Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Hydrocarbon Contamination Decreases Mating Success in a Marine Planktonic Copepod

  • Laurent Seuront

    laurent.seuront@flinders.edu.au

    Affiliations School of Biological Sciences, Flinders University, Adelaide, Australia, Aquatic Sciences, South Australian Research and Development Institute, West Beach, Australia, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, UMR LOG 8187, Wimereux, France, Department of Physics, Center for Polymer Studies, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

Abstract

The mating behavior and the mating success of copepods rely on chemoreception to locate and track a sexual partner. However, the potential impact of the water-soluble fraction of hydrocarbons on these aspects of copepod reproduction has never been tested despite the widely acknowledged acute chemosensory abilities of copepods. I examined whether three concentrations of the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil (0.01%, 0.1% and 1%) impacts (i) the swimming behavior of both adult males and females of the widespread calanoid copepod Temora longcornis, and (ii) the ability of males to locate, track and mate with females. The three concentrations of the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil (WSF) significantly and non-significantly affect female and male swimming velocities, respectively. In contrast, both the complexity of male and female swimming paths significantly decreased with increasing WSF concentrations, hence suggesting a sex-specific sensitivity to WSF contaminated seawater. In addition, the three WSF concentrations impacted both T. longicornis mating behavior and mating success. Specifically, the ability of males to detect female pheromone trails, to accurately follow trails and to successfully track a female significantly decreased with increasing WSF concentrations. This led to a significant decrease in contact and capture rates from control to WSF contaminated seawater. These results indicate that hydrocarbon contamination of seawater decreases the ability of male copepods to detect and track a female, hence suggest an overall impact on population fitness and dynamics.

Introduction

Coastal ecosystems are increasingly threatened by a variety of anthropogenic pollutants. Specifically, one of the most dramatic contaminations of coastal waters is related to accidental crude oil spills, such as Torry Canyon (1967), Amoco Cadiz (1978), Exxon Valdez (1989), Aegean Sea (1992), North Cape (1996), Nakhodka (1997), Erika (1999), Prestige (2002) and more recently the Deepwater Horizon wellhead in the Gulf of Mexico (2010), that severely impacted coastal marine communities. However, leaks from ships, petroleum transport, refining and intentional spills are important sources of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs) in the ocean, particularly in coastal and shelf waters [1]. PAHs have become increasingly important because of their potential carcinogenicity, mutagenicity and teratogenicity to aquatic organisms and man [2]. In this context, understanding the direct and indirect links between hydrocarbon contaminants and marine species is critical, especially for minute planktonic organisms such as copepods that play a main role in the functioning of marine ecosystems and in biogeochemical cycles [3].

Oil slick episodes may induce mass mortalities in zooplankton in general and copepods in particular [4][6]. However, a more pernicious consequence of PAHs resides in the sub-lethal effects induced by their water-soluble fraction that affects copepod physiology, feeding and fecundity [7][9], hence abundance and diversity [10]. Despite the amount of work devoted to copepod chemoreception [11][14] and the evidence for copepods to modify their swimming behavior in response to naphthalene contamination [15] and to detect, avoid and escape localized patches contaminated by the soluble fraction of diesel oil [16], the potential for PAHs to disrupt copepod chemosensory abilities has yet to be investigated, although this would have considerable implication for mating success, hence population dynamics.

Finding mates is challenging in the ocean for copepods, as they typically rely on non-visual senses for detecting, identifying and locating mates in a three-dimensional environment, where concentrations of adults are as low as a few individuals per cubic meter [17]. Typically, males searches for chemical cues signalling the presence and position of the females. In some species, such as the widely distributed coastal calanoid copepod Temora longicornis, females leave a chemical trail in their wake, which males may sense and follow using chemical gradients along and across the trail to detect and locate the female [11][14]. Any disturbance of male chemosensory system may result in a reduced or loss of ability to detect females, which might lead to a decrease in mate encounter rates, hence mating rates.

Here, I evaluated whether the swimming behavior, and both the mating behavior and mating success of the calanoid copepod Temora longicornis are likely to be affected by hydrocarbon contamination of seawater. Specifically, the ability of males (i) to detect and follow the pheromone plume produced by conspecific females, and (ii) to capture them was evaluated in pure seawater and in seawater contaminated by the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil. A specific attention has been given to low concentrations of the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil (0.01%, 0.1% and 1%) that are well below the lethal concentration for T. longicornis to assess the impact of a chronic exposure to low concentrations of petroleum hydrocarbons.

Methods

Study species, sampling and acclimatization

Temora longicornis is very abundant in coastal temperate waters of the Northern Hemisphere [18]. It represents from 35 to 70% of the total population of copepods in the Southern Bight of the North Sea [19], [20] and is able to remove up to 49% of the daily primary production [21]. Its naupliar stages significantly contribute to larval fish diet [22]. T. longicornis were collected with a WP2 (200-µm mesh size) from the inshore surface waters of the Eastern English Channel (50°40′75″N, 1°31′1″E) at a temperature of 18°C and a salinity of 32 PSU. Specimens were subsequently gently diluted in 30-liter isotherm tanks using in situ seawater and transported to the laboratory where adult males and females were immediately sorted by pipette under a dissecting microscope. T. longicornis adults were then reared in 20-liter aquaria filled with filtered (Whatman GF/C glass-fibre filters, porosity 0.45 µm) seawater to which was added a suspension of the diatom Skeletonema coastatum at a concentration of 108 cells l−1 and a mixture of Isochrisis galbana and Nannochloropsis oculata (3/4∶1/4) at a concentration of 107 cell l−1. The larger heterotrophic flagellate Oxyrrhis marina was present as an additional food source at a concentration of 106 cell l−1. T. longicornis were reared under constant conditions of temperature (18°C) and salinity (32 PSU) under a 12/12 light/dark cycle.

Preparation of the soluble-fraction of diesel oil and preliminary toxicity assessment

The product considered as a potential contaminant of coastal waters was commercial diesel fuel oil. The water-soluble fraction of commercial diesel oil (WSF) was prepared stirring 1.8 l of filtered in situ seawater (Whatman GF/C filters) with 0.2 l of commercial diesel fuel oil for 2 h at 100 g. The mixed solution was allowed to stand for 24 h without stirring to separate the oil layer from the oil-saturated water. WSF stock solutions were siphoned into autoclaved, acid-rinsed glass containers and diluted with uncontaminated seawater at ‘high’ (1%), ‘medium’ (0.1%) and ‘low’ (0.01%) concentrations. The water-soluble fraction of oil and their derivatives products contain a mixture of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAH), monoaromatic hydrocarbons often referred to as BTEX (benzene, toluene, ethylbenzene and xylenes), phenols and heterocyclic compounds, containing nitrogen and sulphur [23][25]. While technical limitations hampered the assessment of the precise chemical nature of the WSF stock solutions, the range of WSF concentrations has specifically been chosen to investigate the sub-lethal effects related to ‘natural’ background concentrations of pollutants [26], [27]. More specifically, among those compounds BTEX are the main class of hydrocarbons found in WSF [23], [28], and naphthalene is one of the most abundant polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons dissolved in oil contaminated waters [29], [30] and has been widely used in toxicological assays [29], [31], [32]. BTEX and naphthalene concentrations are respectively in the range 450–35000 µg l−1 and 30–26000 µg l−1 in 100% water-soluble fraction [23], [25]. The ‘high’ (1%), ‘medium’ (0.1%) and ‘low’ (0.01%) concentrations used in the present work hence correspond to concentrations in the range 4.5–350 µg l−1, 0.45–35 µg l−1 and 0.045–3.5 µg l−1 for BTEX, and 0.3–260 µg l−1, 0.03–26 µg l−1 and 0.003–2.6 µg l−1 for naphthalene. These concentrations are all well below the lethal concentrations observed for a range of copepod species [7][9], [15], and for Temora longicornis over 24-h and 48-h toxicity assays. Briefly, the acute responses (mortality and narcosis) produced by the water-soluble fraction of commercial diesel oil followed a nonlinear allosteric decay model (r2 = 0.99, P<0.01) defined as [8]:(1)where S and Smax are the survival rate (%) and the maximum survival rate (%), the concentration at which 50% of the specimens died, CWSF the experimental WSF concentration (50%, 25%, 10%, 5%, 1%, 0.1% and 0.01%) and a fitting parameter. after 24-h and 48-h exposures were 7.5% and 12.7%, respectively (Fig. 1). Note that to discriminate mortality from narcotization, after a period of 4-h in clean seawater animals were examined again to assess the degree of recovery [31]. No narcotic effects were observed and the mortality and narcosis were hence very similar, i.e. and , and and for the 24-h and 48-h toxicity assays, respectively. No significant differences were found in and between males and females (P>0.05). The behavioral experiments were conducted with the same WSF stock solutions than the above-mentioned toxicity assays for mortality and narcosis.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Survival response of adult Temora longicornis females (black circles) to a range of WSF concentrations (50%, 25%, 10%, 5%, 1%, 0.1% and 0.01%) over 24-h (A) and 48-h (B) toxicity assays.

The data have been fitted to an allosteric decay model, see Eq. (1), with r2 = 0.99 (A) and r2 = 0.98 (B). Error bars are standard deviations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026283.g001

Behavioral experiments

Contaminated and uncontaminated behavioral experiments were replicated five times; each experiment involved three adult males and three adult females. WSF stock and working solutions were prepared 24 h before the behavioral experiments took place. The control experiments and the treatments were randomized, as well as the replicates of each experimental condition. The experimental chamber was rinsed with acetone, GF/C filtered seawater and distilled water, and allowed to dry between trials to remove any chemical compound. Prior to each experiment, the six experimental individuals were transferred in a filming set-up (a 15 cm×15 cm×15 cm glass chamber) filled up with uncontaminated seawater and the corresponding treatments, and were allowed to acclimatize for 15 min [16], [33]. All experimental individuals were used only once, and no narcosis or mortality was ever observed on any of the 120 tested individuals. Three-dimensional trajectories of T. longicornis males and females were recorded at a rate of 25 frame s−1 using two orthogonally oriented and synchronized infrared digital cameras (DV Sony DCR-PC120E) facing the experimental chamber. Six arrays of 72 infrared-light-emitting diodes (LEDs), each mounted on a printed circuit board about the size of a business card (i.e. 9.3 cm long and 4.9 cm wide) connected to a 12-volt DC power supply, provided the only light source from the bottom of the chamber. The cameras overlooked the experimental chamber from the side, and the various components of the set-up were adjusted so that the copepods were adequately resolved and in focus. Each experiment lasted 60 min, after which valid video clips were selected for analysis. Valid video clips consisted of pathways in which the animals were swimming freely, at least two body lengths away from any chamber's walls or the surface of the water [16], [33]. All the experiments were conducted at 18°C and 32 PSU in the dark and at night to avoid any potential behavioral artifact related to the diel cycle of the copepods [34]. Selected video clips were captured (DVgate Plus) as MPEG movies and converted into QuickTime TM movies (QuickTime Pro), after which the , and coordinates of swimming pathways were automatically extracted and subsequently combined into a 3D picture using LabTrack software (DiMedia, Kvistgård, Denmark). The time step was always 0.04 s, and output sequences of coordinates were subsequently used to characterize the motion behavior.

Behavioral analysis

Temora longicornis swimming and mate-searching behaviors have been well described [11], [13], [16], [34][40]. Typically, females leave behind them a pheromone trail whose shape and dimensions depend on swimming behavior. Once males have detected the trail, they use chemical gradients along and across the pheromone trail to detect and locate the female. The swimming behavior of T. longicornis was hence classified as non-mating and mating behavioral sequences based on male swimming behavior. Non-mating sequences were obtained from male swimming paths occurring before the initiation of mate-searching behavior, i.e. when a male swam independently of female trails; mating sequences were obtained from male swimming paths occurring after the initiation of mate-searching behavior. Mating behavioral sequences were categorized as (i) tracking events: the male detects the pheromone trail and travels directly towards the female following the pheromone trail, but loses the trail prior to mate contact; (ii) contact events: the male crosses the pheromone trail to the point of mate contact, but fails to capture the female; and (iii) capture events: the male successfully tracked a female and captures her.

The swimming speed (mm s−1) over consecutive tracking intervals was estimated as , where f is the sampling rate of the camera (f = 25 frame s−1), and the distance (mm) between two points in a three-dimensional space. The distance (mm) was computed from the coordinates as , where and are the positions of a copepod at time and , respectively. Average swimming speed and their standard deviations were measured over the duration of each individual track.

The complexity of swimming paths during non-mating and mating sequences was assessed using fractal analysis. In contrast to standard behavioral metrics such as turning angle and net-to-gross displacement ratio (NGDR), fractal analysis and the related fractal dimension have the desirable properties to be independent of measurement scale and to be very sensitive to subtle behavioral changes that may be undetectable to other behavioral variables [16], [35], [36], [40], [41]. Fractal analysis has been applied to describe the complexity of zooplankton and ichtyoplankton swimming paths [33][36], [41][47]. The fractal dimensions of T. longicornis swimming paths were estimated using the box dimension method [41]. The box dimension method relies on the “l cover” of the object, i.e. the number of boxes of length l (or circles of radius l) required to cover the object. A more practical alternative is to superimpose a regular grid of boxes of length l on the object and count the number of ‘occupied’ boxes. This procedure is repeated using different values of l. The volume occupied by a swimming path is then estimated using a series of counting boxes spanning a range of volumes down to some small fraction of the entire volume. The number of occupied boxes increases with decreasing box size, leading to the following power-law relationship:(2)where l is the box size, N(l) is the number of boxes occupied by the swimming path, and is the box fractal dimension. The fractal dimension is estimated from the slope of the linear trend of the log-log plot of N(l) versus l; is bounded between 1 and 2 for linear swimming paths, and swimming paths so complex that they fill-in the two-dimensional space. To avoid potential biases related to both the anisotropy of the swimming paths and the initial orientation of the overlying three-dimensional grid of orthogonal boxes, for each box size l the grid was rotated in 5° increments from to in the plane and from to in the plane. The resulting distribution of fractal dimensions was averaged, and the resulting dimension used to characterize the complexity of a swimming path.

The ability of males to detect female pheromone trails was quantified by the distance between the male and the female when the male initiates a tracking behavior, and the age of the female's trail upon detection by the male. The accuracy with which males followed females trails was estimated using the male-to-female displacement ratio, , where is the length of the male tracking trajectory and the length of the female trajectory [11]; MFDR = 1 when the male's trajectory is matching exactly the female's one, characterized males taking short-cuts along females's trajectories, and characterized males swimming with more frequent turns than females. The contact and capture rates were estimated as the number of contact and capture events normalized by the number of tracking events.

Statistical analyses

Because the behavioral parameters considered in the present work were consistently non-normally distributed (P<0.01), pairwise comparisons were conducted using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney U-test [48]. Multiple comparisons between groups of measurements were conducted using the Kruskal-Wallis test (KW test hereafter [49]), and a subsequent multiple comparison procedure based on the Tukey test was used to identify distinct groups of measurements [49]. Frequencies were compared using a χ2 test and the Fisher's exact test for frequencies with respectively more and less than 5 individuals [49]. All significativity levels were set at P<0.05.

Results

Under conditions of both uncontaminated and contaminated seawater, a total of 439 swimming paths were observed for T. longicornis adult females, and 551 swimming paths for adult males, including 411 non-mating behavioral sequences and 140 mating behavioral sequences. This resulted in the analysis of 522,771 data points for adult females, and 631,136 and 73,907 data points for adult males non-mating and mating behavioral sequences, respectively (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Number of individual swimming paths (N) and related number of successive positions (n) obtained for Temora longicornis adult females and males under conditions of uncontaminated seawater (Control) and seawater contaminated at 0.01%, 0.1% and 1% with the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026283.t001

Seawater contamination and Temora longicornis swimming behavior

Typical swimming trajectories of T. longicornis adult males and females are shown in Figure 2. Females consistently exhibited a cruising behavior, i.e. their rostro-caudal body axis was aligned with the direction of motion, whether they were swimming upward, downward or horizontally. In contrast to previous observations [11], [13], none of them were observed hovering (i.e. swimming upward at low speed, often with a horizontal component, with the rostro-caudal body axis oriented upward). During non-mating behavioral sequences, males swam significantly faster than females, in both uncontaminated and contaminated conditions (P<0.01; Fig. 3). The swimming speeds of females significantly differ between experimental conditions, and significantly decreased from uncontaminated to contaminated seawater (P<0.01; Fig. 3). In contrast, no significant differences were found in the swimming speeds of males between treatments (P>0.05; Fig. 3).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Example of two-dimensional swimming paths of Temora longicornis adult females (A), and adult males (B) during non-mating behavioural sequences.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026283.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Swimming speed v (mm s−1) of Temora longicornis adult males during non-mating behavioural sequences (black bars) and adult females (white bars) in uncontaminated seawater (Control) and seawater contaminated at 0.01%, 0.1% and 1% with the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil.

Error bars are standard deviations, and the symbol ‘*’ identifies significant differences (P<0.05) inferred for each experimental condition through pairwise comparisons using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney U-test. Significant differences between treatments were conducted using the Kruskal-Wallis test, and a subsequent multiple comparison procedure based on the Tukey test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026283.g003

The fractal dimensions estimated for T. longicornis adult females and adult males during non-mating and mating behavioral sequences were significantly different for each experimental condition (P<0.05; Fig. 4). More specifically, estimated for adult females and adult males during mating sequences did not significantly differ (P>0.05), and were both significantly smaller that those observed for adult males during non-mating sequences (P<0.05; Fig. 4). Finally, the fractal dimensions estimated for female swimming paths, and male swimming paths during non-mating and mating sequences significantly differ between treatments (P<0.05; Fig. 4). Specifically, were consistently higher under conditions of WSF contaminations than in uncontaminated seawater, and significantly decreased with increasing WSF concentrations (P<0.05; Fig. 4).

thumbnail
Figure 4. The box fractal dimension

estimated from swimming paths of free swimming Temora longicornis adult females (white bars), and adult males during non-mating (grey bars) and mating behavioural (black bars) sequences in uncontaminated seawater and seawater contaminated at 0.01%, 0.1% and 1% with the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil. The dashed line corresponds to the fractal dimension expected for a Brownian motion (i.e. normal diffusion). Error bars are standard deviations. The letters ‘a’, ‘b’ and ‘c’ identify significant differences (P<0.05) inferred for each experimental condition using the Kruskal-Wallis test, and a subsequent multiple comparison procedure based on the Tukey test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026283.g004

Seawater contamination and Temora longicornis mate-searching behavior

Figure 5 shows unsuccessful and successful mate-searching behavioral sequences; mate-searching proceeds through a trail-following strategy in which the females leave a discrete pheromone trail that mate-seeking males detect and follow until contact (Fig. 5a, b) and eventually capture (Fig. 5b) of the female. Males also exhibited the spinning behavior previously reported for Temora longicornis and Temora stylifera upon detection of the pheromone trail, and continued spinning throughout the pursuit of the female [11], [13]. In uncontaminated seawater, males detected pheromone trails within 3 to 4 mm ( mm; ) of the female track-line, and followed trails up to 29.9 sec old ( sec; Fig. 6a, b). In WSF contaminated seawater, the detection distance and the age of trail at detection significantly differ from those observed in control seawater, and significantly decrease with increasing WSF concentrations (Fig. 6a, b). Offset distances also significantly decreased with WSF concentrations (Fig. 6a). Female pheromone trails were followed at offset distances ranging from 1.2 to 3.1 mm ( mm; Fig. 6a) from the female track line at an increasing velocity ranging from 8.5 to 24.1 mm s−1, which was significantly higher than the velocity observed during non-mating behavior (2.1 to 11.3 mm s−1).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Temora longicornis.

Two female swimming paths (A,B) that led to unsuccessful (C) and successful (D) male mate-searching behavioural sequences. The swimming paths of females and males are shown in black and grey, respectively. The black arrows indicate the beginning of the female swimming paths; the grey arrows indicate the beginning of male swimming paths (M1), the detection of the female pheromone trail by the male and the subsequent initiation of a mating behavioural sequence in which the male follows the female's trail (M2), a contact event where the male failed to capture the female (M3), and a contact event followed by a successful capture (M4) after which male and female sink slowly together.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026283.g005

thumbnail
Figure 6. Temora longicornis.

(A) Male distance from female track-line at detection of female trail (black bars) and during tracking (white bars). (B) Age of female's trail at detection by male. (C) Male-female contact rate (black bars) and capture rate (white bars). (D) Male-to-female displacement ratio (MFDR). Error bars are standard deviations. The symbol ‘*’ identifies significant differences (P<0.05) inferred for each experimental condition (A) through pairwise comparisons using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney U-test, and between experimental conditions (B,D) through multiple comparisons conducted using the Kruskal-Wallis test, and a subsequent multiple comparison procedure based on the Tukey test used to identify distinct groups of measurements.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026283.g006

In uncontaminated seawater males tracked pheromone trails in the incorrect direction in 4 of the 41 tracking events observed (9.8%), and all of them exhibited a ‘back-tracking’ behavior along trails in the correct direction. None of those males lost the female trails. Over the 41 tracking events, 39 (95.1%; Fig. 6c) led to a contact with a female (Fig. 5c) and 26 (63.4%; Fig. 6c) to a successful capture (Fig. 5d). During the 13 unsuccessful captures, 4 females escaped before the contact actually took place (suggesting that they hydromechanically sensed the male approach) and 9 escaped from the males after contact; it is, however, unclear if they were rejected by the males, or escaped from the male grasp.

In contrast, in contaminated seawater an increasing proportion of males followed female trails in the wrong direction (Table 2), and only 20.0 to 58.3% of them exhibited a back-tracking behavior, with this proportion significantly decreasing with increasing WSF concentrations (P<0.05; Table 2). None of the observed back-tracking behavior led to a successful contact with a female. The contact and capture rates significantly decreased with increasing WSF concentration (P<0.05; Fig. 6c). Nearly all the unsuccessful capture events resulted from females escaping before the contact with the male actually took place (Table 2). This is consistent with the increased stress levels demonstrated in Centropages hamatus adult females in response to naphthalene contamination [15], and may indicate an increase in the role played by mechanoreception when chemosensory abilities are hampered.

thumbnail
Table 2. Behavioural properties of adult males Temora longicornis during female tracking events observed in uncontaminated seawater (Control) and seawater contaminated at 0.01%, 0.1% and 1% with the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026283.t002

Finally, the accuracy with which males followed female trails was significantly smaller in uncontaminated seawater () than in WSF contaminated seawater. Specifically, significantly increased with increasing WSF concentrations (Fig. 5d).

Discussion

Seawater contamination and Temora longicornis swimming behavior

Changes in motion behavior, as a response to stress induced by exposure to organic or inorganic pollutants, have been observed in a range of aquatic invertebrates such as Artemia salina [50], Balanus amphitrite [51][53], Brachionus calyciflorus [54], [55], Daphnia magna [56][62], Hippolyte inermis [63] and Daphniopsis australis [41], but still rarely on copepods [15], [16], [34], [41], [64].

In the present work, Temora longicornis adult males and females exhibited distinct behavioral responses to WSF contaminations. Specifically, the swimming speeds of females consistently decreased from uncontaminated to contaminated seawater, while no significant differences were detected between treatments in male swimming speeds (Fig. 3). This is consistent with the behavioral responses previously observed in zooplankton following water contamination; they range from hypoactivity to hyperactivity, depending on the species, concentration and nature of the contaminant, and exposure time. For instance, Daphnia magna decreased their swimming speed after a several day exposure to cadmium [57] and a 9-h exposure to copper at 30 µg l−1 [59]. In contrast, no changes were recorded following a 24-h exposure to copper at 5 µg l−1 [59], and an increase in swimming speed occurred after a 24-h exposure to methyl-paraoxon at 0.7 µg l−1 [62]. More specifically, the differences observed here between T. longicornis males and females further suggest that their chemosensory systems may have different sensitivity, females being more acute than males to the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil.

The decrease in female swimming speed and the steady male swimming speed with increasing WSF concentration contrasts with recent observations conducted on the estuarine calanoid copepod Eurytemora affinis, which consistently increased their swimming speed immediately after exposure to 2 µg l−1 of 4-nonylphenol and nonylphenol-ethoxy-acetic-acid [65]. However, because the swimming behavior of E. affinis was recorded before and after the injection of 15 µl of test solution, the behavioral observations conducted in contaminated water are more likely to result from the exposure to a gradient than a background concentration of nonyphenols. This is consistent with T. longicornis and E. affinis females escaping at high velocities when reaching patches of WSF contaminated seawater [16], hence with distinct behavioral reactions following an exposure to a background concentration of contaminants and a gradient of contaminants. This also generalizes to seawater contaminated by anthropogenic pollutants previous work showing the ability of copepods to detect and react to physical and chemical gradients naturally occurring in the ocean [66][68].

Swimming speed has been considered as a behavioral end-point for sub-lethal toxicity bioassays [53], [69]. However, it has also been shown that conventional metrics such as swimming speed were less sensitive than fractally-derived metrics to describe zooplankton behavioral properties in general [35], [36], [40] and under various conditions of stress in particular [34], [41]. For instance, the swimming speed of the calanoid copepod Centropages hamatus is not significantly different in seawater contaminated by naphthalene for concentrations ranging from 50 to 10,000 µg l−1 than in uncontaminated seawater [15]. In contrast, the cumulative distribution functions of move duration significantly differ between each naphthalene concentration, indicating an increase in behavioral stress with naphthalene concentrations [15]. The fractal dimension of the three-dimensional trajectories and the cumulative probability distribution functions of move lengths of five species of common calanoid copepods (Acartia clausi, Centropages typicus, Paracalanus parvus, Pseudocalanus elongatus and Temora longicornis) were also more sensitive to experimental stress than swimming velocity [34]. Similar conclusions were reached from the assessment of salinity stress in Daphniopsis australis and temperature stress in Temora longicornis [41]. These results are consistent with the significant decrease in the fractal dimensions of male three-dimensional trajectories with increasing WSF concentrations (Fig. 4), indicating a decrease in the complexity of swimming patterns under conditions of WSF contamination. This is also congruent with the generally reported decrease in behavioral complexity under stressful conditions for both invertebrates [15], [34], [41] and vertebrates [70][75], and specifically for organisms contaminated by pollutants such as tetrachloroethylene [76] and lead [77].

Seawater contamination and Temora longicornis mate-searching behavior

The behavioral observations conducted here on T. longicornis in uncontaminated seawater and WSF contaminated seawater are consistent with mate-searching events previously reported for this species; see e.g. [11], [13], [37][39]. This suggests that the contamination of seawater by the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil did not modify T. longicornis overall mate-searching strategies. However, the significant decrease with increasing WSF concentrations in the male distance from female track-line at detection of female trail (Fig. 6a) and during tracking (Fig. 6b) as well as the increase in male-to-female displacement ratio with increasing WSF concentration (Fig. 6d) indicate that the chemosensory ability of male Temora longicornis to successfully detect and track females is affected by the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil for concentrations as low as 0.01%. This ultimately leads to 2.8- and 4.1-fold decreases in mate encounter and capture rates, respectively (Fig. 6c, Table 2).

The observed changes in the swimming behavior of male T. longicornis may be directly related to changes in the ability of males to detect the females pheromone trail, which in turn may be the result of two non-conflicting mechanisms: the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil directly disturbs the males' chemosensory system, and/or a chemical interaction of the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil with the pheromone molecules changes the molecule structure, hence renders it unrecognizable by the males chemosensory system. A decrease in the efficiency of male sensory system is consistent with the anomalous neurotransmission resulting from acetylcholine esterase inhibition following exposure to pollutants [78]; see also [62] and references therein.

The behavioral changes observed in male swimming patterns may also be a consequence of a decrease in the rate of pheromone production by the females. In response to stress and to the related reduction in the energy balance, organisms typically perform a series of compensatory responses to improve the probability of survival, involving the endocrine system and behavior [72], [79][81]. It is hence reasonable to think that T. longicornis females may control the probability of mating by reducing, and ultimately stopping, the production of pheromones under stressful conditions such as contamination by the soluble-fraction of diesel oil. This hypothesis is consistent with previous evidence of reduced egg production in copepods in seawater contaminated by polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons [7][9]. The changes observed in female swimming behavior (Fig. 3) are also congruent with an adaptive strategy leading to decrease mate encounter rates. The encounter rate between a male and a female swimming respectively at velocities u and v is given by [82]:(3)when . is the perceptive distance of the male, and the female density. It is straightforward from Eq. (3) that the decreasing swimming speed of females and the steady swimming speed of males under increasing WSF concentrations (Fig. 3) leads to a decrease in mate encounter rates. Similarly, a modeling approach demonstrated that the number of encounters Emf is a linear function of the three-dimensional fractal dimensions defined as , where the pre-factor depends on the motion behavior and female density as [83]. The decreasing fractal dimensions observed for both males and females under conditions of WSF water contamination (Fig. 4) hence also leads to decrease mate encounter rates.

Seawater contamination and zooplankton: ecological implications

The changes observed in the swimming behavior of both males and females T. longicornis in response to seawater contaminated by the water-soluble fraction of diesel oil have far reaching implications for copepod ecology as in some species of planktonic copepods population growth rate is heavily constrained by relatively low male mating capacity and abundance, and the related fertilisation limitation [84]. In this context, a decrease in encounter and capture rates due to hydrocarbon contamination may profoundly impact the structure and function of the pelagic ecosystems through a destabilization of copepod population dynamics. The ability of copepods to detect, avoid and escape localized patches contaminated by the soluble fraction of diesel oil [16] may limit the behavioral effect reported here for point source contaminations. However, my results suggest that even low background concentrations of hydrocarbons may have more pernicious effects on population dynamics than the previously reported sub-lethal effects on copepod physiology, feeding and fecundity [7][9]. These findings prompt critical considerations of behavioral adaptations in copepods (and potentially other invertebrates) and the incorporation of behavioral changes induced by hydrocarbon contamination into general predictive models of planktonic copepod population dynamics based on the role of individual swimming behavior [85], [86].

Acknowledgments

I thank Mathilde Schapira, Nicolas Spilmont and two anonymous reviewers for constructive comments and suggestions that improved the text.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: LS. Performed the experiments: LS. Analyzed the data: LS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LS. Wrote the paper: LS.

References

  1. 1. Doval MD, Morono A, Pazos Y, Lopez A, Madrinán M, et al. (2006) Monitoring dissolved aromatic hydrocarbons in Rias Baixas embayments (NW Spain) after Prestige oil spills: relationship with hydrography. Est Coast Shelf Sci 67: 205–218.
  2. 2. De Flora S, Bagnasco M, Zanacchi P (1991) Genotoxic, carcinogenic and teratogenic hazards in the marine environment, with special reference to the Mediterranean Sea. Mutation Res Rev Gen Toxicol 258: 285–320.
  3. 3. Roemmich D, McGowan J (1995) Climatic warming and the decline of zooplankton in the California Current. Science 267: 1324–1326.
  4. 4. Johansson S, Larsson U, Boehm P (1980) The Tsesis oil spill. Impact on the pelagic ecosystem. Mar Poll Bull 11: 284–293.
  5. 5. Gajbhiye SN, Mustafa S, Mehta P, Nair VR (1995) Assessment of biological characteristics on coastal environment of Murud (Maharashtra) during the oil spill (17 May 1993). Indian J Mar Sci 24: 196–202.
  6. 6. Guzmán del Próo SA, Chávez EA, Alatriste FM, de la Campa S, De la Cruz G, et al. (1986) The impact of the Ixtoc-1 oil spill on zooplankton. J Plankton Res 8: 557–581.
  7. 7. Cowles TJ, Remillard JF (1983) Effects of exposure to sublethal concentrations of crude oil on the copepod Centropages hamatus. 1. Feeding and egg production. Mar Biol 78: 45–51.
  8. 8. Barata C, Baird DJ, Medina M, Albalat A, Soares AMVM (2002) Determining the ecotoxicological mode of action of toxic chemicals in meiobenthic marine organisms: stage-specific short tests with Tisbe battagliai. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 230: 183–194.
  9. 9. Calbet A, Saiz E, Barata C (2007) Lethal and sublethal effects of naphathalene and 1,2-dimethylnaphthalene on the marine copepod Paracartia grani. Mar Biol 151: 195–204.
  10. 10. Varela M, Bode A, Lorenzo J, Álvarez-Ossorio MT, Miranda A, et al. (2006) The effect of the “Prestige”oil spill on the plankton of the N-NW Spanish coast. Mar Poll Bull 53: 272–286.
  11. 11. Doall MH, Colin SP, Strickler JR, Yen J (1998) Locating a mate in 3D: the case of Temora longicornis. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 353: 681–689.
  12. 12. Bagøien E, Kiørboe T (2005) Blind dating-mate finding in planktonic copepods. I. Tracking the pheromone trail of Centropages typicus. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 300: 105–115.
  13. 13. Goetze E, Kiørboe T (2008) Heterospecific mating and species recognition in the planktonic marine copepods Temora stylifera and T. longicornis. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 370: 185–198.
  14. 14. Yen J, Sehn JK, Catton K, Kramer A, Sarnelle O (2011) Pheromone trail following in three dimensions by the freshwater copepod Hesperodiaptomus shoshone. J Plankton Res 33: 907–916.
  15. 15. Seuront L, Leterme S (2007) Increased zooplankton behavioural stress in response to short-term exposure to hydrocarbon contamination. The Open Oceanography Journal 1: 1–7.
  16. 16. Seuront L (2010) Zooplankton avoidance as a response to point sources of hydrocarbon contaminated water. Mar Fresh Res 61: 263–270.
  17. 17. Boxshall GA (1998) Preface. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 353: 669–670.
  18. 18. Razouls C, de Bovée F, Kouwenberg J, Desreumaux N (2005) Diversité et répartition géographique chez les copépodes planctoniques marins. Available: http://copepods.obs-banyuls.fr. Accessed 2011 September 28.
  19. 19. Daan R (1989) Factors controlling the summer development of the copepod populations in the Southern Bight of the North Sea. Neth J Sea Res 23: 305–322.
  20. 20. Seuront L (2005) First record of the calanoid copepod Acartia omorii (Copepoda: Calanoide: Acartiidae) in the southern bight of the North Sea. J Plankton Res 27: 1301–1306.
  21. 21. Dam HG, Peterson WT (1993) Seasonal contrasts in the diel vertical distribution, feeding behaviour and grazing impact of the copepod Temora longicornis. J Mar Res 51: 561–594.
  22. 22. Ringuette M, Castonguay M, Runge JA, Grégoire F (2002) Atlantic mackerel (Scomber scombrus) recruitment fluctuations in relation to copepod production and juvenile growth. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 59: 646–656.
  23. 23. Saeed T, Al-Mutairi M (1999) Chemical composition of the water-soluble fraction of the leaded gasolines in seawater. Environ Int 25: 117–129.
  24. 24. Elordui-Zapatarietxe S, Albaigé J, Rosell-Melé A (2008) Fast preparation of the seawater accomodated fraction of heavy oil by sonication. Chemosphere 73: 1811–1816.
  25. 25. Rodrigues RV, Miranda-Filho KC, Gusmão EP, Moreira CB, Romano LA, et al. (2010) Deleterious effects of water-soluble fraction of petroleum, diesel and gasoline on marine pejerrey Odontesthes argentinensis larvae. Sci Tot Environ 408: 2054–2059.
  26. 26. Ohwada K, Nishimura M, Wada M, Nomura H, Shibata A, et al. (2003) Study of the effect of water-soluble fractions of heavy-oil on coastal marine organisms using enclosed ecosystems, mesocosms. Mar Poll Bull 47: 78–84.
  27. 27. Hashim AA (2010) Effect of sublethal concentrations of fuel oil on the behaviour and survival of larvae and adults of the barnacle Balanus amphitrite (Darwin). Turkish J Fish Aquat Sci 10: 499–503.
  28. 28. Carls MG, Rice SD (1990) Abnormal development and growth reductions of pollock, Theragra chalcogramma, embryos exposed to water soluble fraction of oil. Fish Bull 88: 29–37.
  29. 29. Corner EDS, Harris RP, Kilvington CC, O'Hara SCM (1976) Petroleum compounds in the marine food web: short-term experiments on the fate of naphthalene in Calanus. J Mar Biol Assoc UK 56: 121–133.
  30. 30. Albaigés J, Bayona JM, Fundación Santiago Rey Fernández-Latorre AC, Spain (ed) (2003) El fuel. la huella del fuel. Ensayos sobre el Prestige. A Coruña. Spain.
  31. 31. Berdugo V, Harris RP, O'Hara SC (1977) The effect of petroleum hydrocarbons on reproduction of an estuarine planktonic copepod in laboratory cultures. Mar Poll Bull 8: 138–143.
  32. 32. Harris RP, Berdugo V, O'Hara SCM, Corner EDS (1977) Accumulation of 14C-1-Naphthalene by an oceanic and an estuarine copepod during long-term exposure to low-level concentrations. Mar Biol 42: 187–195.
  33. 33. Seuront L (2006) Effect of salinity on the swimming behaviour of the estuarine calanoid copepod Eurytemora affinis. J Plankton Res 28: 805–813.
  34. 34. Seuront L (2011) Behavioral fractality in marine copepods: endogenous rhythms versus exogenous stressors. Physica A 390: 250–256.
  35. 35. Seuront L, Hwang JS, Tseng LC, Schmitt FG, Souissi S, et al. (2004) Individual variability in the swimming behavior of the tropical copepod Oncaea venusta (Copepoda: Poecilostomatoida). Mar Ecol Prog Ser 283: 199–217.
  36. 36. Seuront L, Yamazaki H, Souissi S (2004) Hydrodynamic disturbance and zooplankton swimming behaviour. Zool Stud 43: 377–388.
  37. 37. Weissburg MJ, Doall MH, Yen J (1998) Following the invisible trail: kinematic analysis of mate-tracking in the copepod Temora longicornis. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 353: 701–712.
  38. 38. Yen J, Doall MH, Weissburg MJ (1998) The fluid physics of signal perception by mate-tracking copepods. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 353: 787–804.
  39. 39. Yen J, Prusak A, Caun M, Doall MH, Brown J, Strickler JR (2004) Signaling during mating in the pelagic copepod, Temora longicornis. In: Seuront L, Strutton PG, editors. Handbook of scaling methods in aquatic ecology: measurements, analysis, simulation. CRC Press, Boca Raton. pp. 149–159.
  40. 40. Seuront L, Vincent D (2008) Impact of a Phaeocystis globosa spring bloom on Temora longicornis feeding and swimming behaviours. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 363: 131–145.
  41. 41. Seuront L (2010) Fractals and multifractals in ecology and aquatic science. CRC Press, Boca Raton. 344 p.
  42. 42. Coughlin DJ, Strickler JR, Sanderson B (1992) Swimming and search behaviour in clownfish, Amphiprion perideraion, larvae. Anim Behav 44: 427–440.
  43. 43. Bundy MH, Gross TF, Coughlin DJ, Strickler JR (1993) Quantifying copepod searching efficiency using swimming pattern and perceptive ability. Bull Mar Sci 53: 15–28.
  44. 44. Dowling NA, Hall SJ, Mitchell JG (2000) Foraging kinematics of barramundi during early stages of development. J Fish Biol 57: 337–353.
  45. 45. Uttieri M, Zambianchi E, Strickler JR, Mazzocchi MG (2005) Fractal characterization of three-dimensional zooplankton swimming trajectories. Ecol Model 185: 51–63.
  46. 46. Uttieri M, Nihongi A, Mazzocchi MG, Strickler JR, Zambianchi E (2007) Pre-copulatory swimming behaviour of Leptodiaptomus ashlandi (Copepoda: Calanoida): a fractal approach. J Plankton Res 29: i17–i26.
  47. 47. Uttieri M, Paffenhöfer GA, Mazzocchi MG (2008) Prey capture in Clausocalanus (Copepoda: Calanoida). The role of swimming behaviour. Mar Biol 153: 925–935.
  48. 48. Siegel S, Castellan NJ (1988) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. McGraw-Hill, New York. 399 p.
  49. 49. Zar JH (2009) Biostatistical analysis. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ. 960 p.
  50. 50. Venkateswara Rao J, Kavitha P, Jakka NM, Sridhar V, Usman PK (2007) Toxicity of organophsphates on morphology and locomotor behavior in brine shrimp, Artemia salina. Arch Environ Contam Tox 53: 227–232.
  51. 51. Amsler MO, Amsler CD, Rittschoff D, Becerro MA, Mc Clintock JB (2006) The use of computer-assisted motion analysis for quantitative studies of the behaviour of barnacle (Balanus amphitrite) larvae. Mar Freshw Behav Physiol 39: 259–268.
  52. 52. Faimali M, Magillo F, Piazza V, Garaventa F, Geraci S (2002) A simple toxicological bioassay using phototactic behaviour of Balanus amphitrite (Darwin) nauplii: role of some cultural parameters and application with experimental biocides. Period Biol 104: 225–232.
  53. 53. Faimali M, Garaventa F, Piazza V, Greco G, Corra C, et al. (2006) Swimming speed alteration of larvae of Balanus amphitrite (Darwin) as a behavioural end-point toxicological bioassays. Mar Biol 149: 87–96.
  54. 54. Charoy C, Janssen CR (1999) The swimming behaviour of Brachionus calyciflorus (rotifer) under toxic stress. II. Comparative sensitivity of various behavioural criteria. Chemosphere 38: 3247–3260.
  55. 55. Charoy CP, Janssen CR, Persoone G, Clément P (1995) The swimming behaviour of Brachionus calyciflorus (rotifer) under toxic stress. I. The use of automated trajectory for determining sublethal effects of chemicals. Aquat Toxicol 32: 271–282.
  56. 56. Janssen CR, Ferrando MD, Persoone G (1994) Ecotoxicological studies with the freshwater rotifer Brachionus calcyflorus. 4. Rotifer behavior as a sensitive and rapid sublethal test criterion. Ecotoxicol Environ 28: 244–255.
  57. 57. Baillieul M, Blust R (1999) Analysis of the swimming velocity of cadmium-stressed Daphnia magna. Aquat Toxicol 44: 245–254.
  58. 58. Shimizu N, Ogino C, Kawanishi T, Hayashi Y (2002) Fractal analysis of Daphnia motion for acute toxicity bioassay. Environ Toxicol 17: 441–448.
  59. 59. Untersteiner H, Kahapka J, Kaiser H (2003) Behavioural response of the cladoceran Daphnia magna Straus to sublethal copper stress - validation by image analysis. Aquat Toxicol 65: 435–442.
  60. 60. Goto T, Hiromi J (2003) Toxiciy of 17a-ethynylestradiol and norethindrone, constituents of any oral contraceptive pill to the swimming and reproduction of cladoceran Daphnia magna, with special reference to their synergetic effect. Mar Poll Bull 47: 139–142.
  61. 61. Gerhardt A, Janssens de Bisthoven L, Soares AMV (2005) Evidence for the stepwise stress model: Gambusia holbrooki and Daphnia magna under acid mine drainage and acidified reference water stress. Environ Sci Technol 39: 4150–4158.
  62. 62. Duquesne S, Küster E (2010) Biochemical, metabolic, and behavioural responses and recovery of Daphnia magna after exposure to an organophosphate. Ecotoxicol Environ 73: 353–359.
  63. 63. Untersteiner H, Gretschel G, Puchner T, Napetschnig S, Kaiser H (2005) Monitoring behavioural responses to the heavy metal cadmium in the marine shrimp Hippolyte inermis leach (Crustacea: Decapoda) with video imaging. Zool Stud 44: 71–80.
  64. 64. Sullivan BK, Buskey E, Miller DC, Ritacco PJ (1983) Effects of copper and cadmium on growth, swimming and predator avoidance in Eurytemora affinis (Copepoda). Mar Biol 77: 299–306.
  65. 65. Cailleaud K, Michalec FG, Forget-Leray J, Budzinski H, Hwang JS, et al. (2011) Changes in the swimming behavior of Eurytemora affinis (Copepoda, Calanoida) in response to a sub-lethal exposure to nonyphenolds. Aquat Toxicol 102: 228–231.
  66. 66. Woodson CB, Webster DR, Weissburg MJ, Yen J (2005) Response of copepods to physical gradients associated with structure in the ocean. Limnol Oceanogr 50: 1552–1564.
  67. 67. Woodson CB, Weissburg MJ, Yen J (2007) Cue hierarchy and foraging in calanoid copepods: ecological implications of oceanographic structure. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 330: 163–177.
  68. 68. Woodson CB, Webster DR, Weissburg MJ, Yen J (2008) The prevalence and implications of copepod behavioral responses to oceanographic gradients and biological patchiness. Integr Comp Biol 47: 831–846.
  69. 69. Garaventa F, Gambardella C, Di Fino A, Pittore M, Faimali M (2010) Swimming speed alteration of Artemia sp. and Brachionus plicatilis as a sub-lethal behavioural end-point for ecotoxicological surveys. Ecotoxicol 19: 512–519.
  70. 70. Escós J, Alados CL, Emlen JM (1995) Fractal structures and fractal functions as disease indicators. Oikos 74: 310–314.
  71. 71. Alados CL, Huffman MA (2000) Fractal long-range correlations in behavioural sequences of wild chimpanzees: a non-invasive analytical tool for the evaluation of health. Ethology 106: 105–116.
  72. 72. Rutherford KMD, Haskell MJ, Glasbey C, Jones RB, Laurence AB (2004) Fractal analysis of animal behavior as an indicator of animal welfare. Animal Welfare 13: 99–103.
  73. 73. Rutherford KMD, Haskell MJ, Glasbey C, Jones RB, Laurence AB (2003) Detrended fluctuation analysis of behavioural responses to mild acute stressors in domestic hens. Appl Anim Behav Sci 83: 125–139.
  74. 74. María GA, Escós J, Alados CL (2004) Complexity of behavioural sequences and their relation to stress conditions in chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus): a non-invasive technique to evaluate animal welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci 86: 93–104.
  75. 75. Seuront L, Cribb N (2011) Fractal analysis reveals pernicious stress levels related to boat presence and type in the Indo–Pacific bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops aduncus. Physica A 390: 2333–2339.
  76. 76. Motohashi Y, Miyazaki Y, Takano T (1993) Assessment of behavioural effects of tetrachloroethylene using a set of time-series analyses. Neurotoxicol Teratol 15: 3–10.
  77. 77. Alados CL, Weber D (1990) Lead effects on the predictability of reproductive behaviour in fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas): a mathematical model. Environ Toxicol Chem 18: 2392–2399.
  78. 78. Pope C, Karanth S, Liu J (2005) Pharmacology and toxicology of cholinesterase inhibitors, uses and misuses of acommonmechanism of action. Environ Toxicol Pharmacol 19: 433–446.
  79. 79. Togo F, Yamamoto Y (2000) Decreased fractal component of human heart rate variability during non-REM sleep. Am J Physiol Circ Physiol 280: H17–H20.
  80. 80. Goldberger AL, Amaral LAN, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov PCh, Peng CK, et al. (2002) Fractal dynamics in physiology: alterations with desease and aging. Proc Nat Acad Sci U S A 99: 2466–2472.
  81. 81. West BJ, Scafetta N (2003) Nonlinear dynamical model of human gait. Phys Rev E 67: 051917.
  82. 82. Gerritsen J, Strickler JR (1977) Encounter probabilities and community structure in zooplankton: a mathematical model. J Fish Res Bd Can 34: 73–82.
  83. 83. Uttieri M, Cianelli D, Strickler JR, Zambianchi E (2007) On the relationship between fractal dimension and encounters in three-dimensional trajectories. J Theor Biol 247: 480–491.
  84. 84. Kiørboe T (2007) Mate finding, mating, and population dynamics in a planktonic copepod Oithona davisae: there are too few males. Limnol Oceanogr 52: 1511–1522.
  85. 85. Visser AW, Kiørboe T (2006) Plankton motility patterns and encounter rates. Oecologia 148: 538–546.
  86. 86. Kiørboe T (2008) Optimal swimming strategies in mate-searching pelagic copepods. Oecologia 155: 179–192.