Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sex and Caste-Specific Variation in Compound Eye Morphology of Five Honeybee Species

  • Martin Streinzer ,

    martin.streinzer@uni-wuerzburg.de

    Affiliations Department of Behavioral Physiology and Sociobiology, Biozentrum, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany, Department of Evolutionary Biology, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Axel Brockmann,

    Affiliation National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, Bangalore, India

  • Narayanappa Nagaraja,

    Affiliation University Grants Commission, Academic Staff College, Bangalore University, Bangalore, India

  • Johannes Spaethe

    Affiliation Department of Behavioral Physiology and Sociobiology, Biozentrum, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany

Abstract

Ranging from dwarfs to giants, the species of honeybees show remarkable differences in body size that have placed evolutionary constrains on the size of sensory organs and the brain. Colonies comprise three adult phenotypes, drones and two female castes, the reproductive queen and sterile workers. The phenotypes differ with respect to tasks and thus selection pressures which additionally constrain the shape of sensory systems. In a first step to explore the variability and interaction between species size-limitations and sex and caste-specific selection pressures in sensory and neural structures in honeybees, we compared eye size, ommatidia number and distribution of facet lens diameters in drones, queens and workers of five species (Apis andreniformis, A. florea, A. dorsata, A. mellifera, A. cerana). In these species, male and female eyes show a consistent sex-specific organization with respect to eye size and regional specialization of facet diameters. Drones possess distinctly enlarged eyes with large dorsal facets. Aside from these general patterns, we found signs of unique adaptations in eyes of A. florea and A. dorsata drones. In both species, drone eyes are disproportionately enlarged. In A. dorsata the increased eye size results from enlarged facets, a likely adaptation to crepuscular mating flights. In contrast, the relative enlargement of A. florea drone eyes results from an increase in ommatidia number, suggesting strong selection for high spatial resolution. Comparison of eye morphology and published mating flight times indicates a correlation between overall light sensitivity and species-specific mating flight times. The correlation suggests an important role of ambient light intensities in the regulation of species-specific mating flight times and the evolution of the visual system. Our study further deepens insights into visual adaptations within the genus Apis and opens up future perspectives for research to better understand the timing mechanisms and sensory physiology of mating related signals.

Introduction

Honeybee colonies comprise three adult phenotypes, males (drones), reproductive females (queens) and sterile females (workers). Drones, queens and workers differ in reproductive organs, as well as in the morphology of mouthparts, flight musculature, number of glands, sensory systems and structural organization of brains [1][5]. Morphological and physiological differences correlate with behavioral differences and result from different natural and sexual selection pressures.

The virgin queen leaves the colony for mating flights; after mating, she remains in the hive and lays eggs for most of her life [1], [6]. The workers perform all tasks necessary to maintain the colony, e.g. brood care, foraging, and colony defense. Corresponding to this division of labor, queens show reductions in many morphological traits, e.g. mouthparts, pollen collecting structures, olfactory system and brain size [1], [4]. In contrast, drones engage neither in social nor foraging tasks; they serve predominantly for reproduction, i.e. searching for and mating with queens. Drones show enlarged and elaborated olfactory [2], [7], [8] and visual systems [9], as well as flight musculature that is adapted for fast pursuit flights [10].

Body size is considered the most characteristic morphological difference among honeybee species [11], [12]. Presumably, body size affects all sex and caste-specific morphological traits and, most importantly, sensory organs, the brain and motor system [10], [13], [14]. Body size scaling in bumblebees, for instance, results in more sensitive olfactory systems [14] and more acute and sensitive visual systems [13], [15] in larger bodied individuals, enabling foraging activities at lower light levels [13]. Large body size is further considered as an important pre-requisite for the behavioral transition to crepuscular activity [16].

Honeybees possess apposition compound eyes that consist of several thousand optically isolated ommatidia. Despite the general limitations given by the eye design, this eye type is well suited for orientation and object detection in bright daylight. Studies in sweat and carpenter bees show that, with some modification, the apposition compound eyes also enable reasonable visual orientation during night [17][20]. Eye design affects ecological success of a species, e.g. via improved flower detection capabilities in females and improved mate detection in males. It also sets an important boundary to the specific timeframe in which the animal is able to operate [20]. Numerous studies in Hymenoptera (e.g. ants [21], [22], bees [13], [17][19], [23], [24] and wasps [25]) document the relation between the structure of the visual system and the specific light environment in which the animal is active. In a recent study, Somanathan et al. [24] document visual adaptations of honeybee workers in three Asian species and the Western honeybee and discuss the implications of the eye design in the context of photic niche utilization for foraging. Temporal niche partitioning in honeybees is further important in the context of mating. Due to their common behavioral pattern of long range sex-pheromone attraction by a similar odor bouquet and short range visual chasing, geographically co-occurring species are forced to temporally separate mating times [11].

As an initial step to explore the variability of sensory and neural structures and the interaction between size limitations and sex and caste-specific selection pressures (e.g. selection on fecundity in queens, efficient foraging in workers and mate detection in drones) in Apis, we studied the periphery of the visual system. We investigated all sexes and castes of five species (Apis andreniformis, A. florea, A. dorsata, A. mellifera and A. cerana), to contribute to our knowledge of caste and species-specific visual systems in the genus [17], [26]. We compared number and arrangement of ommatidia and facet lens diameters in the compound eyes and ocellar size. In particular, we asked whether and how eye size, ommatidia number and facet size correlate with body size and differ among sexes and castes. We hypothesize that clear deviations from body size correlations indicate specific adaptations, either with respect to spatial resolution (ommatidia number) or light sensitivity (facet diameter), both of which are traded-off against each other in relation to the specific lifestyle of the animal.

Materials and Methods

Honeybee Specimens

We investigated queens, workers and drones of the two dwarf honeybee species (A. andreniformis Smith, 1858 and A. florea Fabricius, 1787), the giant honeybee (A. dorsata Fabricius, 1793), the Western honeybee (A. mellifera Linnaeus, 1758) and the Eastern honeybee (A. cerana Fabricius, 1793). Specimens were collected near Bangalore, India (Doddaballapur, 13°17'32"N, 77°32'35"E) between 2003 and 2012 (A. florea, A. dorsata, A. cerana), in Chiang Rai Province, Northern Thailand (Mae Sai, 20°25'60"N, 99°52'60"E; Mae Fang Luang, 19°52'25"N, 99°43'23"E) in 2011 (A. andreniformis), in Vienna, Austria (48°13'47"N, 16°21'32"E) and Würzburg, Germany (49°46'48"N, 9°58'25"E) between 2009 and 2011 (A. mellifera carnica) and obtained from the collection maintained at the bee research unit, Bremen, Germany (A. florea, obtained from Feyriz, Fars Province, Iran in 1991). Specimens were either pin-mounted or preserved in ethanol and pin-mounted prior to measurements. For each species a minimum of four males and workers and two queens, were examined (except for A. dorsata; see Table 1 for sample numbers in parentheses).

thumbnail
Table 1. Body and eye measurements of five honeybee species.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057702.t001

Eye and Body Size Measurements

Size measurements of thorax, compound eyes and ocelli were performed on digital photographs using ImageJ (National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda Maryland, USA). Photographs were taken with stereomicroscopes (Nikon SMZ-U equipped with DS-Fi1, Tokyo, Japan and Leica EZ4D, Leica Microsystems, Wetzlar, Germany) at different magnifications (4–16×). Size measurements were calibrated with respect to photographs of an object micrometer at the same magnification. As a measure of body size we used intertegulae span, which was previously shown to be an appropriate estimate of body size in bees [15], [27]. Eye length was measured as the longest linear measure across the eye from a frontal view. Eye surface area measurements were performed on eye surface replicas made of nail polish [26]. Replicas were photographed using light microscopes (Nikon Labophot equipped with DS-Fi1 and Zeiss Axiophot, Zeiss Germany equipped with Spot Insight Color, Diagnostic Instruments Inc., USA) at 100–400× magnifications in overlapping sections and subsequently stitched in Adobe Photoshop CS2 (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA, USA). The eye surface was then measured by tracing the outlines in ImageJ. Measurements of the ocelli were performed as the longest linear measure across the median and the left lateral ocellus.

Ommatidia Measurements

Ommatidia number was determined by manually marking all facet imprints of the eye replica in ImageJ. To measure the facet diameter, a row of 5 ommatidia in all three axes (x, y and z) was measured in ImageJ. We then calculated the mean diameter of a single ommatidium [13]. Measurements were performed on the largest facets. Additionally, eye maps were created to illustrate facet diameter distribution over the entire eye surface. ImageJ, Meshlab (Visual Computing Lab - ISRI - CNR, http://meshlab.sourceforge.net/) and CorelDraw X5 (Corel Corporation) was used to create the maps. In brief, ommatidia diameters were estimated from the distance between neighboring ommatidia centers. For visualization, ommatidia diameters across the eye surface were color coded.

Mating Flight Activity

In addition to the size measurements, we analyzed published records of drone and queen flight activity of all investigated species. Reported flight times were corrected for solar azimuth differences according to the procedure employed by Otis et al. [28] when such a correction was not performed in the original study. For observations over several days we only report the total range of drone flight activity (e.g. [29]), and we aimed to avoid pseudo-replication from subsequent citations of the same original data set. Ambient light intensity is a function of solar elevation and not only depends on the time of the day but also on geographic latitude and time of the year. To transform daytime records of mating flight times to solar elevation information, we calculated the range of solar elevation for the flight period of all studies that reported location and date, using equations provided by the NOAA (U.S. Department of Commerce).

Statistics

Body and eye parameters were compared between and within species with a Kruskal-Wallis H test. All P-values below the 5%-level were considered to be statistically significant. Statistical analyses were performed with Statistica 10 (StatSoft Inc., OK, USA).

Results

Body and Eye Size

The five investigated honeybee species differ with respect to body and eye size. The largest variation is found in workers and the smallest in drones (Table 1). Body size (intertegulae span) differs significantly, both between castes and sexes within species (Handreniformis(2,11) = 8.6,p<0.05; Hcerana(2,17) = 12.3,p<0.005; Hdorsata(2,12) = 7.6,p<0.05; Hflorea(2,13) = 8.6,p<0.05; Hmellifera(2,15) = 12.4,p<0.005) and within castes and sexes among species (Hworkers(4,31) = 28.6,p<0.005; Hqueens(4,15) = 12.2,p<0.05; Hdrones(4,22) = 16.5,p<0.005). In all species, queens and males are larger than workers, whereas the polarity of size differences between males and queens varies among species. Males are larger than queens in A. andreniformis and A. mellifera, smaller than queens in A. dorsata, and similar in body size in A. florea and A. cerana (Fig. 1, Table 1). Among drones, A. mellifera drones are larger than A. dorsata drones, and the drones of A. andreniformis, A. florea and A. cerana are similar in size. Our results are consistent with previous weight measurements performed on several honeybee species [11].

thumbnail
Figure 1. Morphological measurements of eye parameters in five honeybee species.

Eye surface area (left panel) and ommatidia number (right panel) measured in workers (A, B), queens (C, D) and drones (E, F) of the Western and four Asian honeybee species. Species are indicated by color (A. andreniformis – yellow, A. florea – red, A. dorsata – black, A. mellifera – blue, A. cerana – green). Each circle represents one measured individual (see Table 1 for sample sizes). Trend lines are based on all measured specimens (A: y = 1.82x−2.15, R2 = 0.79; B: y = 1,421.66 x +1,399.65, R2 = 0.94; C: y = 1.46x −2.75, R2 = 0.81; D: y = 514.11x +2,312.18, R2 = 0.38; E: y = 2.88x –2.29, R2 = 0.51; F: y = 1,250.49x +4,264.23, R2 = 0.34).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057702.g001

Eye size (eye surface area) differs significantly between sexes and castes in all species except for A. dorsata (Handreniformis(2,10) = 7.9,p<0.05; Hcerana(2,12) = 9.7,p<0.01; Hdorsata(2,9) = 5.4,p = 0.07; Hflorea(2,10) = 7.9,p<0.05; Hmellifera(2,10) = 7.9,p<0.05). Within species, eye size is similar between workers and queens (Fig. 1, Fig. 2, Table 1). However, due to larger body size, queen eyes appear relatively smaller. In contrast, drones have much larger and differently shaped eyes (Fig. 2). Differences between drone and female eyes range from 2.6-fold in A. dorsata to 5.4-fold in A. florea (Fig. 1, Table 1). While eye size of queens and workers positively scales with body size in all species, drone eye size does not simply so. In particular, A. florea and A. dorsata drone eyes are disproportionally enlarged relative to body size compared with drones of the other species (Fig. 1, Table 1).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Facet size distribution in compound eyes.

Eye maps illustrate eye size differences and facet size distribution between castes and sexes of the Western and four Asian honeybee species. Each circle represents one facet lens. Color indicates facet size (scale at the bottom). The largest facets in queens and workers are usually found in the fronto-ventral region of the eye. In drones, facet diameters are dorso-ventrally separated and the largest facets are found in the dorsal two-thirds of the eye. The eye maps are accompanied by line drawings of all individuals, which allow comparison of eye placement, eye size and ocellar size between species, sexes and castes (all to scale, scale bar 5 mm). Relative facet diameter frequencies are illustrated by histograms (right panel, bin width 2 µm) of one randomly selected queen (gray line), worker (dashed line) and drone (dotted line) from each species.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057702.g002

Ommatidia Numbers and Facet Size Distribution

The number of ommatidia range from c. 3,500 in the queens of A. cerana to c. 11,000 in drones of the Western honeybee (Fig. 1, Table 1). In all species, except for A. andreniformis, queens possess the lowest, while drones possess the highest number of ommatidia (Table 1). The number of ommatidia differs significantly between castes and sexes in all species (Fig. 1, Table 1; Handreniformis(2,10) = 6.9,p<0.05; Hcerana(2,9) = 7.0,p<0.05; Hflorea(2,10) = 7.9,p<0.05; Hmellifera(2,10) = 7.9,p<0.05). For A. dorsata (Hdorsata(2,8) = 5.8,p = 0.054) the p-value was marginal significant, due to the low sample size of queens (N = 1). Ommatidia numbers in worker and drone eyes differ among all species (Hworkers(4,20) = 18.3,p<0.005; Hdrones(4,17) = 14.3,p<0.01), whereas ommatidia number in queens do not differ among species (Hqueens(4,10) = 8.1,p = 0.09) despite strong variation in body size.

In apposition compound eyes facet diameters are usually not evenly distributed along the eye surface and the largest facets are commonly found in regions associated with high spatial acuity and light sensitivity [30]. In Apis, facet diameter frequency and distribution differs between sex and castes. Both female castes have a nearly Gaussian distribution of facet diameters and their largest facets are located in the fronto-ventral region of the eye (Fig. 2). In all species, except A. mellifera, queen eyes are composed of ommatidia with larger facet diameter compared with worker eyes. The largest difference in facet diameter between worker and queen eyes is found in A. dorsata (Fig. 2, Table 1). Drone eyes show a strong dorso-ventral regionalization, indicated by a steep transition in facet diameter (Fig. 2). The dorsal two thirds of the eye are equipped with large facets of which the largest are located in the dorso-lateral region. The ventral third is equipped with smaller facets that are similar in size to the largest facets in workers. The distinct dorso-ventral separation is also reflected in the diameter frequency distribution, which is flatter and shows more than one maximum in all species (Fig. 2).

Ocelli

The three ocelli are located at the top of the head in queens and workers and frontal in drones (Fig. 2). Usually, the median ocellus is larger than the lateral ocelli (Table 1). Ocellus size differs significantly between castes and sexes in all species, both for the median (Handreniformis(2,11) = 8.6,p<0.05; Hcerana(2,17) = 13.8,p<0.005; Hdorsata(2,12) = 2.6,p = 0.28; Hflorea(2,13) = 10.6,p<0.01; Hmellifera(2,14) = 9.6,p<0.01), and the lateral ocellus (Handreniformis(2,11) = 8.6,p<0.05; Hcerana(2,17) = 12.4,p<0.005; Hdorsata(2,12) = 7.3,p<0.05; Hflorea(2,13) = 9.5,p<0.01; Hmellifera(2,14) = 8.9,p<0.05), with the exception of the median ocellus in A. dorsata. The largest ocelli are found in A. dorsata and the smallest in workers of the dwarf honeybees A. andreniformis and A. florea. In general, ocelli are larger in queens than in workers of the small species (A. andreniformis, A. florea, A. cerana), similar in size in A. mellifera, and smaller in A. dorsata. In drones, ocellus diameters show the same trend as facet size; A. dorsata drones have the largest, followed by A. mellifera. Ocelli in A. florea are larger than in the similarly sized drones of A. andreniformis and A. cerana (Table 1).

Discussion

Our study documents sex and caste-specific variation in the compound eyes of five honeybee species. In queens and workers, eye size, ommatidia number, facet diameter and ocellus size positively correlate with body size among the species. Although queens are larger, queen and worker eyes are of similar size, but worker eyes on average comprise a higher number of ommatidia with smaller facets. Compared with the female castes, drones of all species show enlarged and highly modified compound eyes but drone eye size does not simply correlate with body size. Particularly, drones of the dwarf honeybee A. florea have disproportionately enlarged eyes in relation to body size and exhibit more ommatidia than drone eyes of the giant honeybee A. dorsata and almost as many ommatidia as drone eyes of the Western honeybee, A. mellifera (Figs. 1, 2; Table 1). Overall, the findings of our study indicate a greater variability in the design of drone visual systems than previously assumed and this variability is probably the result of the interaction of species-specific body size limitations and sex-specific selection pressures.

Female Eye Morphology and Female Behavior

In all species, eye size is similar between queens and workers, but queens usually possess less yet larger ommatidia (Fig. 1). Queens spend most of their lives in the colony where vision plays a minor role. The few occasions when they leave the hive (mating flights, swarming, absconding and migration) certainly require good spatial vision; however, the demands for visual acuity are likely less strong than for workers, which need to detect and identify flowers and orient themselves during foraging flights. Additionally, we found that the compound eyes of queens from the open nesting honeybee species (A. andreniformis, A. florea and A. dorsata) have relatively enlarged facets compared with workers, while such an enlargement is only marginal (A. cerana) or absent (A. mellifera) in the cavity nesting species. The two cavity nesting species are closely related [31], yet we do not know whether the smaller relative queen facet size (i) is related to the predominant life inside the nest, (ii) constitutes a phylogenetic constraint or (iii) is a byproduct of other selection pressures (e.g. [32]).

The largest caste difference in facet diameters is found in A. dorsata. Queen facets are enlarged, at the expense of ommatidia number, suggesting that queens trade-off spatial resolution for increased light sensitivity, a likely adaptation for crepuscular mating activity. In the two dwarf honeybee species, the decrease in worker body (and thus eye) size is accompanied by a reduction of facet diameters, but not ommatidia number, suggesting that workers trade-off light sensitivity to retain spatial resolution, which is important in foraging tasks. The smaller facet diameters, however, may limit their foraging abilities during the twilight hours, compared with the species that possess larger facet diameters [13]. Somanathan et al. [24] recently investigated the worker eyes of four honeybee species. In concordance with our results, they found smaller eyes and facets in the dwarf species and enlarged facet and ocellar lenses in A. dorsata. Model calculation further suggest, that light sensitivity is highest in A. dorsata and lowest in A. florea, which correlates with the observed temporal foraging patterns ([24] and citations therein).

It must be noted that both spatial resolution and light sensitivity not only depend on the morphology of the peripheral optical system (ommatidia diameters and numbers), but also on the photoreceptor arrangement (interommatidial angles, rhabdom diameter) and potentially on neuronal computation strategies [18], [19], [23], [33]. For instance, model calculations by Somanathan et al. [24] suggested that A. dorsata has the lowest spatial resolution, despite having the highest number of ommatidia and that their light sensitivity is additionally increased due to larger rhabdom diameters. So far, detailed measurements of the interommatidial and acceptance angles, light sensitivity, and behavioral assessment of the spatial resolution, object detection threshold and light intensity threshold are lacking for all Asian honeybee species.

Our measured eye parameters of A. florea differ from the previously published data [24]. We suggest that regional intra-specific variation in body (and thus eye) size may account for the c. 900 more ommatidia we find in A. florea workers. We investigated A. florea workers and drones from Iran, while Somanathan et al. [24] collected workers in India. No subspecies are officially recognized in A. florea, but morphometric studies revealed the existence of several morphotypes and workers from Iranian populations are larger than workers from Indian populations (own measurements and [34]). Similarly, our measurements on A. mellifera differ from earlier reports (e.g. [17], [26]). The Western honeybee is widely distributed and comprises several distinct subspecies. For instance, three of the economically important subspecies, A. m. mellifera, A. m. carnica and A. m. ligustica differ with respect to body size (in this sequence, A. m. mellifera being the largest [35]). In addition, historical and regional differences in bee keeping management (e.g. the used foundation cell size) artificially constrain body size and may account for large intra-specific variation [36].

Drone Eye Morphology and Drone Behavior

Several studies on eye morphology and drone chasing behavior in A. mellifera indicate that the drone compound eye is specialized to detect small moving objects against the bright blue sky [9], [37][41]. The eyes show distinct regional specializations, e.g. extremely enlarged facets located in the dorsal region of the eye [26]. Our study demonstrates a similar organization in drone eyes of all investigated honeybee species (Fig. 2). However, the drone compound eyes of A. florea and A. dorsata apparently have evolved specific adaptations. A. florea drones have much larger compound eyes than drones of the other dwarf honeybee A. andreniformis. Furthermore, relative to body size, their eyes are larger than in drones of all other honeybee species. The enlargement of the compound eyes is accompanied by an increase of ommatidia number suggesting a substantial increase in spatial resolution. In contrast, the large compound eyes of A. dorsata drones consist of a lower number of ommatidia compared with A. florea drones, but these ommatidia exhibit much larger facet lenses, suggesting a significant increase in light and, probably more important, contrast sensitivity [20].

In most honeybee species drone mating flights start around noon or early afternoon at times of highest solar elevation and light intensities and last until late afternoon (Fig. 3). Mating flight times in A. dorsata diverge from this pattern; they occur at sunset and are much shorter in duration. Daily onset and end of mating flights are strongly affected by changing ambient light and temperature conditions as well as the animal’s motivation [42]. In A. dorsata, facet and ocelli diameters resemble those of some strictly nocturnal bee species [17], [23], and may thus allow mating flights around sunset. A. andreniformis also seems to have a narrow mating flight period that is constrained to times of highest light intensities (Fig. 3). The findings of our study indicate a correlation between morphological characters of drone eyes and species-specific mating flight times, which reflects the importance of visual mate detection in honeybee mating behavior.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Species-specific mating flight activity.

Drone (A, B, C) and queen (D, E, F) flight activity compiled from literature records [11], [29], [42], [45], [46], [49], [50], [56], [58][84]. (A, D) Temporal range and (B, E) corresponding solar elevation range of the flight time. When necessary, time was converted to solar azimuth time. For studies that did not report the date of observation, solar elevation could not be calculated (bars missing in B, E). (C, F) Graphical representation of the heads (left) and ommatidia diameters (right) for all species (scale bars below). Species are sorted in an ascending order of drone eye size, ommatidia and ocellar diameters.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057702.g003

We find a similar correlation between facet and eye size and mating flight time in queens. Both sexes are active at the same time, ensuring successful meeting and mating. Queens, however, possess smaller facets than drones, suggesting that light levels alone may not account for the large facets in drone eyes. Drones face a tremendous challenge when detecting a fast moving queen from a distance. The visual acuity in the dorsal eye region is high [43] but probably even more importantly, the enlarged facet lenses give Apis drones an extremely high contrast sensitivity [39]. The large facet diameters of A. dorsata drones may thus be particularly important to maximize contrast sensitivity in low light environments that generally limit visual contrast [20]. The smaller facets and ocelli in A. andreniformis may constrain mating flights to high noon when light level and contrast ratios are sufficiently high (Fig. 2, Fig. 3). In the case of A. dorsata, body size might have been a pre-adaptation, which allowed a shift of mating flights to lower light levels [16]. Interestingly, the only report on drone mating flight times in A. laboriosa, the similar-sized Himalayan sister species of A. dorsata, indicated that mating flights start in early afternoon [44]. This shift is most likely a response to the harsh weather conditions at high elevations and a similar shift of mating flights is observed in the second Asian mountain honeybee A. nuluensis [45].

Based on our observations, we hypothesize that ambient light intensity is a major factor for the timing of honeybee mating flights. Although mating flight times exhibit a high degree of variability (Fig. 3, left panels), our calculations show that at least some of the variation is a result of differences in location [46] and time of year [42], [47] and can be explained by differences in solar elevation (Fig. 3, right panels). Similar to hypotheses on worker foraging behavior [48], we suggest that eye morphology and ambient light intensity define a species-specific timeframe for mating behavior. In the case of geographically co-occurring honeybee species, mating flight times can be shortened and shifted within this basic timeframe according to sensitivity of the visual system [49], [50]. Recent studies in Australian Myrmecia ants demonstrated that worker foraging activity is exclusively controlled by absolute light levels [21], and caste and species-specific activity schedules are determined by eye morphology [22]. However, at this time we cannot exclude the possibility that mating flight times in honeybees are also affected by other environmental parameters, such as ambient temperature and humidity, which correlate with light intensity. In addition, an impact of ambient light intensity levels on mating flight activity does not exclude that mating behavior is regulated by the circadian clock [51], [52]. The clock likely regulates physiological processes involved in mating behavior in anticipation of the actual mating flight. Future experiments should focus on the hitherto unknown proximate physiological and neuronal mechanisms that generate narrow and temporally separated mating flight periods in Apis. The potential to quickly adapt the mating timeframe in response to sympatric honeybee species in order to avoid inter-specific interference provides an avenue for future research on the function and evolution of the mechanisms that regulate the timing of mating flights.

The Curious Case of Apis florea Drones

Characteristic of the honeybee mating behavior is that drones chase the queens [1]. Queens signal their presence by releasing their sex-pheromone, which triggers an upwind search by drones and also heightens their motivation to chase any small and dark object moving against the sky [39]. Although all Apis species are assumed to be highly sensitive to queen pheromone, differences in the number of olfactory sensilla suggest a unique exaggeration of the sex-pheromone specific olfactory system in A. mellifera [53], [54]. In contrast, A. florea drones have the lowest number and density of olfactory sensilla [53] and much smaller sex-pheromone processing macroglomeruli compared with A. mellifera drones [54]. Neural tissue maintenance and information processing are energetically costly and thus may be particularly prone to counter selection [55]. This limitation certainly affects the trade-off between different sensory systems, e.g. an enlarged visual system comes at the cost of a poorer olfactory system and vice versa. The current data on the olfactory and visual sensory systems in drone honeybees suggest that A. mellifera drones have specifically improved the sensitivity of their olfactory system in their evolution, whereas A. florea drones invested particularly in their visual system. We can only speculate about the ultimate causes for the differences among honeybee species. Brockmann and Brückner [53] suggested that low mate density may have promoted the evolution of a particularly sensitive olfactory system in A. mellifera. The question why drones of A. florea have evolved relatively enlarged eyes is of particular interest with respect to the fact that drones of the sister species A. andreniformis did not evolve similar traits. Almost nothing is known about the mating behavior and drone congregation areas of the latter two species [56]. Both species differ with respect to their preference for nesting and probably also mating areas [57]. However, current knowledge on mating related signals and cues and the specific tasks of the sensory system in honeybee mating behavior is limited and does not permit us to draw further conclusions about the evolution of their sensory systems.

Conclusion and Future Perspective

Based on the assumption that body size differences interact with sex and caste-specific selection pressures, we compared four different characters of the visual system in drones, queens and workers of five honeybee species. This approach successfully identified common patterns of adaptation within castes and revealed distinct adaptations in the drone eyes of two species, A. florea and A. dorsata. In general, the variability among species seems to be caused by the interaction of different factors, such as body size limitations, different selection pressures (e.g. selection for mate detection, foraging efficiency and fecundity that are exclusive to drones, workers and queens, respectively), temporal activity pattern and different relative roles of the sensory systems (e.g. the importance of vision vs. olfaction during mate detection). In the future, it will be interesting to test whether these morphological differences are accompanied by differences in the behavioral responses to visual and olfactory signals.

Acknowledgments

We thank K. Möller for her invaluable help with data collection, J. Makinson for providing A. andreniformis samples and D. Brückner, F. Etl and D. Ahrens for providing additional specimens of the other Apis species. We further thank J. Plant for linguistic improvements. Thanks are also due to the two anonymous reviewers and their helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MS AB JS. Performed the experiments: MS NN. Analyzed the data: MS. Wrote the paper: MS AB JS.

References

  1. 1. Winston ML (1991) The biology of the honey bee. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. 281 p.
  2. 2. Esslen J, Kaissling KE (1976) Zahl und Verteilung antennaler Sensillen bei der Honigbiene (Apis mellifera L.). Zoomorphology 83: 227–251.
  3. 3. Arnold G, Masson C, Budharugsa S (1985) Comparative study of the antennal lobes and their afferent pathway in the worker bee and the drone (Apis mellifera). Cell Tissue Res 242: 593–605.
  4. 4. Arnold G, Budharugsa S, Masson C (1988) Organization of the antennal lobe in the queen honey bee, Apis mellifera L (Hymenoptera, Apidae). Int J Insect Morphol Embryol 17: 185–195.
  5. 5. Groh C, Rössler W (2008) Caste-specific postembryonic development of primary and secondary olfactory centers in the female honeybee brain. Arthropod Struct Dev 37: 459–468.
  6. 6. Hepburn HR (2011) Absconding, migration and swarming. In: Hepburn HR, Radloff SE, editors. Honeybees of Asia. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. 133–158.
  7. 7. Wanner KW, Nichols AS, Walden KKO, Brockmann A, Luetje CW, et al. (2007) A honey bee odorant receptor for the queen substance 9-oxo-2-decenoic acid. P Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 14383–14388.
  8. 8. Brockmann A, Brückner D, Crewe RM (1998) The EAG response spectra of workers and drones to queen honeybee mandibular gland components: the evolution of a social signal. Naturwissenschaften 85: 283–285.
  9. 9. Menzel JG, Wunderer H, Stavenga DG (1991) Functional morphology of the divided compound eye of the honeybee drone (Apis mellifera). Tissue Cell 23: 525–535.
  10. 10. Radloff SE, Hepburn HR, Koeniger G (2003) Comparison of flight design of Asian honeybee drones. Apidologie 34: 353–358.
  11. 11. Koeniger G, Koeniger N, Phiancharoen M (2011) Comparative reproductive biology of honeybees. In: Hepburn HR, Radloff SE, editors. Honeybees of Asia. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. 159–206.
  12. 12. Michener CD (2007) The bees of the world. 2nd Edition. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. 992 p.
  13. 13. Kapustjanskij A, Streinzer M, Paulus HF, Spaethe J (2007) Bigger is better: implications of body size for flight ability under different light conditions and the evolution of alloethism in bumblebees. Funct Ecol 21: 1130–1136.
  14. 14. Spaethe J, Brockmann A, Halbig C, Tautz J (2007) Size determines antennal sensitivity and behavioral threshold to odors in bumblebee workers. Naturwissenschaften 94: 733–739.
  15. 15. Spaethe J, Chittka L (2003) Interindividual variation of eye optics and single object resolution in bumblebees. J Exp Biol 206: 3447–3453.
  16. 16. Wcislo WT, Tierney SM (2009) Behavioural environments and niche construction: the evolution of dim-light foraging in bees. Biol Rev 84: 19–37.
  17. 17. Greiner B, Ribi WA, Warrant EJ (2004) Retinal and optical adaptations for nocturnal vision in the halictid bee Megalopta genalis. Cell Tissue Res 316: 377–390.
  18. 18. Greiner B, Ribi WA, Warrant EJ (2005) A neural network to improve dim-light vision? Dendritic fields of first-order interneurons in the nocturnal bee Megalopta genalis. Cell Tissue Res 322: 313–320.
  19. 19. Somanathan H, Kelber A, Borges RM, Wallen R, Warrant EJ (2009) Visual ecology of Indian carpenter bees II: adaptations of eyes and ocelli to nocturnal and diurnal lifestyles. J Comp Physiol A 195: 571–583.
  20. 20. Warrant E, Dacke M (2011) Vision and visual navigation in nocturnal insects. Annu Rev Entomol 56: 239–254.
  21. 21. Narendra A, Reid SF, Hemmi JM (2010) The twilight zone: ambient light levels trigger activity in primitive ants. Proc Biol Sci 277: 1531–1538.
  22. 22. Narendra A, Reid SF, Greiner B, Peters RA, Hemmi JM, et al. (2011) Caste-specific visual adaptations to distinct daily activity schedules in Australian Myrmecia ants. Proc Biol Sci 278: 1141–1149.
  23. 23. Somanathan H, Borges RM, Warrant EJ, Kelber A (2008) Visual ecology of Indian carpenter bees I: Light intensities and flight activity. J Comp Physiol A 194: 97–107.
  24. 24. Somanathan H, Warrant EJ, Borges RM, Wallen R, Kelber A (2009) Resolution and sensitivity of the eyes of the Asian honeybees Apis florea, Apis cerana and Apis dorsata. J Exp Biol 212: 2448–2453.
  25. 25. Greiner B (2006) Visual adaptations in the night-active wasp Apoica pallens. J Comp Neurol 495: 255–262.
  26. 26. Ribi WA, Engels E, Engels W (1989) Sex and caste specific eye structures in stingless bees and honey bees (Hymenoptera: Trigonidae, Apidae). Entomol Gen 14: 233–242.
  27. 27. Cane JH (1987) Estimation of bee size using intertegular span (Apoidea). J Kansas Entomol Soc 60: 145–147.
  28. 28. Otis GW, Koeniger N, Rinderer TE, Hadisoesilo S, Yashida T, et al.. (2000) Comparative mating flight times of Asian honey bees. Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Tropical Bees: 137–141.
  29. 29. Woyke J, Wilde J, Wilde M (2001) Apis dorsata drone flights, collection of semen from everted endophalli and instrumental insemination of queens. Apidologie 32: 407–416.
  30. 30. Land MF (1997) Visual acuity in insects. Annu Rev Entomol 42: 147–177.
  31. 31. Koeniger N, Koeniger G, Smith D (2011) Phylogeny of the genus Apis In: Hepburn HR, Radloff SE, editors. Honeybees of Asia Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. 23–50.
  32. 32. Tan S, Amos W, Laughlin SB (2005) Captivity selects for smaller eyes. Curr Biol 15: R540–542.
  33. 33. Warrant EJ, Porombka T, Kirchner WH (1996) Neural image enhancement allows honeybees to see at night. Proc Biol Sci 263: 1521–1526.
  34. 34. Hepburn HR, Radloff SE, Otis GW, Fuchs S, Verma LR, et al. (2005) Apis florea: morphometrics, classification and biogeography. Apidologie 36: 359–376.
  35. 35. Ruttner F (2003) Naturgeschichte der Honigbienen. Stuttgart: Franck-Kosmos Verlag. 357 p.
  36. 36. Erickson EH, Lusby DA, Hoffman GD, Lusby EW (1990) On the size of cells: speculations on foundation as a colony management tool. Glean Bee Culture 118: 98–101, 174–174.
  37. 37. Gries M, Koeniger N (1996) Straight forward to the queen: Pursuing honeybee drones (Apis mellifera L.) adjust their body axis to the direction of the queen. J Comp Physiol A 179: 539–544.
  38. 38. Stavenga DG (1992) Eye regionalization and spectral tuning of retinal pigments in insects. Trends Neurosci 15: 213–218.
  39. 39. Vallet AM, Coles JA (1993) The perception of small objects by the drone honeybee. J Comp Physiol A 172: 183–188.
  40. 40. Peitsch D, Fietz A, Hertel H, Desouza J, Ventura DF, et al. (1992) The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision. J Comp Physiol A 170: 23–40.
  41. 41. Van Praagh JP, Ribi W, Wehrhahn C, Wittmann D (1980) Drone bees fixate the queen with the dorsal frontal part of their compound eyes. J Comp Physiol A 136: 263–266.
  42. 42. Taber S (1964) Factors influencing the circadian flight rhythm of drone honey bees. Ann Entomol Soc Am 57: 769–775.
  43. 43. Seidl R (1980) Die Sehfelder und Ommatidien-Divergenzwinkel der drei Kasten der Honigbiene (Apis mellifica). Verh dt zool Gesell: 367.
  44. 44. Underwood BA (1990) Time of drone flight of Apis laboriosa Smith in Nepal. Apidologie 21: 501–504.
  45. 45. Koeniger N, Koeniger G, Gries M, Tingek S, Kelitu A (1996) Reproductive isolation of Apis nuluensis Tingek, Koeniger and Koeniger, 1996 by species-specific mating time. Apidologie 27: 353–359.
  46. 46. Ruttner F, Woyke J, Koeniger N (1972) Reproduction in Apis cerana 1. Mating Behaviour. J Apic Res 11: 141–146.
  47. 47. Hepburn HR, Radloff SE (1998) Honeybees of Africa. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. 386 p.
  48. 48. Kelber A, Warrant EJ, Pfaff M, Wallen R, Theobald JC, et al. (2006) Light intensity limits foraging activity in nocturnal and crepuscular bees. Behav Ecol 17: 63–72.
  49. 49. Koeniger N, Koeniger G (2000) Reproductive isolation among species of the genus Apis. Apidologie 31: 313–339.
  50. 50. Hadisoesilo S, Otis GW (1996) Drone flight times confirm the species status of Apis nigrocincta Smith, 1861 to be a species distinct from Apis cerana F, 1793, in Sulawesi, Indonesia. Apidologie 27: 361–369.
  51. 51. Sasaki M (1990) Photoperiodic regulation of honeybee mating-flight time: exploitation of innately phase-fixed circadian oscillation. In: Hoshi M, Yamashita O, editors. Advances in Invertebrate Reproduction. Amsterdam, New York, Oxford: Elsevier Science Publishers 503–508.
  52. 52. Tychsen PH, Fletcher BS (1971) Studies on the rhythm of mating in the Queensland fruit fly, Dacus tryoni. J Insect Physiol 17: 2139–2156.
  53. 53. Brockmann A, Brückner D (2003) Drone antennae and the evolution of sex-pheromone communication in honeybees. Indian Bee J 65: 131–138.
  54. 54. Brockmann A, Brückner D (2001) Structural differences in the drone olfactory system of two phylogenetically distant Apis species, A. florea and A. mellifera. Naturwissenschaften 88: 78–81.
  55. 55. Niven JE, Laughlin SB (2008) Energy limitation as a selective pressure on the evolution of sensory systems. J Exp Biol 211: 1792–1804.
  56. 56. Nagaraja N, Brockmann A (2009) Drone congregation areas of red dwarf honeybee, Apis florea. Available from Nature Precedings <http://hdlhandlenet/10101/npre200939551> (2009).
  57. 57. Wongsiri S, Lekprayoon C, Thapa R, Thirakupt K, Rinderer TE, et al. (1996) Comparative biology of Apis andreniformis and Apis florea in Thailand. Bee World 77: 23–35.
  58. 58. Berg S, Koeniger N, Koeniger G, Fuchs S (1997) Body size and reproductive success of drones (Apis mellifera L.). Apidologie 28: 449–460.
  59. 59. Boucher M, Schneider SS (2009) Communication signals used in worker–drone interactions in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Anim Behav 78: 247–254.
  60. 60. Brockmann A, Dietz D, Spaethe J, Tautz J (2006) Beyond 9-ODA: Sex pheromone communication in the European honey bee Apis mellifera L. J Chem Ecol. 32: 657–667.
  61. 61. Buawangpang N, Sukumalanand P, Burgett M (2009) Apis florea drone flight: longevity and flight performance. Apidologie 40: 20–25.
  62. 62. Burgett DM, Titayavan M (2005) Apis florea drone flights: duration, temporal period and inter-flight period. J Apic Res 44: 36–37.
  63. 63. Burgett DM, Titayavan M, Sukumalanand P (2007) The drone mating flight of the Eastern honeybee (Apis cerana F.): Duration, temporal period, and inter-flight period. Nat hist bull Siam Soc 55: 99–104.
  64. 64. Fujiwara S, Miura H, Kumagai T, Sawaguchi T, Naya S, et al. (1994) Drone congregation of Apis cerana japonica (Radoszkowski, 1877) above large trees. Apidologie 25: 331–337.
  65. 65. Hadisoesilo S, Shanti EN, Kuntadi TH (2001) Drone flight times of Apis cerana and Apis mellifera in the island of Java; 2001 28. Oct - 01. Nov Durban, South Africa.
  66. 66. Jordan LA, Allsopp MH, Oldroyd BP, Wossler TC, Beekman M (2007) A scientific note on the drone flight time of Apis mellifera capensis and A. m. scutellata. Apidologie 38: 436–437.
  67. 67. Koeniger G, Koeniger N, Tingek S, Kelitu A (2000) Mating flights and sperm transfer in the dwarf honeybee Apis andreniformis (Smith, 1858). Apidologie 31: 301–311.
  68. 68. Koeniger N, Koeniger G, Tingek S, Kalitu A, Mardan M (1994) Drones of Apis dorsata (Fabricius 1793) congregate under the canopy of tall emergent trees in Borneo. Apidologie 25: 249–264.
  69. 69. Koeniger N, Koeniger G, Tingek S, Mardan M, Rinderer TE (1988) Reproductive isolation by different time of drone flight between Apis cerana Fabricius, 1793 and Apis vechti (Maa, 1953). Apidologie 19: 103–106.
  70. 70. Koeniger N, Koeniger G, Wongsiri S (1989) Mating and sperm transfer in Apis florea. Apidologie 20: 413–418.
  71. 71. Loper GM, Wolf WW, Taylor OR (1987) Detection and monitoring of honeybee drone congregation areas by radar. Apidologie 18: 163–172.
  72. 72. Loper GM, Wolf WW, Taylor ORJ (1992) Honey bee drone flyways and congregation areas - radar observations. J Kansas Entomol Soc 65: 223–230.
  73. 73. Mathew KP, Mathew S (1990) Further observations on characters and behaviour of Apis koschevnikovi, the ‘red bee’ of Sabah. Bee World 71: 61–66.
  74. 74. Oldroyd BP, Reddy MS, Chapman NC, Thompson GJ, Beekman M (2006) Evidence for reproductive isolation between two colour morphs of cavity nesting honey bees (Apis) in south India. Insect Soc 53: 428–434.
  75. 75. Punchihewa RWK, Koeniger N, Koeniger G (1990) Congregation of Apis cerana indica Fabricius 1798 drones in the canopy of trees in Sri Lanka. Apidologie 21: 201–208.
  76. 76. Rinderer TE, Oldroyd BP, Wongsiri S, Sylvester HA, Deguzman L, et al. (1993) Time of drone flight in four honey-bee species in south-eastern Thailand. J Apic Res 32: 27–33.
  77. 77. Tan NQ, Mardan M, Thai PH, Chinh PH (1999) Observations on multiple mating flights of Apis dorsata queens. Apidologie 30: 339–346.
  78. 78. Woyke J, Wilde J, Wilde M, Reddy CC, Cervancia C (2005) Workers often predominate in dusk ‘drone flights’ of the giant honey bee Apis dorsata. J Apic Res 44: 130–132.
  79. 79. Yoshida T, Saito J, Kajigaya N (1994) The mating flight times of native Apis cerana japonica Radoszkowski and introduced Apis mellifera L in sympatric conditions. Apidologie 25: 353–360.
  80. 80. Haddad N, Bataineh A, Megdadi O, Rinderer T, de Guzman L, et al.. (2012) Observation on Apis florea in the Middle East and North Africa Region. 5th Eurbee Conference. Halle, Germany.
  81. 81. Koeniger G, Koeniger N, Pechhacker H, Ruttner F, Berg S (1989) Assortative mating in a mixed population of European honeybees Apis mellifera ligustica and Apis mellifera carnica. Insect Soc 36: 129–138.
  82. 82. Verma LR, Rana BS, Mattu VK (1990) Mating behaviour of Apis cerana Fab. and Apis mellifera Linn. in the Shimla Hills of the northwest Himalayas. J Apic Res 3: 130–132.
  83. 83. Lensky Y, Demter M (1985) Mating flights of the queen honeybee (Apis mellifera) in a subtropical climate. Comp Biochem Physiol A 81: 229–241.
  84. 84. Woyke J (1975) Natural and instrumental insemination of Apis cerana indica in India. J Apic Res 14: 153–159.