates of fox activity, obtained using indexes based on baits taken and foot tracks on sand pads [56], confirm the efficacy of baiting. Whilst we acknowledge that indirect methods, such as track counts, have strengths and weaknesses with respect to their ability to explicitly detail actual population changes in fox abundance [57], they remain the most common methods for evaluating the effectiveness of poison baiting on fox population numbers. Thus we do not know how much poison baiting has reduced fox numbers, other than that fox densities in poison baited areas are considerably lower than in control areas.

Thus, fox activity declined markedly (up to a five-fold decrease in 1080 bait take) in the fox suppression areas prior to and during our study, while remaining relatively stable in the controls [53]. Lace monitors and ringtails were sampled across the study region between November 2007 and February 2010.

Field Procedures

We captured animals to obtain blood samples and morphometric data for predictions 1 and 3, and used direct or indirect observations of foraging behaviour to test prediction 2. We captured ringtails via trapping (folding wire traps 805×265×325 mm, baited with a peanut butter, oats and rose oil mixture) and opportunistic hand capture using a noose and pole. Basal glucocorticoid blood samples were taken only from hand-captured individuals within 3 min of capture, with ∼1 ml of blood drawn from the caudal vein (27 g needle, 2 ml syringe). We collected the following measurements from all captured individuals: sex, age class, mass (g), head and pes length (mm). We assessed foraging and vigilance behaviour by measuring the heights at which animals were observed foraging and by assaying levels of responsiveness to capture. As the fox is terrestrial we assumed that ringtails foraging high above ground would experience less fox-related risk of predation than individuals lower down. Ringtails were opportunistically located by walking slowly along 500 m transects of forest tracks on dark, still nights and using a spotlight (variable beam 100 W, FaunaTech, Victoria) to detect their eye shine or movements. Using a range finder while standing under the branch or position on the tree where the ringtail was first observed, we recorded the vertical distance, or height (m), of each ringtail above the ground. Individuals were not included if movement occurred prior to the capture attempt to ensure they were not influenced by the observers. An attempt was made to capture individuals using a noose and pole, and their escape responses categorized into three vigilance levels: low (successful capture, no escape attempt), medium (delayed escape behaviour, with individuals remaining in place after initial approach), and high (immediate escape behaviour upon approach). Once an individual had been approached, the observers moved a minimum distance of 500 metres before resuming spotlighting to ensure that any individuals with overlapping home ranges were not influenced by any capture attempts.

To sample lace monitors we established 76 sites (38 in fox suppression areas, 38 in controls; Fig. 2), each containing a trap and a sand pad separated by a minimum distance of 150 m. Animals were captured using aluminium box traps (2000×300×300 mm, baited with meat and tuna emulsion oil) and by opportunistic hand capture with a noose and pole. Individuals were physically restrained using tape. We took basal glucocorticoid blood samples within 3 min of hand capture, drawing ∼3 ml of blood from the coccygeal vein (22 g needle, 5 ml syringe). We measured all individuals to obtain mass (kg), head length, head width and snout-ventral length (SVL) (cm). To assess risk-taking during foraging, we laced traps and sand pads with equal amounts of tuna emulsion oil as an attractant, but provided a greater rate of food reward (meat) in traps (350 g/day) than at sand pads (50 g/day). We assumed that animals would perceive lower foraging risks on open sand pads than at the novel structures represented by traps, and assessed the foraging choices of lace monitors as a trade-off between risk vs reward. Each site was monitored for six days during optimum weather conditions, with a mean daytime temperature above 26°C. The numbers of trap captures and sand pad detections (determined by the presence of diagnostic claw and tail marks) were tallied for each treatment to determine relative differences in foraging risk behaviour.

To avoid re-sampling, all ringtails and lace monitors were identified with a PIT tag (TROVAN® ID-100BC, Microchips, Australia, Pty Ltd.) implanted subcutaneously either between the shoulder blades (ringtails) or in the dorsal right thigh (lace monitor). Upon completion of these procedures, animals were immediately released.

Haematology

All blood samples were stored immediately in individually heparinized containers at 4°C and prepared for analysis within 4 h of collection. Blood smears were prepared and stained using Diff-Quick (Rapid Diff, Australian Biostain Pty Ltd). Whole blood samples were then centrifuged (6000 rpm) and extracted plasma stored at −20°C until analysis. Blood smears were examined for the presence of haemoparasites at 1000× magnification under oil immersion using a compound light microscope. Five hundred erythrocytes per slide were counted and parasite load was calculated as the proportion of infected erythrocytes.

Radioimmunoassay

We obtained glucocorticoid concentrations of total plasma cortisol in ringtails and total plasma corticosterone in lace monitors using radioimmunoassay (RIA) techniques similar to those of Jessop et al. [58]. Thus, plasma concentrations of cortisol were assayed using an extracted radioimmunoassay developed for foetal sheep plasma [59] with hydrocortisone (H4001, Sigma) as standard. The mean recovery of [1, 2, 6, 7 3H]-cortisol (NET396, Perkin Elmer) from ringtail plasma using dichloromethane extraction was 81.6%, with an intra-assay coefficient of variation (CV) of 16.2% and a inter-assay CV of 0.14%. Assay sensitivity was 0.33 ng/ml. The antibody had a cross reactivity of 100% with cortisol, 22% with prednisolone, 6.1% with cortexolone, 2% with cortisone, 1.3% with corticosterone and <1% with the steroids DOC and 17-hydroxy progesterone. Plasma samples of V. varius were extracted for corticosterone concentrations using a Corticosterone 3H Kit (MP Biomedicals, LLC). Final steroid concentrations were calculated from standard curves and corrected for individual sample recovery, individual plasma volume and the addition of tritiated steroid. Average (± se) sample recovery was 75.7%±0.028 with an intra-assay CV of 7.6% and an inter-assay CV of 13.04%. The antibody had 100% cross reactivity with corticosterone, 11% with 11-Dehydrocorticosterone, 7% with 11-Deoxycorticosterone, and <1% with the following steroids: progesterone, cortisol, aldosterone, testosterone, pregnenolone and 5α-DHT.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were designed to compare the responses of the native species in areas where fox numbers were suppressed and not suppressed. All data are expressed as mean ± se, with the exception of behavioural data which are presented as percentages of the total number of individuals. Cortisol concentrations of ringtails were log10 transformed prior to analysis to satisfy assumptions of normality for statistical testing. Generalized linear modelling (GLM) was used to compare adrenocortical responsiveness and body condition indices for both study species. In the ringtail we derived condition indices by regressing mass on head length and in the lace monitor by regressing mass on SVL. Regression slopes were calculated using CurveExpert 1.4 (Microsoft Corporation). We also used GLM to compare foraging risk behaviour in lace monitors and sighting height and response to capture in the ringtail. Haemoparasite loads were analysed in lace monitors using a GLM Poisson log-linear model. Only one ringtail was observed to have an erythrocyte infection, so data were not analysed further. Differences were considered significant at P<0.05. GLM analyses were performed using SPSS (version 18) and R 2.10.1 (http://www.r-project.org).

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out under strict accordance with animal welfare guidelines issued by the University of Melbourne. The protocols used were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the University of Melbourne (Permit Number: 0911328). Research was carried out on public land under Department of Sustainability Wildlife and National Parks Act (1975) research permit 10005037 and did not involve sampling of any protected species.

Results

Common Ringtail Possum

In total, 186 ringtails were observed and scored for height above ground when first seen; of these, 48 were captured and blood-sampled for cortisol, 150 were scored for vigilance behaviour and 56 were measured for body condition. Females and males and animals of different age were pooled for analyses after preliminary inspection using generalized linear modelling revealed no obvious differences between them in any of the response variables.

Generalized linear modelling did not detect any differences between treatments in cortisol concentrations of the ringtail (Wald χ21 = 14.16, P = 0.97). After controlling for differences in the time taken to sample blood there was no elevation of glucocorticoids in response to fox predation risk. The estimated marginal means for ringtail cortisol levels were 16.77 ng/ml (±2.71) in fox suppression areas compared with 16.92 ng/ml (±2.84) in the controls.

No change was recorded in possum foraging behaviour, with sighting height similar between treatments (GLM: Wald χ21 = 1.12, P = 0.29). Height was 5.75 m (±0.63) in the fox suppression areas compared to 4.86 m (±0.56) in the controls. Similarly, fox predation risk did not increase vigilance behaviour in ringtail, with no change in response to capture between treatments (GLM: Wald χ21 = 0.70, P = 0.40) (Fig. 3).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Percentage of common ringtail possums (Pseudocheirus peregrinus) that adopted low, medium or high vigilance behaviours in response to capture in areas of fox suppression (with low densities of foxes) and control (high densities of foxes).

Capture responses were categorized into 3 vigilance levels; low (successful capture, no escape attempt), medium (delayed escape behaviour), and high (immediate escape behaviour).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060916.g003

Body condition did not differ in response to fox predation (GLM: Wald χ21 = 1.91, P = 0.17). Residual values of body condition were −0.007 (±0.007) in the fox suppression areas compared to 0.006 (±0.007) in the controls. Immunosuppression was not observed through an increase in haemoparasite load; the single ringtail with an erythrocyte infection represented 3% of all animals sampled.

Lace Monitor

In total, 45 lace monitors were observed and scored for choice of foraging strategy, and a further 21 individuals were hand captured and blood sampled for corticosterone. Of these 66 animals, 41 were assessed for haemoparasite load and 40 were measured for body condition.

There was no statistical difference in basal corticosterone levels between lace monitors in the control areas (14.97 ng/ml ±3.06) compared with those in the fox suppression areas (11.96 ng/ml ±2.55) (GLM: Wald χ21 = 0.57, P = 0.45).

Foraging behaviour in lace monitors differed between the treatments (GLM: Wald χ21 = 12.48, P = 0.002), representing a trade-off between risk (foraging behaviour) and reward (food). In control areas 66.7% of individuals foraged on sand pads, while 33.3% were captured. In contrast, those in fox suppression areas exhibited a shift in foraging behaviour, with 27.5% using sand pads and 72.5% being captured in traps (Fig. 4).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Foraging choice (%) of lace monitors (Varanus varius) in areas of fox suppression (with low densities of foxes) and control (high densities of foxes).

Choice of foraging behaviour was between high risk-high reward sites (represented by traps) vs. low risk-low reward sites (represented by sand pads).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060916.g004

Body condition of lace monitors declined markedly in response to fox presence (GLM: Wald χ21 = 5.75, P = 0.016). Residual values of body condition were −0.002 (±0.014) in fox suppression areas compared to −0.05 (±0.015) in the controls. The haemoparasite Haemogregarina varanicola was detected in 86% of lace monitor erythrocyte samples. However, fox-treatment did not affect haemoparasite load (number of parasites present in 500 red blood cells ± se); loads were 3.96±0.41 in the fox suppression areas and 3.53±0.46 in the controls (GLM: Wald χ21 = 0.44, P = 0.49).

Discussion

The results provide mixed support for our initial predictions and suggest that the native study species respond in quite different ways to the sub-lethal effects of fox presence. While neither study species showed differences in plasma glucocorticoid (GC) concentrations in areas with and without fox suppression (prediction 1), lace monitors showed changes in body condition and foraging behaviour that supported predictions 2) and 3). Ringtails, by contrast, showed no measurable response to differences in fox abundance. In the discussion below, we first review the evidence for the predator stress hypothesis and then consider the divergent behavioural and morphometric responses of the two native species to the invasive fox.

Predator-induced stress responses have been documented in a wide range of vertebrates. Acute responses typically result in sharp increases in GC levels that tail off slowly after the stressor has been removed [17], [20], [60], whereas chronic responses result in GC levels that are more persistently elevated [24]. There are several possible reasons why we failed to find any responses here. In the first instance, we potentially may have collected blood samples too slowly to detect differences in basal GC levels, instead recording levels that were rising due to the acute stress of capture. However, this is unlikely. GC concentrations typically rise within 3–5 minutes of capture in mammals [20], [61], [62], which is well within the time (three minutes) that we took to catch and bleed both lace monitors and ringtails. In addition, the GC concentrations we recorded were within the basal range reported for other vertebrate taxa [20], [60], [63]. Secondly, it is possible that our sample sizes were too low and variances so high that trends in our data were masked. Again, this seems unlikely. For the lace monitor a post hoc power analysis [64] suggested that a nine-fold increase in sample size would be needed to detect a between-treatment difference at α = 0.05, but for the ringtail the increase would need to exceed two orders of magnitude. Any differences in GC levels between treatments for lace monitors, and especially for ringtails, thus appear to be too slight to be meaningful. Thirdly, and most plausibly, overall stress levels were more similar between the fox suppression and control areas than we had anticipated. This interpretation is supported by the lack of haemoparasites in the ringtail and their uniform distribution between areas in the lace monitor. We explore this idea further below.

As predicted, the native predator showed the strongest response to fox presence, with improved body condition in the fox suppression areas where interspecific competition would be lowered. In other work we have shown that monitors also have lower site occupancy in areas of high fox density (unpublished data – J. Anson). Taken together, these findings suggest that foxes restrict access of the monitor to parts of the forest environment and to prey resources. As there is little evidence of intra-guild predation, this restriction most likely arises from competition for food. Of the two main types of interspecific competition interference is more common between predators, with overt agonistic behaviour often resulting in displacement of the inferior competitor [65], especially if the dominant is an invasive one [39]. In the present case, however, the diurnal activity of the lace monitor and nocturnal activity of the fox would reduce their frequency of contact and suggest that competition is at least partly exploitative in form. Temporal partitioning of activity would limit aggressive interactions, potentially attenuate stress responses, and help to explain the lack of adrenocortical elevation in the lace monitor.

Lace monitors also shifted their foraging behaviour in the way that we had predicted, avoiding risky but high reward trap sites where foxes were abundant but selecting them where foxes were suppressed. Risk-averse foraging presumably reduces access by lace monitors to food resources generally in areas where foxes remain abundant and helps to account for their poorer body condition in these areas. Given the temporal separation in the activity patterns of the two predators, however, what risks might lace monitors face from foxes? In the first instance, temporal segregation will only reduce the frequency of direct interactions but not preclude them; if there is a possibility of intense aggression or intra-guild killing [66], it would benefit lace monitors to be strongly risk-averse. Secondly, avoidance can be mediated via other channels, such as odours, even if the frequency of contact between contestants is low. Many species that fall prey to the fox or compete with it distinguish and avoid its faecal or urinary odours, thus reducing the likelihood of encountering the predator itself [14], [67], [68]; it is possible that lace monitors show similar avoidance. If this is correct, the tendency for foxes to defecate and urinate on or near traps in the study region, but much less so on sand pads [69], would probably further strengthen the perception of risk by monitors and reinforce their risk-averse behaviour in the fox control areas. Interspecific competition has been shown to promote similar changes in the foraging strategies of species that partition resources by time [70], [71]. Although we do not know how lace monitors might affect foxes, our observations suggest that monitors face reduced fitness in the presence of the fox and support other work in showing that invasive species are strong and often dominant competitors over native species [65], [72], [73].

We predicted initially that the ringtail would show weaker responses to fox manipulation than the lace monitor because it would continue to be depredated in all areas by the native predator. The absence of any obvious response was unexpected, but may be explained in several ways. Firstly, other stressors, in particular other predators, may have replaced foxes in the fox suppression areas, thus neutralizing the fox removal effect. In other studies fox suppression has led to the release of mesopredators such as the feral cat (Felis catus) [74] and, in the study region, has led to an increase in site occupancy of the lace monitor (J. Anson, unpublished). However, this seems an unlikely explanation for our results. On the one hand independent monitoring in the study region has shown no evident response by feral cats to the suppression of foxes [75]. On the other, if lace monitors were acting as fox ‘surrogates’ in the fox suppression areas, we would expect ringtails to show different rather than equal responses to their presence such as shifting to higher and less accessible sites in trees. There is a suite of other co-evolved predators in the study region including large forest owls (Tyto tenebricosa, Ninox strenua) and the tiger quoll (Dasyurus maculatus); however, the quoll is too scarce to have additional effects on ringtails and the owls are unlikely to be affected in any way by fox suppression.

A second possibility is that ringtails do not perceive foxes to be a threat and thus mount no anti-predator response. Complete naiveté is unusual as it is associated most commonly with the initial invasion phase of a new predator [12], and has been linked to local declines and extinctions of several species of Australian vertebrates [76][79]. Often, however, it appears that native species do respond to the risk posed by a novel predator, but the response is more appropriate to countering the threat of a coevolved predator rather than the new one [80][83]. Prey that employ such ineffective or energetically costly responses can be expected to experience marked consumptive impacts [12], [84]. In the present study the lack of any evident physiological or behavioural response by the ringtail means that we cannot reject the possibility that this species is naïve to the risk of fox predation. However, the lack of any consumptive effect of foxes on ringtail site occupancy and density in the study region (J. Anson, unpublished) does suggest that complete naiveté is unlikely.

Thirdly, and perhaps most intriguingly, it is possible that ringtails already have evolved effective anti-predator responses and now perceive the threat of fox predation as low and predictable. This could explain their general lack of response to fox presence, including their lack of hormonal response and negligible incidence of blood parasites. In an analogous situation, Belding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi) have lower glucocorticoid levels in areas of high risk from coevolved predators, indicating that a marked hormonal response does not necessarily occur if risk is predictable or manageable [85]. There is evidence that predator recognition and avoidance can develop under strong selection on a rapid evolutionary time scale, with fox avoidance behaviours recorded in native Australian rodent species and naïve island species including Galapagos marine iguanas [14], [79], [86]. If the ringtail has been subjected to sufficiently strong selection pressure over the 130 or more years since fox introduction, the lack of obvious vigilance and anti-predator behaviours may indicate that coevolution has occurred and that ringtails now experience minimal predation risk. It is possible, for example, that the mid-level (4–6 m) foraging height of this semi-arboreal species so minimizes the risk of fox predation that the energetic costs of mounting further responses would exceed any small benefits that might accrue. As ringtails still fall prey to foxes but show no impact at the population level, it is conceivable further that only the ‘doomed surplus’ is taken [87].

Two general sets of observations are consistent with this third explanation. Firstly, early accounts of ringtail behaviour prior to the arrival of the fox suggest that it was often active on the ground [88] and was “… seldom met with in the gum-trees, [and] generally frequents the so called tea-tree scrub …” [89∶95]. As tea-tree often grows to just 3–4 m, animals using this habitat presumably would have been at some risk of predation from ground-active predators. Secondly, more recent and quantitative work shows that naïve hand-reared ringtails reintroduced to forest habitat were depredated more heavily by ground-active foxes and cats, and had a shorter survival time, than resident individuals [48]. Although these observations are not conclusive, they provide some support for the argument that the common ringtail possum has evolved effective anti-predator responses towards the fox.

If the ringtail now persists with little cost in the presence of the red fox, it may have been subject to stronger selection than has the sympatric lace monitor which still experiences non-consumptive costs from the novel predator. If correct, this could reflect greater overlap in resource use between the intra-guild competitors than between predator and prey. As ringtails now occur with foxes over most of their range the behavioural and perhaps other changes [90] that have allowed reduction in the costs of predation remain to be uncovered. However, ringtails are likely still to carry the ‘signal’ of their interaction with foxes and we predict, for example, that animals exposed to olfactory, auditory or other fox cues should be able to distinguish and avoid them compared with the cues of different predator archetypes [2], [91], [92].

While introduced predators often have direct effects on prey populations that are more strongly negative than those of native predators, our results show that they can drive non-consumptive costs as well. We suggest further that, if local species survive an invader’s initial impact and experience strong selection, these costs may be reduced rapidly to negligible levels. The evolutionary element is often overlooked in interactions between native and introduced species, but appears crucial to incorporate if we are to understand the suite of forces that shape community assembly and function in the wake of biological invasions.

Acknowledgments

Work was conducted under Department of Sustainability Wildlife and National Parks Act (1975) research permit 10005037, and University of Melbourne ethics permit 0911328. The authors would like to acknowledge both volunteers and Zoos Victoria staff for their assistance; including Graeme Gillespie, Franciscus Scheelings, Kwai Chang, Melissa Mack and Tim Lockwood. The authors would like to acknowledge both Parks Victoria and Department of Sustainability and Environment personnel located at Cape Conran Coastal Park (VIC, Australia).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JRA CRD RB TSJ. Performed the experiments: JRA TSJ. Analyzed the data: JRA TSJ. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: JRA TSJ RB. Wrote the paper: JRA CRD TSJ.

References

  1. 1. Preisser EL, Bolnick DI, Benard MF (2005) Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator-prey interactions. Ecology 86: 501–509.
  2. 2. Lima SL (1998) Stress and decision making under the risk of predation: Recent developments from behavioral, reproductive, and ecological perspectives. Advances in the Study of Behavior 27: 215–290.
  3. 3. Schmitz OJ (2005) Behavior of predators and prey and links with population-level processes. In: Barbosa P, Castellanos I, editors. Ecology of Predator-Prey Interactions. New York: Oxford University Press. 256–278.
  4. 4. Creel S, Christianson D, Liley S, Winnie JA (2007) Predation risk affects reproductive physiology and demography of elk. Science 315: 960–960.
  5. 5. Glen AS, Dickman CR (2005) Complex interactions among mammalian carnivores in Australia, and their implications for wildlife management. Biological Reviews 80: 387–401.
  6. 6. Caro T (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  7. 7. Sergio F, Caro T, Brown D, Clucas B, Hunter J, et al.. (2008) Top predators as conservation tools: ecological rationale, assumptions, and efficacy. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics. 1–19.
  8. 8. Salo P, Banks PB, Dickman CR, Korpimaki E (2010) Predator manipulation experiments: impacts on populations of terrestrial vertebrate prey. Ecological Monographs 80: 531–546.
  9. 9. Salo P, Korpimaki E, Banks PB, Nordstrom M, Dickman CR (2007) Alien predators are more dangerous than native predators to prey populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 274: 1237–1243.
  10. 10. Saunders GR, Gentle MN, Dickman CR (2010) The impacts and management of foxes Vulpes vulpes in Australia. Mammal Review 40: 181–211.
  11. 11. Cox JG, Lima SL (2006) Naivete and an aquatic-terrestrial dichotomy in the effects of introduced predators. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 21: 674–680.
  12. 12. Banks PB, Dickman CR (2007) Alien predation and the effects of multiple levels of prey naivete. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 22: 229–230.
  13. 13. Strauss SY, Lau JA, Carroll SP (2006) Evolutionary responses of natives to introduced species: what do introductions tell us about natural communities? Ecology Letters 9: 354–371.
  14. 14. Kovacs EM, Crowther MS, Webb JK, Dickman CR (2012) Population and behavioural responses of native prey to alien predation. Oecologia.
  15. 15. McEwen BS, Wingfield JC (2003) The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior 43: 2–15.
  16. 16. Wikelski M, Cooke SJ (2006) Conservation physiology. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 21: 38–46.
  17. 17. Boonstra R, Hik D, Singleton GR, Tinnikov A (1998) The impact of predator-induced stress on the snowshoe hare cycle. Ecological Monographs 68: 371–394.
  18. 18. Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU (2000) How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews 21: 55–89.
  19. 19. Romero LM (2004) Physiological stress in ecology: lessons from biomedical research. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19: 249–255.
  20. 20. Boonstra R (2005) Equipped for life: The adaptive role of the stress axis in male mammals. Journal of Mammalogy 86: 236–247.
  21. 21. Wingfield JC, Maney DL, Breuner CW, Jacobs JD, Lynn S, et al. (1998) Ecological bases of hormone-behavior interactions: The "emergency life history stage". American Zoologist 38: 191–206.
  22. 22. Lee AK, McDonald IR (1985) Stress and population regulation in small mammals. Oxford Reviews of Reproductive Biology 7: 261–304.
  23. 23. Krebs CJ, Boonstra R, Boutin S, Sinclair ARE (2001) What drives the 10-year cycle of snowshoe hares? Bioscience 51: 25–35.
  24. 24. Hik DS, McColl CJ, Boonstra R (2001) Why are Arctic ground squirrels more stressed in the boreal forest than in alpine meadows? Ecoscience 8: 275–288.
  25. 25. Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation - a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 68: 619–640.
  26. 26. Houston AI, McNamara JM, Hutchinson JMC (1993) General results concerning the trade-off between gaining energy and avoiding predation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 341: 375–397.
  27. 27. Creel S, Winnie J, Christianson D (2009) Glucocorticoid stress hormones and the effect of predation risk on elk reproduction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106.
  28. 28. Creel S, Winnie J, Maxwell B, Hamlin K, Creel M (2005) Elk alter habitat selection as an antipredator response to wolves. Ecology 86: 3387–3397.
  29. 29. Sinclair ARE, Pech RP, Dickman CR, Hik D, Mahon P, et al. (1998) Predicting effects of predation on conservation of endangered prey. Conservation Biology 12: 564–575.
  30. 30. Watson M, Aebischer NJ, Cresswell W (2007) Vigilance and fitness in grey partridges Perdix perdix: the effects of group size and foraging-vigilance trade-offs on predation mortality. Journal of Animal Ecology 76: 211–221.
  31. 31. Koivula K, Rytkonen S, Orell M (1995) Hunger-dependency of hiding behaviour after a predator attack in dominant and subordinate willow tits. Ardea 83: 397–404.
  32. 32. Christianson D, Creel S (2010) A nutritionally mediated risk effect of wolves on elk. Ecology 91: 1184–1191.
  33. 33. Hik DS (1995) Does risk of predation influence population dynamics - Evidence for the cyclic decline of snowshoe hares. Wildlife Research 22: 115–129.
  34. 34. Rehage JS, Barnett BK, Sih A (2005) Foraging behaviour and invasiveness: do invasive gambusia exhibit higher feeding rates and broader diets than their noninvasive relatives? Ecology of Freshwater Fish 14: 352–360.
  35. 35. Watts HE, Holekamp KE (2008) Interspecific competition influences reproduction in spotted hyenas. Journal of Zoology 276: 402–410.
  36. 36. Macdonald DW, Buesching CD, Stopka P, Henderson J, Ellwood SA, et al. (2004) Encounters between two sympatric carnivores: red foxes (Vulpes vulpes) and European badgers (Meles meles). Journal of Zoology 263: 385–392.
  37. 37. Trewby LD, Wilson GJ, Delahay RJ, Walker N, Young R, et al. (2008) Experimental evidence of competitive release in sympatric carnivores. Biology Letters 4: 170–172.
  38. 38. Sheriff MJ, Krebs CJ, Boonstra R (2009) The sensitive hare: sublethal effects of predator stress on reproduction in snowshoe hares. Journal of Animal Ecology 78: 1249–1258.
  39. 39. Dickman CR (2011) Animal competition. In: Simberloff D, Rejmanek M, editors. Encyclopedia of Biological Invasions. Berkley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. 117–122.
  40. 40. Clinchy M, Zanette L, Boonstra R, Wingfield JC, Smith JNM (2004) Balancing food and predator pressure induces chronic stress in songbirds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 271: 2473–2479.
  41. 41. Burbidge AA, McKenzie NL (1989) Patterns in the modern decline of Western Australia vertebrate fauna - causes and conservation implications. Biological Conservation 50: 143–198.
  42. 42. Guarino F (2001) Diet of a large carnivorous lizard, Varanus varius. Wildlife Research 28: 627–630.
  43. 43. Weavers BW (1988) Vital statistics of the lace monitor lizard (Varanus varius) in south-eastern Australia. Victorian Naturalist 105: 142–145.
  44. 44. Guarino F (2002) Spatial ecology of a large carnivorous lizard, Varanus varius (Squamata : Varanidae). Journal of Zoology 258: 449–457.
  45. 45. Smith B, Augee M, Rose S (2003) Radio-tracking studies of common ringtail possums, Pseudocheirus peregrinus, in Manly Dam Reserve, Sydney. Proceedings of the Linnean Society of New South Wales 124: 183–194.
  46. 46. McKay GM, Ong P (2008) Common ringtail possum. In: Van Dyck S, Strahan R, editors. Mammals of Australia. 3rd edition ed. Sydney, Australia: Reed New Holland.
  47. 47. Lindenmayer DB, MacGregor C, Welsh A, Donnelly CF, Brown D (2008) The use of hollows and dreys by the common ringtail possum (Pseudocheirus peregrinus) in different vegetation types. Australian Journal of Zoology 56: 1–11.
  48. 48. Augee ML, Smith B, Rose S (1996) Survival of wild and hand-reared ringtail possums (Pseudocheirus peregrinus) in bushland near Sydney. Wildlife Research 23: 99–108.
  49. 49. Russell BG, Smith B, Augee ML (2003) Changes to a population of common ringtail possums (Pseudocheirus peregrinus) after bushfire. Wildlife Research 30: 389–396.
  50. 50. Triggs B, Brunner H, Cullen JM (1984) The food of fox, dog and cat in Croajingalong National Park, southeastern Victoria. Australian Wildlife Research 11: 491–499.
  51. 51. Jessop TS, Urlus J, Lockwood T, Gillespie G (2010) Preying possum: assessment of the diet of lace monitors (Varanus varius) from coastal forests in southeastern Victoria. Biawak 4: 59.
  52. 52. Sutherland DR, Glen AS, de Tores PJ (2011) Could controlling mammalian carnivores lead to mesopredator release of carnivorous reptiles? Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 278: 641–648.
  53. 53. Murray AJ, Poore RN, Dexter N (2006) Project Deliverance - the response of ‘critical weight range’ mammals to effective fox control in mesic forest habitats in far East Gippsland, Victoria. Melbourne: Department of Sustainability and Environment.
  54. 54. Krebs CJ, Boutin S, Boonstra R, Sinclair ARE, Smith JNM, et al. (1995) Impact of food and predation on the snowshoe hare cycle. Science 269: 1112–1115.
  55. 55. Tilman D (1994) Competition and biodiversity in spatially structured habitats. Ecology 75: 2–16.
  56. 56. Engeman RM (2005) Indexing principles and a widely applicable paradigm for indexing animal populations. Wildlife Research 32: 203–210.
  57. 57. Wilson GJ, Delahay RJ (2001) A review of methods to estimate the abundance of terrestrial carnivores using field signs and observation. Wildlife Research 28: 151–164.
  58. 58. Jessop TS, Hamann M (2005) Interplay between age class, sex and stress response in green turtles (Chelonia mydas). Australian Journal of Zoology 53: 131–136.
  59. 59. Bocking AD, McMillen IC, Harding R, Thorburn GD (1986) Effect of reduced uterine blood flow on fetal and maternal cortisol. Journal of Developmental Physiology 8: 237–245.
  60. 60. Wingfield JC, Romero LM (2001) Adrenocortical responses to stress and their modulation in free-living vertebrates. In: McEwan BS, editor. Coping with the Environment: Neural and Endocrine Mechanisms. New York: Oxford University Press. 211–236.
  61. 61. Hellgren EC, Lochmiller RL, Amoss MS, Grant WE (1985) Endocrine and metabolic responses of the collared peccary (Tayassu tajacu) to immobilization with ketamine-hydrochloride. Journal of Wildlife Diseases 21: 417–425.
  62. 62. Reeder DM, Kosteczko NS, Kunz TH, Widmaier EP (2004) Changes in baseline and stress-induced glucocorticold levels during the active period in free-ranging male and female little brown myotis, Myotis lucifugus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae). General and Comparative Endocrinology 136: 260–269.
  63. 63. Romero LM, Romero RC (2002) Corticosterone responses in wild birds: the importance of rapid initial sampling. Condor 104: 129–135.
  64. 64. Cohen J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  65. 65. Amarasekare P (2002) Interference competition and species coexistence. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 269: 2541–2550.
  66. 66. Moseby K, Neilly H, Read J, Crisp H (2012) Interactions between a top order predator and exotic mesopredators in the Australian rangelands. International Journal of Ecology in press.
  67. 67. Dickman CR (1992) Predation and habitat shift in the house mouse, Mus domesticus. Ecology 73: 313–322.
  68. 68. Molsher RL (1999) The ecology of feral cats (Felis catus) in open forest in New South Wales: interactions with food resources and foxes: University of Sydney.
  69. 69. Diment AN (2010) Monitoring the ecological impacts of invasive predator control [PhD thesis]: University of Sydney.
  70. 70. Jones M, Mandelik Y, Dayan T (2001) Coexistence of temporally partitioned spiny mice: roles of habitat structure and foraging behavior. Ecology 82: 2164–2176.
  71. 71. Harrington LA, Harrington AL, Yamaguchi N, Thom MD, Ferreras P, et al. (2009) The impact of native competitors on an alien invasive: temporal niche shifts to avoid interspecific aggression? Ecology 90: 1207–1216.
  72. 72. Byers JE (2000) Competition between two estuarine snails: Implications for invasions of exotic species. Ecology 81: 1225–1239.
  73. 73. Holway DA (1999) Competitive mechanisms underlying the displacement of native ants by the invasive Argentine ant. Ecology 80: 238–251.
  74. 74. Risbey DA, Calver MC, Short J, Bradley JS, Wright IW (2000) The impact of cats and foxes on the small vertebrate fauna of Heirisson Prong, Western Australia. II. A field experiment. Wildlife Research 27: 223–235.
  75. 75. Buckmaster T (2011) Ecology and management of feral cats in Far East Gippsland, Victoria. [PhD thesis]: University of Sydney.
  76. 76. McLean IG, Lundie-Jenkins G, Jarman PJ (1996) Teaching an endangered mammal to recognise predators. Biological Conservation 75: 51–62.
  77. 77. Banks PB, Dickman CR, Newsome AE (1998) Ecological costs of feral predator control: Foxes and rabbits. Journal of Wildlife Management 62: 766–772.
  78. 78. Griffin AS, Evans CS, Blumstein DT (2001) Learning specificity in acquired predator recognition. Animal Behaviour 62: 577–589.
  79. 79. Russell BG, Banks PB (2007) Do Australian small mammals respond to native and introduced predator odours? Austral Ecology 32: 277–286.
  80. 80. Banks PB (1998) Responses of Australian bush rats, Rattus fuscipes, to the odor of introduced Vulpes vulpes. Journal of Mammalogy 79: 1260–1264.
  81. 81. McEvoy J, Sinn DL, Wapstra E (2008) Know thy enemy: Behavioural response of a native mammal (Rattus lutreolus velutinus) to predators of different coexistence histories. Austral Ecology 33: 922–931.
  82. 82. Jones ME, Smith GC, Jones SM (2004) Is anti-predator behaviour in Tasmanian eastern quolls (Dasyurus viverrinus) effective against introduced predators? Animal Conservation 7: 155–160.
  83. 83. Short J, Kinnear JE, Robley A (2002) Surplus killing by introduced predators in Australia - evidence for ineffective anti-predator adaptations in native prey species? Biological Conservation 103: 283–301.
  84. 84. Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B, Orrock JL, Peacor SD, et al. (2010) Predator-prey naivete, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator invasions. Oikos 119: 610–621.
  85. 85. Mateo JM (2007) Ecological and hormonal correlates of antipredator behavior in adult Belding's ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 37–49.
  86. 86. Berger S, Wikelski M, Romero LM, Kalko EK, Roedl T (2007) Behavioral and physiological adjustments to new predators in an endemic island species, the Galapagos marine iguana. Hormones and Behavior 52: 653–663.
  87. 87. Banks PB (1999) Predation by introduced foxes on native bush rats in Australia: do foxes take the doomed surplus? Journal of Applied Ecology 36: 1063–1071.
  88. 88. Gould J (1863) The mammals of Australia London: J. Gould.
  89. 89. Lydekker R (1896) A Hand-book to the Marsupialia and Monotremata. London: Edward Lloyd.
  90. 90. Hawlena D, Hughes KM, Schmitz OJ (2011) Trophic trait plasticity in response to changes in resource availability and predation risk. Functional Ecology 25: 1223–1231.
  91. 91. Kats LB, Dill LM (1998) The scent of death: Chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience 5: 361–394.
  92. 92. Anson JR, Dickman CR (2012) Behavioral responses of native prey to disparate predators: naiveté and predator recognition. Oecologia.