Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

An Estimate of Avian Mortality at Communication Towers in the United States and Canada

  • Travis Longcore ,

    longcore@urbanwildlands.org

    Affiliations The Urban Wildlands Group, Los Angeles, California, United States of America, Spatial Sciences Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Catherine Rich,

    Affiliation The Urban Wildlands Group, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Pierre Mineau,

    Affiliation National Wildlife Research Centre, Science and Technology Branch, Environment Canada, Ottawa, Canada

  • Beau MacDonald,

    Affiliation The Urban Wildlands Group, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Daniel G. Bert,

    Affiliation National Wildlife Research Centre, Science and Technology Branch, Environment Canada, Ottawa, Canada

  • Lauren M. Sullivan,

    Affiliation Department of Geography, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Erin Mutrie,

    Affiliation National Wildlife Research Centre, Science and Technology Branch, Environment Canada, Ottawa, Canada

  • Sidney A. Gauthreaux Jr,

    Affiliation Department of Biological Sciences, Clemson University, Clemson, South Carolina, United States of America

  • Michael L. Avery,

    Affiliation National Wildlife Research Center, Florida Field Station, United States Department of Agriculture/Wildlife Services, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America

  • Robert L. Crawford,

    Affiliation Thomasville, Georgia, United States of America

  • Albert M. Manville II,

    Affiliation Division of Migratory Bird Management, United States Fish and Wildlife Service, Arlington, Virginia, United States of America

  • Emilie R. Travis,

    Affiliation Department of Forest and Wildlife Ecology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin, United States of America

  • David Drake

    Affiliation Department of Forest and Wildlife Ecology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin, United States of America

Abstract

Avian mortality at communication towers in the continental United States and Canada is an issue of pressing conservation concern. Previous estimates of this mortality have been based on limited data and have not included Canada. We compiled a database of communication towers in the continental United States and Canada and estimated avian mortality by tower with a regression relating avian mortality to tower height. This equation was derived from 38 tower studies for which mortality data were available and corrected for sampling effort, search efficiency, and scavenging where appropriate. Although most studies document mortality at guyed towers with steady-burning lights, we accounted for lower mortality at towers without guy wires or steady-burning lights by adjusting estimates based on published studies. The resulting estimate of mortality at towers is 6.8 million birds per year in the United States and Canada. Bootstrapped subsampling indicated that the regression was robust to the choice of studies included and a comparison of multiple regression models showed that incorporating sampling, scavenging, and search efficiency adjustments improved model fit. Estimating total avian mortality is only a first step in developing an assessment of the biological significance of mortality at communication towers for individual species or groups of species. Nevertheless, our estimate can be used to evaluate this source of mortality, develop subsequent per-species mortality estimates, and motivate policy action.

Introduction

On the morning of September 11, 1948, “a good number of dead, dying, and exhausted birds” were found at the base of the WBAL radio tower in Baltimore, Maryland [1]. Reports of such avian mortality at communication towers in North America became common in the 1950s [2][7]. These observations were consistent with the long documented mortality of birds at lights, including lighthouses [8], light towers [9], buildings [1], [10], and ceilometers [1], [11]. Although initially dismissed as being of minor consequence [12], the ongoing and chronic mortality of nocturnally migrating birds at lighted structures has become a recognized conservation issue [7], [13][15]. Bats are also killed in collisions with tall towers in unknown numbers [16][18]. An estimate of the total number of birds killed at communication towers in the United States and Canada is particularly relevant because the current transition from analog to exclusively digital broadcasting in the United States is expected to lead to the construction of more tall towers and a similar trend will likely follow in Canada.

In 1979, Banks [13] developed a widely circulated estimate of avian mortality at television towers, which revised upward a previous estimate by Mayfield [12]. In Banks’s assessment of various sources of human-caused avian mortality, he extrapolated the results of three studies at tall towers – two in Florida [19], [20] and one in North Dakota (for which he did not provide a citation but which was almost certainly [21]) – to all television towers. He calculated the average mortality at these three sites to be roughly 2,500 birds per year, and multiplied it by the number of television towers (1,010 in 1979). He then assumed that half of all television towers would cause a hazard to migrating birds. The resulting estimate of annual mortality was 1,250,000 [13]. Also in 1979, Avery [22] applied bird mortality results from seven towers that had been monitored for at least 10 years and derived an overall mortality estimate of 940,000/year for the United States. More recent estimates of total avian mortality at towers in the United States by the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service (USFWS) in 2001 [14], [23] adjusted the Banks estimate by accounting for the increased number of towers since 1979. Application of Banks’s method today results in an estimate of 4–5 million birds killed annually by tall towers, with Manville [15], [24] indicating a possibility of mortality an order of magnitude higher.

No estimate of avian mortality at communication towers has been made for the United States and Canada as a whole, and the only estimate for Canada was presented in a preliminary unpublished report preceding this paper. The bulk of species killed at towers in the United States and Canada are Neotropical migrants, i.e., birds that breed in Canada and the United States and spend the non-breeding period south of the U.S. border [13], [25]. Because the ranges of these species extend into Canada, mortality in both the United States and Canada contribute to their population dynamics.

In this paper we develop a new estimate of avian mortality at communication towers in the United States and Canada. This estimate derives from a review and re-analysis of tower mortality studies (following [26]). We improve on Longcore et al. [26] by adjusting mortality records at towers for sampling effort, search efficiency, and scavenging, and by incorporating additional studies. We produced a regression for avian mortality by tower height and then applied this regression to a geographic database of communication towers for the United States and Canada. This approach recognizes that taller towers kill more birds on average than do shorter towers [26][28], but also incorporates mortality estimates for lighted towers that are less than 600 ft (∼180 m) above ground level (AGL), which have previously been left out of estimates of total avian mortality. These “shorter” (60–180 m) lighted towers, which constitute >95% of lighted towers, do regularly kill birds [28][30] and their sheer number argues against ignoring them. We do not, however, estimate mortality from collisions with other lighted structures. Attraction to light at night leads to avian mortality at buildings, monuments, cooling towers, bridges, offshore platforms, ships, lighthouses, and wind turbines [24], [31], [32], and the same group of species (Neotropical migrants) are especially susceptible.

Our goal is to improve upon past estimates, which relied on a very limited set of data and did not reflect current understanding of the tower height–mortality relationship. Because of the nature of the existing data on avian mortality at towers and the lack of a systematic continent-wide survey effort, additional field studies will be required to refine further our approach. Our results do, however, increase both the transparency and accuracy associated with the estimate of this source of avian mortality.

Methods

We assigned average mortality values to tower height classes (every 30 m) using a regression of tower height by annual mortality (following [26]). Longcore et al. [26] identified reports of birds killed at 26 communication towers over at least two migratory seasons (e.g., spring and fall, two falls), consisting of a minimum of 10 total carcass-searching visits per site. We added figures from additional studies [33], [34], tested the sensitivity of the regression to inclusion of studies, and developed adjustments for sampling effort, search efficiency, and scavenging to produce estimates of mortality.

Sensitivity of Tower Height–mortality Regression

We collected as many studies of bird mortality at communication towers as possible from the literature and, when necessary, obtained raw data from study authors. Some studies had to be dropped from our analysis (e.g., [28]) because we were unable to obtain data from study authors and published reports did not allow us to assign mortality to specific towers. Because the number of tower studies available to us was finite, and because the choice of studies may have influenced our results, we tested the extent to which the regression was robust to sampling variation among the towers available for analysis. We used a randomization and resampling procedure to select random subsets of the 38 towers included in the analysis. To explore a range of plausible tower subsets that could produce a regression, we resampled subsets that included just under half of the available towers (18) up to those with one fewer than the complete dataset (37 towers) and re-iterated the sampling procedure 5,000 times. We used the natural logarithm of both the dependent and independent variables to normalize their distributions.

Adjustment for Scavenging and Search Efficiency

Loss of birds to scavengers and failure to detect all dead birds (search efficiency) are sources of error and variation in tower studies. Some authors have opted to apply searching and scavenging factors to final kill estimates (e.g., [28], [35]). Recognizing that search efficiency and scavenging losses are likely tower-specific, we opted to correct the number of kills at each tower before regressing these estimated losses against tower height.

We assumed that scavenging would be lower at a small tower that sporadically generates only a few mortalities compared with a well-established tall tower that kills birds reliably and therefore maintains scavenger interest [36][39]. This assumption is supported by high scavenging rates documented at tall towers such as WCTV in Florida [20], [36], [38] and rapid increases in scavenging when researchers provide carcasses [33]. Even with extensive scavenger control efforts, Stoddard estimated that he was losing at least 10% of bird carcasses to scavengers daily [40]. Therefore, we adjusted our scavenging rate by tower height.

We assumed that it is easier to find carcasses under a short tower because carcasses are likely to be less dispersed under shorter guy wires or in the absence of guy wires. Whether the area around the tower is bare or heavily vegetated will affect both scavenging and search rates [41]. Open habitats with little concealing vegetation are, predictably, more conducive to efficient searching for carcasses [41]. Scavengers detecting prey by sight can find the carcasses more easily as well. Notwithstanding the use of smell by some carnivores to find prey, dense cover makes it more difficult in general for both scavengers and searchers to find carcasses [42]. Support for our assumptions about the effect of cover on these rates is found in research on avian mortality caused by pesticides, power lines, and wind turbines [41][45]. We avoided attempts to calculate probability of detection by searchers that involved the “life expectancy” of carcasses because these methods are biased [46]. If a carcass was not found on the first search day, the probability that it will be found on subsequent days is considerably less than the average search rate would suggest. Therefore, for the purpose of this analysis, the likelihood that a carcass was found more than one day after it was generated is considered negligible. Removal of dead birds by scavengers at sites with regular mortality also follows an exponential decay model such that the probability of small dead birds remaining to be found falls quickly following the mortality event [45], [47].

We divided towers into height classes to which we could assign differential search and scavenging rates. Based on breaks in the raw tower mortality data, we chose to divide the towers into three height classes: 0–200 m, 201–400 m, and ≥401 m. To assign search and scavenging rates we relied on our published summaries of available rates from a range of carcass searching contexts (Table 1) [41], [42], other existing studies and reviews [37], [43], [44], [46], [48], and values reported at the towers in our dataset where these rates were measured [28], [33], [34], [49]. Taking into account patterns from these data, we used tower height as well as any information about cover as a way to assign search and scavenging corrections by height and cover class to the towers for which these rates had not been measured and reported by the authors (Table 2). All search and scavenging rates, both measured and assigned, are reported in Table 3.

thumbnail
Table 1. Average search and scavenging rates taken from pesticide impact studies [42].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Assumed rates for search efficiency and scavenger removal by tower height and habitat type when not provided by investigator.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Summary of factors used to develop the search and scavenging correction for bird mortality at communication towers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.t003

We investigated the sensitivity of our final results to these assumptions about search efficiency and scavenging by recalculating our total mortality estimates while assigning the average search efficiency and scavenging rates reported from those studies that did estimate these rates. This approach tested the alternative assumption that studies from all towers where search efficiency or scavenging were not measured had the same search efficiency, scavenging rate, or both, as did studies at the towers where they were measured, regardless of the physical conditions at the tower or the height of the tower.

Adjustment for Sampling Effort and Design

Studies included in the tower height–mortality regression varied in sampling design and duration. Following Longcore et al. [26], we required a minimum of 10 searches for a study to be included. Authors of most of the studies used in the regression assumed that most birds would be found by sampling during peak migration, on bad weather days preceding or following the passing of a cold front (e.g., J. Herron, pers. comm.), or both (Table 4). The logic behind this approach is that many high avian mortality days are correlated with these factors [31]. Nevertheless, “trickle kills” on fair weather days even outside the typical migration period can contribute substantially to overall mortality [40]. Substantial mortality during clear and calm weather during the migration season has also been documented [30], [50] (Figure 1). For these reasons we used raw data from two studies that carried out daily carcass searches – WCTV Florida tower data from 1956–1967 initiated by Herbert L. Stoddard and Tall Timbers Research Station [40] and North Dakota “Omega” tower [21], [51], [52] – as a baseline to develop estimates of the effectiveness of the various sampling designs for the 38 tower studies included in our dataset. The Florida estimates were averaged over the 10 years of sampling during which height of tower and predator control were the same; the North Dakota estimates are for two years of sampling. When the estimate was (partially) based on sampling outside the migratory period (as defined), we used the Florida dataset, which had continuous, year-round sampling. We did not, however, correct upward all kill estimates to account for the trickle of kills recorded in the non-migratory seasons. We believe, therefore, that our estimates are conservative. To control for differences between spring and fall migration we developed estimates for both spring and fall separately.

thumbnail
Table 4. Summary data with sampling efficiency correction for the 38 studies used to develop an estimate of bird mortality at communication towers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.t004

thumbnail
Figure 1. Relationship of bird fatalities to free airspace at WCTV Tower, 1956–1967.

Raw data from Crawford and Engstrom (2001) were used to plot daily bird fatalities against the mean free airspace between the top of the tower and the cloud ceiling each day. Days with maximum ceiling were excluded. Daily avian mortality increases significantly as free airspace decreases (Ln(Bird Fatalities +1) = 1.443928 – 0.0016667 · Mean Free Airspace (m), R2 = 0.17, p<0.001).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.g001

To adjust for the kills between sampling days during the migratory seasons we resampled (with replacement) daily mortality data from the Florida and North Dakota datasets within each of the spring and fall migration periods by randomly selecting a subset of days and summing avian mortality for the selected days. We calculated average bird mortality for 5,000 iterations and then used the ratio of the average bird mortality from the 5,000 iterations to the total number of birds killed during either spring or fall migration or outside of the migration period to adjust mortality estimates for studies without daily sampling. We averaged estimates between the Florida and North Dakota datasets. This adjustment was applied to studies where researchers sampled on bad weather days (see below) and to those with weekly sampling outside the migration period.

For studies that did not provide complete details on their sampling design, we made simplifying assumptions (see below). If more than one sampling strategy was used, we developed estimates for each and used the sum as our overall estimate. For example, sampling may have been done weekly (regular sampling) outside of the migration period and also on “bad weather days” during the migration period.

We defined the spring and fall migration periods as a 60-day window before and after the migration peak for both spring and fall for each dataset, recognizing that for some recent studies (e.g., [28]) monitoring only occurred during the three-week peak of migration. We determined the peak for the Florida and North Dakota datasets by plotting the number of birds killed (from the raw data) against Julian date for all years of data combined and using negative exponential smoothing.

Some investigators reported the total number of days sampled during one or both migration periods and sometimes outside the migration periods. When the sampling interval (e.g., weekly) was identified in the study design, we constrained the resampling procedure to randomly select a day within that sampling interval. If no sampling interval was defined, selection was random.

Some investigators sampled on so-called “bad weather days” or following bad weather nights, i.e., overcast, often associated with advancing cold fronts and potentially including precipitation. Usually no other information was provided to define bad weather or the number of days when bad weather occurred. High bird mortality at communication towers is correlated with bad weather days [40], [50], [53]. This is shown by plotting ln(n+1)-transformed daily mortality data from the Florida tower dataset for the 1956–1967 fall migrations against the mean free airspace (distance between the top of the tower and the bottom of the cloud cover). Days where maximum free airspace was recorded were excluded from analysis because measurements did not vary for total ceiling greater than 610 m (2,000 ft). Mortality for days with mean ceiling at the maximum was 4.0–8.0 birds per day (95% C.I., n = 871), while mortality for all days with less than the maximum ceiling was 16.0–33.5 birds per day (95% C.I., n = 569). Considering these remaining points, a linear regression reveals a highly significant effect of mean free airspace, but also low explanatory power (Figure 1). Based on these data, we used days with mean free airspace equal to or below 335 m (1,100 ft) as an index of bad weather days because mortality was significantly lower on days with airspace greater than 335 m (10.3–17.8 birds per day, 95% C.I., n = 387) compared with days with airspace below this threshold (21.5–73.3 birds per day, 95% C.I., n = 182).

For some studies, the only information provided was the number of days sampled and the timing of sampling (during migration or all year). For these studies we assumed that researchers sampled on bad weather days during migration when large bird kills at communication towers were expected, given that this was the response obtained when we were able to contact researchers to ask about papers where this detail was not provided (e.g., J. Herron, pers. comm.).

Several researchers sampled only on days when so called “big kills” were reported. The definitions of “big kill” were not included. The typical daily trickle of dead birds for the Florida dataset over the 1956–1967 period was five. We therefore defined big kills as six or more birds located after any given night.

We investigated the sensitivity of our results to our assumptions about sampling effort by varying these assumptions for the 13 studies in our dataset that either did not indicate the number of days sampled or did not provide a definition of sampling design, or did neither. Some researchers had indicated that they had sampled on overcast or bad weather days or following bad weather days. For all of these studies and for those that did not mention anything specific, we made the conservative assumption that towers were sampled on bad weather days. We then recalculated the sampling adjustment and total mortality using three different scenarios: 1) researchers sampled on bad weather days and weekly during migration (e.g., [49]); 2) researchers sampled on bad weather days and weekly all year (e.g., [33]; excludes 5 of the 13 studies that clearly indicated they only sampled during migration); and 3) researchers sampled only following big kill days, about which they were notified by personnel at the tower (e.g., [5]).

Evaluation of Model Correction Factors

We plotted either raw carcass counts or mortality estimates corrected for either sampling effort or search efficiency and scavenging, or both, against tower height and looked for improvements in the regression coefficient as an indication that the corrections improved the model.

Description of Communication Towers and their Characteristics

We used a Geographic Information System (GIS) to extract the locations and characteristics of towers in the Antenna Structure Registration (ASR) database maintained by the U.S. Federal Communications Commission (FCC) and the NAV CANADA obstruction database. The FCC data are freely available and we purchased a license for the Canadian obstruction data for the limited purpose of this study. We compared and crosschecked these with the FCC’s microwave tower database and the commercial TowerMaps database (also purchased, see http://www.towermaps.com/), which provides locations of cellular towers to potential lessees and incorporates both data for shorter towers and information that was not included in the FCC databases. We did considerable quality control on the tower data, confirming from independent sources that all towers greater than 300 m existed. This was necessary because the data were prone to multiple types of errors; for example, the FCC database included a record claiming to be located in the “Land of Oz” in Kansas, associated with geographic coordinates in Minnesota. Full details of the quality assurance are available from the authors.

The NAV CANADA database did not contain comprehensive information about either the presence of guy wires or the presence and type of lighting. We therefore relied on data from the FCC and TowerMaps datasets and assumed that lighting and guy wire use was similar in both countries for towers of the same height class, an assumption supported by the similarity in marking and lighting standards between the two countries. The U.S. Federal Aviation Administration requirements are found in the advisory circular AC 70/7460-1K. Those of Canada are found in Standard 621 of the Canadian Aviation Regulations.

Calculation of Annual Mortality

Avian mortality was estimated with the antilogarithm of the regression of the log transformed variables, which was adjusted for transformation bias using the smearing estimator after testing to confirm homoscedasticity of variance in the regression [54], [55]. Most recorded tower kill events take place at guyed towers, and steady-burning lights increase the probability of large tower kills [26], [28]. We assumed that unguyed towers caused 85% less mortality than guyed towers (midpoint of 69–100% estimate in [56]) and that towers without steady-burning lights caused 60% less mortality than towers with such lights (midpoint of 50–71% estimate in [28]). Following Longcore et al. [26], all estimates were calculated assuming that when both seasons were not measured, 75% of annual mortality occurred during the fall and 25% during the spring [40].

We overlaid locations of towers within each Bird Conservation Region (BCR) in the study area and calculated the number of towers in each 30 m height class for all towers ≥60 m. Bird Conservation Regions are divisions defined by habitat and topography that have been delineated for the purpose of bird conservation by the North American Bird Conservation Initiative and are endorsed by a range of bird conservation organizations and government agencies. BCRs are based on the North American ecoregions developed to promote international conservation efforts [57]. For each height class within each BCR we calculated the average number of birds killed per year, using the tower height–mortality regression adjusted for sampling effort, search efficiency, and scavenging as described above. For purposes of calculating total mortality we included all towers in the continental portions of the United States and Canada. Although most literature on tower mortality in North America describes studies from east of the Rocky Mountains, we included the West as well for purposes of estimating total mortality, a decision supported by records of tower mortality in Colorado [33], New Mexico [58], and Alaska [59], in addition to documented kills at lighthouses in California and British Columbia [60], [61]. We did not attempt to assign differential mortality for so-called flyways because radar studies and other observations strongly support the existence of “broad front” migration [62], [63]. To investigate this assumption, we plotted the residuals of the tower height–mortality regression by their geographic coordinates and calculated Moran’s I as a measure of spatial autocorrelation. We acknowledge that local habitat factors may influence mortality at particular towers, but because only 18.4% of towers were originally selected for monitoring on the basis of knowledge of prior mortality (see below), it is unlikely that these variations would result in a systematic bias in the resulting mortality estimates.

To illustrate the contribution of each part of our adjustment to the final estimate of mortality, we calculated the extrapolated mortality estimates for the unadjusted mortality data, with the sampling correction only, with the search efficiency and scavenging corrections only, and corrected for all factors.

We do not report estimates of bird mortality at short (<60 m) towers in this paper because they contribute negligibly to overall annual bird mortality and are not usually illuminated unless located near an airport. We note, however, that single-night mortality events with several hundred identified dead birds at unlit <60 m towers have been reported, often related to lighting at adjacent infrastructure [30], which is consistent with reports from turbines and towers monitored at industrial wind facilities [64]. Our analysis therefore applies to towers ≥60 m.

Results

Tower Height–mortality Regression

Towers used in the height–mortality regression were located throughout the eastern United States (Figure 2). We were able to confirm from original sources and personal communications that 68.4% of the towers were chosen for study with no prior knowledge of avian mortality; status is unknown for 13.2% of towers; and only 18.4% of towers were chosen with any knowledge of prior avian mortality. Log-transformed annual avian mortality, when adjusted for sampling effort, search efficiency, and scavenging, was significantly explained by log-transformed tower height in a linear regression (R2 = 0.84, F1,36 = 191.62, p<0.0001) (Table 5; Figure 3).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Bird Conservation Regions and locations of towers used for tower height–mortality regression.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Regression and 95% confidence intervals of annual avian fatalities by tower height.

Annual avian fatalities were adjusted for sampling effort, search efficiency, and scavenging and regressed by log-transformed tower height (Ln(Mean Annual Fatalities +1) = 3.4684 · Ln(Tower Height) – 12.86, R2 = 0.84, p<0.0001).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.g003

thumbnail
Table 5. Regression results for mean annual fatalities by tower height, when unadjusted, corrected for sampling only, corrected for search efficiency and scavenging only, and corrected for both sampling and search efficiency/scavenging, with estimated annual fatalities after back transformation, adjustment for bias, and application to all towers in the United States and Canada.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.t005

Tower Height–mortality Regression Sensitivity to Study Inclusion

The median R2 values of the resampled distributions are similar to those obtained from using all of the available studies (Figure 4, Table 6) and are not sensitive to the addition or elimination of a few or a set of studies. The results of the resampling procedure for subsets of 18 studies (a little under half of the studies) and for 37 studies (1 fewer than the total) show the range of influence that study inclusion could have on the regression line (Table 6).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Influence of study choice on tower height–mortality regression.

Distribution of counts for R2 (adjusted), standard error, and coefficient for 5,000 iterations (subset = 18 studies, left; subset = 37 studies, right) for a linear regression between the natural logarithms of tower height (m) and mean annual fatalities.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.g004

thumbnail
Table 6. Confidence intervals and median values for model parameters using randomized subsets of 18 or 37 studies (5,000 iterations).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.t006

Evaluation of Model Adjustment Factors

Models using either sampling correction alone or the combination of sampling correction with the combined search efficiency and scavenging correction were found to be superior to the model using tower height alone at explaining annual kills (R2 = 0.84 vs. R2 = 0.79; Table 5). Correcting for search efficiency and scavenging losses appeared to provide the best improvement to the overall model (Table 5).

Tower Characteristics

Our database of ≥60 m towers included 70,414 towers in the continental United States and Canada after all quality assurance and quality control was done (Figure 5). Most towers in the United States dataset (31,486; 50.3%) were freestanding with steady-burning lights at night, while the fewest towers (4,898; 7.8%) were guyed with strobe lights at night. Some towers had strobe lights during the day but red flashing and red solid lights at night so these were included as having solid lights.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Map of communication towers in the United States and Canada by height class.

Data acquired from Federal Communications Commission, Towermaps.com, and NAV CANADA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.g005

Total Mortality and Estimates by Bird Conservation Region

Combination of the tower height–mortality regression with estimates of reduced mortality at towers without guy wires or steady-burning lights produced a matrix of mortality by height class and tower characteristics. These estimates, already adjusted for sampling effort, search efficiency, and scavenging, ranged from zero for short unguyed towers to over 20,000 birds per year for the tallest guyed towers with steady-burning lights.

The back-transformed tower height–mortality regression, adjusted for bias (smearing estimator) and applied to towers in the continental United States and Canada, produced an annual mortality estimate of 6.8 million birds per year (Table 5). Extrapolation from the unadjusted data yielded an estimate of 1.4 million birds per year, meaning that our cumulative assumption is that searchers find only around 20% of the birds that are killed, because of search efficiency, scavenging, and incomplete sampling (Table 5).

These results are sensitive to the assumptions that were made about these factors. As an illustration, we calculated total mortality while assuming a constant search efficiency equal to the average of the measured search efficiency from those towers where this was measured (36.4%), which resulted in a total mortality estimate of 9.4 million birds per year. Applying the average scavenging rate (15.8%) to all towers resulted in a mortality estimate of 4.7 million birds per year. Using both averages (for scavenging and search efficiency) yielded an estimate of 6.4 million birds per year. For the sampling effort adjustments, recalculated mortality estimates for the three scenarios applied to studies with unknown sampling schemes were: 5.4 million birds per year for sampling only on big kill days, 5.7 million birds per year for sampling on bad weather days and weekly year round, and 6.2 million birds per year for sampling on bad weather days and weekly during migration only. Finally, if we recalculate mortality after omitting all towers selected with prior knowledge of any mortality on site (18.4% of our sample of towers), the estimate of total mortality declines to 5.5 million birds per year.

Over two-thirds of the estimated mortality is attributed to towers ≥300 m, of which only 1,040 were found in our database (1.6% of towers ≥60 m; Table 7). Fully 71% of mortality is attributed to the tallest 1.9% of towers. Shorter towers (60–150 m) contribute approximately 17% of all mortality because of their sheer numbers (Table 7).

thumbnail
Table 7. Number of communication towers ≥60 m by type and associated avian mortality estimates for Canada and the continental United States.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.t007

Our estimates of mortality vary by region, influenced both by the size of the region and the number and height distribution of towers (Figure 6; Table 8). The number of towers in each BCR does not directly correlate with estimated annual mortality because of differing numbers and heights of towers. As a result, Peninsular Florida is associated with more mortality than all of Canada; even though fewer towers are reported in Peninsular Florida, they are on average much taller. The concentration of migrants resulting from Florida’s geographic position would increase mortality even more, but this factor is not considered in our method because mortality rates for any given tower height are assumed to be constant across the continent. The Southeastern Coastal Plain BCR accounts for greater mortality than other BCRs, followed by Eastern Tallgrass Prairie, Oaks and Prairies, and Piedmont (Table 8). Canadian mortality accounts for only a fraction of the total (approximately 3.2%), because Canada has far fewer, and generally shorter, towers.

thumbnail
Figure 6. Estimated annual avian mortality from communication towers by Bird Conservation Region.

High mortality estimates in Peninsular Florida and Southeastern Coastal Plain reflect the more numerous and taller communication towers in these regions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.g006

thumbnail
Table 8. Total estimated annual avian mortality at towers ≥60 m in the United States and Canada by Bird Conservation Region (BCR).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.t008

Although we extended mortality estimates to all towers in Canada and the continental United States, few studies are available from the West (Figure 2). This may be a function of a higher number of nocturnal migrants in the East, different patterns of migration, different weather patterns, or it may simply reflect the fewer and shorter towers in the West as a whole. We investigated the effect of location on annual mortality by regressing the residuals of our height regression against longitude and also by testing the residuals for spatial autocorrelation. The resulting plot showed slightly higher mortality in the East, but the relationship was not significant and was largely driven by a single data point in Colorado. Residuals were not spatially autocorrelated using inverse Euclidean distance weighting (Figure 7; Moran’s I = 0.09, z = 0.23, p = 0.816). More comprehensive surveys of towers in the West are needed to see if the lower mortality at the site in Colorado represents an anomaly or a different pattern of mortality in the West. Pending such further analysis, extrapolation of mortality at towers in the western portions of the United States and Canada should be regarded as provisional.

thumbnail
Figure 7. Distribution of residuals of tower height–mortality regression for 38 towers in the United States as adjusted for sampling effort, search efficiency, and scavenging.

Contour lines indicate regions above and below the regression line. Although exhibiting a geographically variable pattern, the residuals are not significantly spatially autocorrelated.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034025.g007

Discussion

Our total mortality estimate of 6.8 million birds per year is ∼50% greater than the current USFWS estimate of 4–5 million birds per year [14], [15], [23], [24]. Our results do not support the suggestion that mortality might be an order of magnitude higher [14], [15], which had been made before this type of synthetic analysis had been attempted. Our approach to estimating total avian mortality at towers uses far more data than previous efforts. For example, Banks’s [13] estimate was based on mortality rates from only three tower studies and assumed that all towers caused the same rate of mortality, regardless of tower height. Our method incorporates evidence from 38 towers to establish the relationship between tower height and avian mortality. We accounted for the height distribution and physical characteristics of ∼84,000 towers across the United States and Canada (including towers <60 m, which we mapped but did not include in our mortality estimates). Notwithstanding the sources of uncertainty in our estimate, the method improves previous efforts, is transparent, and can be revised in conjunction with additional field studies.

Although mortality at some towers has apparently declined over time [31], the influence of any such trend (if a true decline in mortality and not the result of increased scavenging) is offset by the large portion (>50%) of towers in the regression having survey end dates after 1990. If only these studies ending after 1990 are used in the regression, the total mortality estimate decreases to 4.8 million birds per year. The residuals of the tower height–mortality regression, however, are not significantly explained by the ending year of the survey (results not shown) so we did not exclude the older studies from our final regression. Even if the decline in number of birds killed at towers is a real phenomenon, the effect of these kills on sensitive species could still be substantial if populations have declined by a greater proportion.

Estimated tower mortality increases exponentially with tower height [26], which makes our results sensitive to the use of the height classes. For example, if we used the top of each height class rather than the middle to calculate total mortality, the estimate would increase by 25%. The use of the height classifications was necessary for ease of calculation and because attributes of the Canadian towers that were not known had to be assigned probabilistically. We used log transformations of both variables to normalize the distributions and because the total volume of airspace occupied by guy wires increases far more rapidly than does height. The increasing length of guy wires provides a mechanistic explanation for the exponentially increasing probability of avian collisions as tower height increases. Extremely tall towers also extend into the “normal” flight altitudes of many migrants so that mortality events can occur under clear skies and favorable migration conditions; this provides another plausible mechanism for the exponential increase in mortality rates observed by height. We also considered using separate regressions for towers less than and greater than 200 m, given the break in the data, but found that doing so had little effect on the overall estimate and we could not formulate a functional explanation why the tower height–mortality relationship should change in this manner.

Further research is needed on the mortality rates at the tallest towers (i.e., >500 m). These data are needed to confirm that the tower height–mortality relationship is exponential [26]. The nature of this relationship is important because it leads directly to a policy recommendation of focusing on the tallest towers first for mitigation. If more extensive tower datasets show a different relationship (e.g., logistic) then mitigation actions would be much different, requiring treatment of many more towers to address the same proportion of mortality.

Producing this estimate of avian mortality at towers required many assumptions, the implications of which we have explored to the degree possible with the data available. By undertaking this exercise, we have reaffirmed what elements should be included in tower studies going forward – explicit measurement of search efficiency, scavenging rates, and the effect of sampling schemes for any study, as well as investigation of geographic variation in mortality and inclusion of towers representative of the extremes of the height distribution. Such research will help refine our regionalized mortality estimates.

In 1989, the Exxon Valdez oil spill killed approximately 250,000 birds in what has become the benchmark for a major environmental disaster [65]. Our estimates show that communication towers are responsible for bird deaths equivalent to more than 27 Exxon Valdez disasters each year. Our estimate of the number of birds killed annually by communication towers is 2–4 times greater than the estimate for annual fatalities from lead poisoning before lead shot was phased out for hunting waterfowl [66]. Previous efforts (e.g., [25]) and our compiled database illustrate that most of the birds killed at communication towers are Neotropical migrants, which have suffered population declines and many of which are formally recognized as “Birds of Conservation Concern” [67], [68]. Data on per species mortality would provide even more clarity about the biological significance of avian mortality at communication towers. In a companion manuscript, we estimate species-specific losses based on total losses estimated here and species-specific casualty reports for Bird Conservation Regions following methods we developed previously [35]. But even without such estimates, the aggregate mortality numbers developed here should lead policymakers to pursue mitigation measures to reduce this source of chronic mortality.

Mitigation of avian mortality at communication towers could most practicably be achieved by implementing several measures: 1) concomitant with permission from aviation authorities, remove steady-burning red lights from towers, leaving only flashing (not slow pulsing) red, red strobe, or white strobe lights [24], [26], [28], [31]; 2) avoid floodlights and other light sources at the bases of towers, especially those left on all night [64]; 3) avoid guy wires where practicable [26], [28]; 4) minimize the number of new towers by encouraging collocation of equipment owned by competing companies; and 5) limit height of new towers when possible. Concentrating on removing steady-burning lights from the roughly 4,500 towers ≥150 m tall in the United States and Canada with such lights should be a top priority because, according to our model, it would reduce overall mortality by approximately 45% through remedial action at only 6% of lighted towers.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the outstanding contribution of Herbert L. Stoddard and Tall Timbers Research Station in executing the long-term study of avian mortality at the WCTV tower. We thank Charles Francis for his help in extracting weather records used in Figure 1 and Martin Raillard for his support. We thank several anonymous reviewers for constructive criticisms and Gerald Winegrad, Caroline Kennedy, and Eugene A. Young for productive discussions about this research and access to unpublished reports.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: TL CR PM DGB BM SAG. Performed the experiments: TL PM DGB BM LMS EM. Analyzed the data: TL PM DGB BM LMS EM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: TL PM DGB BM LMS EM ERT DD. Wrote the paper: TL CR PM DGB BM SAG MLA RLC AMM ERT.

References

  1. 1. Aronoff A (1949) The September migration tragedy. Linnaean News-Letter 3: 1–2.
  2. 2. Johnston DW (1955) Mass bird mortality in Georgia, October, 1954. Oriole 20: 17–26.
  3. 3. Laskey AR (1956) Television towers and nocturnal bird migration. Migrant 27: 66–67.
  4. 4. Cochran WW, Graber RR (1958) Attraction of nocturnal migrants by lights on a television tower. Wilson Bulletin 70: 378–380.
  5. 5. Brewer R, Ellis JA (1958) An analysis of migrating birds killed at a television tower in east-central Illinois, September 1955–May 1957. Auk 75: 400–414.
  6. 6. Tordoff HB, Mengel RF (1956) Studies of birds killed in nocturnal migration. University of Kansas Publications, Museum of Natural History 10: 1–44.
  7. 7. Avery ML, Springer PF, Dailey NS (1980) Avian mortality at man-made structures: an annotated bibliography (revised). U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Biological Services Program. FWS/OBS-80/ 54: 152.
  8. 8. Harvie-Brown JA, Cordeaux J (1880) Report on the migration of birds in the autumn of 1879. Zoologist 4: 161–204.
  9. 9. Gastman EA (1886) Birds killed by electric light towers at Decatur, Ill. American Naturalist 20: 981.
  10. 10. Overing R (1936) The 1935 fall migration at the Washington Monument. Wilson Bulletin 48: 222–224.
  11. 11. Spofford WR (1949) Mortality of birds at the ceilometer of the Nashville airport. Wilson Bulletin 61: 86–90.
  12. 12. Mayfield H (1967) Shed few tears. Audubon Magazine 69: 61–65.
  13. 13. Banks RC (1979) Human related mortality of birds in the United States. In: Washington SpecialScientificReport–WildlifeNo215, editor. D.C.: United States Department of the Interior, Fish and Wildlife Service. 16 p.
  14. 14. Manville AM (2001) Avian mortality at communication towers: steps to alleviate a growing problem. In: Levitt BB, editor. Cell towers: wireless convenience. or environmental hazard?: proceedings of the cell towers forum state of science/state of law Litchfield, Connecticut. pp. 75–86.
  15. 15. Manville AM (2005) Bird strikes and electrocutions at power lines, communication towers, and wind turbines: state of the art and state of the science – next steps toward mitigation. In: Ralph CJ, Rich TD, editors. Bird conservation implementation and integration in the Americas: proceedings of the third international Partners in Flight conference; 2002 March 20–24; Asilomar, California; Volume 2; Gen Tech Rep PSW-GTR-191. Albany, California: Pacific Southwest Research Station, Forest Service, U.S. Department of Agriculture. pp. 1051–1064.
  16. 16. Orbach DN, Fenton B (2010) Vision impairs the abilities of bats to avoid colliding with stationary obstacles. PLoS ONE 5: e13912.
  17. 17. Van Gelder RG (1956) Echo-location failure in migratory bats. Transactions of the Kansas Academy of Science 59: 220–222.
  18. 18. Crawford RL, Baker WW (1981) Bats killed at a north Florida television tower: a 25-year record. Journal of Mammalogy 62: 651–652.
  19. 19. Taylor WK, Anderson BH (1973) Nocturnal migrants killed at a central Florida TV tower; autumns 1969–1971. Wilson Bulletin 85: 42–51.
  20. 20. Stoddard HL Sr, Norris RA (1967) Bird casualties at a Leon County, Florida TV tower: an eleven-year study. Bulletin of Tall Timbers Research Station 8: 1–104.
  21. 21. Avery ML, Springer PF, Cassel JF (1978) The composition and seasonal variation of bird losses at a tall tower in southeastern North Dakota. American Birds 32: 1114–1121.
  22. 22. Avery ML (1979) Review of avian mortality due to collisions with manmade structures. Proceedings of the Bird Control Seminar 8: 3–11.
  23. 23. Manville AM (2001) The ABCs of avoiding bird collisions at communication towers: next steps. In: Carlton RG, editor. Proceedings of workshop on avian interactions with utility and communication structures, December 2–3, 1999. Palo Alto, California: Electric Power Research Institute. pp. 85–103.
  24. 24. Manville AM (2009) Towers, turbines, power lines, and buildings – steps being taken by the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service to avoid or minimize take of migratory birds at these structures. In: Rich TD, Arizmendi C, Demarest DW, Thompson C, editors. Tundra to tropics: connecting birds, habitats and people; proceedings of the fourth international Partners in Flight conference, 13–16 February, 2008. McAllen, Texas: Partners in Flight. pp. 262–272.
  25. 25. Shire GG, Brown K, Winegrad G (2000) Communication towers: a deadly hazard to birds. In: Washington , editor. D.C.: American Bird Conservancy. 23 p.
  26. 26. Longcore T, Rich C, Gauthreaux SA (2008) Height, guy wires, and steady-burning lights increase hazard of communication towers to nocturnal migrants: a review and meta-analysis. Auk 125: 485–492.
  27. 27. Karlsson J (1977) Fågelkollisioner med master och andra byggnadsverk [Bird collisions with towers and other man-made constructions]. Anser 16: 203–216.
  28. 28. Gehring J, Kerlinger P, Manville AM (2009) Communication towers, lights, and birds: successful methods of reducing the frequency of avian collisions. Ecological Applications 19: 505–514.
  29. 29. Seets JW, Bohlen HD (1977) Comparative mortality of birds at television towers in central Illinois. Wilson Bulletin 89: 422–433.
  30. 30. Manville AM (2007) Comments of the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service submitted electronically to the FCC on 47 CFR Parts 1 and 17, WT Docket No. D.C.: U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. 32 p.
  31. 31. Gauthreaux SA, Belser CG (2006) Effects of artificial night lighting on migrating birds. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Washington, D.C.: Island Press. pp. 67–93.
  32. 32. Bocetti CI (2011) Cruise ships as a source of avian mortality during fall migration. Wilson Journal of Ornithology 123: 176–178.
  33. 33. Nielsen LA, Wilson KR (2006) Clear Channel of northern Colorado Slab Canyon KQLF-FM broadcasting tower avian monitoring project 2002–2004. Fort Collins, Colorado: EDM International, Inc. and Colorado State University. 38 p.
  34. 34. Travis E (2009) Impacts of communication towers on avian migrants [M.S. Thesis]. Madison, Wisconsin: University of Wisconsin. 94 p.
  35. 35. Longcore T, Rich C, Gauthreaux SA (2005) Scientific basis to establish policy regulating communications towers to protect migratory birds: response to Avatar Environmental, LLC, report regarding migratory bird collisions with communications towers, WT Docket No. Federal Communications Commission Notice of Inquiry. Los Angeles, California: Land Protection Partners. 33 p.
  36. 36. Stoddard HL (1962) Bird casualties at a Leon County, Florida TV tower, 1955–1961. Bulletin of Tall Timbers Research Station 1: 1–94.
  37. 37. Crawford RL (1971) Predation on birds killed at TV tower. Oriole 36: 33–35.
  38. 38. Crawford RL (2004) The great effort: Herbert L. Stoddard and the WCTV tower study. Miscellaneous Publication Tall Timbers Research Station 14: 1–52.
  39. 39. Velie ED (1963) Report of a survey of bird casualties at television towers, ceilometers, and other obstructions. Flicker 35: 79–84.
  40. 40. Crawford RL, Engstrom RT (2001) Characteristics of avian mortality at a north Florida television tower: a 29-year study. Journal of Field Ornithology 72: 380–388.
  41. 41. Mineau P, Collins BT (1988) Avian mortality in agro-ecosystems: 2. methods of detection. In: Greaves MP, Smith BD, Greig-Smith PW, editors. Field methods for the study of environmental effects of pesticides. Croydon, U.K.: British Crop Protection Council. pp. 13–27.
  42. 42. Mineau P (2002) Estimating the probability of bird mortality from pesticide sprays on the basis of the field study record. Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry 21: 1497–1506.
  43. 43. Johnson GD, Erickson W (2001) Use of scavenger removal and searcher efficiency data to estimate total number of wildlife fatalities. Extended poster handout, SETAC 22nd North America Annual Meeting, 11–15 November, 2001, Baltimore, Maryland. 8 p.
  44. 44. Kostecke RM, Linz GM, Bleier WJ (2001) Survival of avian carcasses and photographic evidence of predators and scavengers. Journal of Field Ornithology 72: 439–447.
  45. 45. Ponce C, Alonso JC, Argandoña G, García Fernández A, Carrasco M (2010) Carcass removal by scavengers and search accuracy affect bird mortality estimates at power lines. Animal Conservation 13: 603–612.
  46. 46. Smallwood KS (2007) Estimating wind turbine-caused bird mortality. Journal of Wildlife Management 71: 2781–2791.
  47. 47. Smallwood KS, Bell DA, Snyder SA, Didonato JE (2010) Novel scavenger removal trials increase wind turbine-caused avian fatality estimates. Journal of Wildlife Management 74: 1089–1097.
  48. 48. Morrison ML (2002) Searcher bias and scavenging rates in bird/wind energy studies. NREL/SR-500–30876. Golden, Colorado: National Renewable Energy Laboratory. 5 p.
  49. 49. Nicholson CP, Tankersley RD, Fiedler JK, Nicholas NS (2005) Assessment and prediction of bird and bat mortality at wind energy facilities in the southeastern United States, Final Report, 2005. Knoxville: Tennessee Valley Authority. 57 p.
  50. 50. Avery M, Springer PF, Cassel JF (1977) Weather influences on nocturnal bird mortality at a North Dakota tower. Wilson Bulletin 89: 291–299.
  51. 51. Avery M, Clement T (1972) Bird mortality at four towers in eastern North Dakota – fall 1972. Prairie Naturalist 4: 87–95.
  52. 52. Avery ML, Springer PF, Cassel JF (1975) Progress report on bird losses at the Omega tower, southeastern North Dakota. Proceedings of the North Dakota Academy of Science 27, Part II: 40–49.
  53. 53. Crawford RL (1981) Weather, migration and autumn bird kills at a north Florida TV tower. Wilson Bulletin 93: 189–195.
  54. 54. Beauchamp JJ, Olson JS (1973) Corrections for bias in regression estimates after logarithmic transformation. Ecology 54: 1403–1407.
  55. 55. Duan H (1983) Smearing estimate: a nonparametric retransformation method. Journal of the American Statistical Association 78: 605–610.
  56. 56. Gehring J, Kerlinger P, Manville AM (2011) The role of tower height and guy wires on avian collisions with communication towers. Journal of Wildlife Management 75: 848–855.
  57. 57. Commission for Environmental Cooperation (1997) Ecological regions of North America: toward a common perspective. Montreal, Québec, Canada: Commission for Environmental Cooperation. 71 p.
  58. 58. Ginter DL, Desmond MJ (2004) Avian mortality during fall 2001 migration at communication towers along the Rio Grande corridor in southern New Mexico. Southwestern Naturalist 49: 414–417.
  59. 59. Dickerman RW, Winker K, Gibson DD (1998) Sooty Tern reaches the Aleutian Islands, Alaska. Western Birds 29: 122–123.
  60. 60. Munro JA (1924) A preliminary report on the destruction of birds at lighthouses on the coast of British Columbia. Canadian Field-Naturalist 38: 141–145, 171–175:
  61. 61. Squires WA, Hanson HE (1918) The destruction of birds at the lighthouses on the coast of California. Condor 20: 6–10.
  62. 62. Gauthreaux SA, Belser CG (2003) Radar ornithology and biological conservation. Auk 120: 266–277.
  63. 63. Williams TC, Williams JM, Williams PG, Stokstad P (2001) Bird migration through a mountain pass studied with high resolution radar, ceilometers, and census. Auk 118: 389–403.
  64. 64. Kerlinger P, Gehring JL, Erickson WP, Curry R, Jain A, et al. (2010) Night migrant fatalities and obstruction lighting at wind turbines in North America. Wilson Journal of Ornithology 122: 744–754.
  65. 65. Piatt JF, Ford RG (1996) How many seabirds were killed by the Exxon Valdez oil spill? American Fisheries Society Symposium 18: 712–719.
  66. 66. Bellrose FC (1959) Lead poisoning as a mortality factor in waterfowl populations. Illinois Natural History Survey Bulletin 27: 235–288.
  67. 67. Robbins CS, Sauer JR, Greenberg RS, Droege S (1989) Population declines in North American birds that migrate to the neotropics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 86: 7658–7662.
  68. 68. U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service (2008) Birds of conservation concern 2008. Arlington, Virginia: Division of Migratory Bird Management. 85 p.
  69. 69. Young EA, Wiens G, Harding M (2000) Avian surveys for the wind turbine site and the Jeffrey Energy Center, Western Resources, Pottawatomie County, Kansas, October 1998–October 1999. In: Resources FinalreporttoWestern, editor. Inc. and Kansas Electric Utilities Research Program. Winfield, Kansas: Southwestern College.
  70. 70. Sawyer PJ (1961) Bird mortality at the WENH-TV tower in Deefield, New Hampshire. New Hampshire Audubon Quarterly 14: 46–49.
  71. 71. Herron J (1997) Television transmission tower kills in Lewis County, West Virginia. Redstart 64: 114–117.
  72. 72. Laskey AR (1957) Television tower casualties. Migrant 28: 54–57.
  73. 73. Laskey AR (1960) Bird migration casualties and weather conditions autumns 1958–1959–1960. Migrant 31: 61–65.
  74. 74. Laskey AR (1962) Migration data from television tower casualties at Nashville. Migrant 33: 7–8.
  75. 75. Laskey AR (1963) Casualties at WSIX TV tower in autumn, 1962. Migrant 34: 15.
  76. 76. Laskey AR (1963) Mortality of night migrants at Nashville T V towers, 1963. Migrant 34: 65–66.
  77. 77. Laskey AR (1964) Data from the Nashville T. V. tower casualties autumn 1964. Migrant 35: 95–96.
  78. 78. Laskey AR (1968) Television tower casualties at Nashville, autumn 1967. Migrant 39: 25–26.
  79. 79. Laskey AR (1969) T.V. tower casualties at Nashville in autumn 1968. Migrant 40: 25–27.
  80. 80. Laskey AR (1969) Autumn 1969 T. V. tower casualties at Nashville. Migrant 40: 79–80.
  81. 81. Laskey AR (1971) T.V. tower casualties at Nashville: spring and autumn, 1970. Migrant 42: 15–16.
  82. 82. Bierly ML (1973) 1971 fall television tower casualties in Nashville. Migrant 44: 5–6.
  83. 83. Goodpasture KA (1974) Fall 1972 television tower casualties in Nashville. Migrant 45: 29–31.
  84. 84. Goodpasture KA (1974) Fall 1973 television tower casualties in Nashville. Migrant 45: 57–59.
  85. 85. Goodpasture KA (1975) Nashville television tower casualties, 1974. Migrant 46: 49–51.
  86. 86. Goodpasture KA (1976) Nashville television tower casualties, 1975. Migrant 47: 8–10.
  87. 87. Goodpasture KA (1978) Television tower casualties, 1976. Migrant 49: 53–54.
  88. 88. Goodpasture KA (1984) Television tower casualties, Nashville, Tennessee 1976–1983. Migrant 55: 53–57.
  89. 89. Morris SR, Clark AR, Bhatti LH, Glasgow JL (2003) Television tower mortality of migrant birds in western New York and Youngstown, Ohio. Northeastern Naturalist 10: 67–76.
  90. 90. Caldwell LD, Wallace GJ (1966) Collections of migrating birds at Michigan television towers. Jack-Pine Warbler 44: 117–123.
  91. 91. Kemper CA (1996) A study of bird mortality at a west central Wisconsin TV tower from 1957–1995. Passenger Pigeon 58: 219–235.
  92. 92. Carter JH, III , Parnell JF (1976) TV tower kills in eastern North Carolina. Chat 40: 1–9.
  93. 93. Boso B (1965) Bird casualties at a southeastern Kansas TV tower. Transactions of the Kansas Academy of Science 68: 131–136.
  94. 94. Strnad FV (1975) More bird kills at KROC-TV tower, Ostrander, Minnesota. Loon 47: 16–21.
  95. 95. Baird J (1970) Mortality of fall migrants at the Boylston television tower in 1970. Chickadee 40: 17–21.
  96. 96. Baird J (1971) Mortality of birds at the Boylston television tower in September of 1971. Chickadee 41: 20–23.
  97. 97. Ganier AF (1962) Bird casualties at a Nashville T-V tower. Migrant 33: 58–60.
  98. 98. Laskey AR (1967) Spring mortality of Black-poll Warblers at a Nashville T. V. tower. Migrant 38: 43.
  99. 99. Goodpasture KA (1986) Nashville, Tennessee television tower casualties, 1984. Migrant 57: 70–72.
  100. 100. Goodpasture KA (1987) 1985 tower casualties at WSMV, Nashville, Tennessee. Migrant 58: 85–86.
  101. 101. Young EA, Robbins MB (2001) Bird mortality at the KTKA-TV tower, near Topeka, Kansas, 1998–2000 (Grant # 60181-8-P269). In: Fish ReporttotheUS, Service Wildlife, editors. Region 6 Nongame Migratory Bird Program. Arkansas City, Kansas and LawrenceKansas: Cowley County Community College and Museum of Natural History, The University of Kansas. 10 p.
  102. 102. Taylor WK, Anderson BH (1974) Nocturnal migrants killed at a central Florida TV tower, autumn 1972. Florida Field Naturalist 2: 40–43.
  103. 103. Mosman D (1975) Bird casualties at Alleman, Ia. TV tower. Iowa Bird Life 45: 88–90.