Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Incongruence in Doping Related Attitudes, Beliefs and Opinions in the Context of Discordant Behavioural Data: In Which Measure Do We Trust?

  • Andrea Petróczi ,

    A.Petroczi@kingston.ac.uk

    Affiliations School of Life Sciences, Kingston University London, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, United Kingdom, Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom

  • Martina Uvacsek,

    Affiliation Faculty of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary

  • Tamás Nepusz,

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, Kingston University London, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, United Kingdom

  • Nawed Deshmukh,

    Affiliation School of Pharmacy and Chemistry, Kingston University London, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, United Kingdom

  • Iltaf Shah,

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, Kingston University London, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, United Kingdom

  • Eugene V. Aidman,

    Affiliation Land Operations Division, Defence Science and Technology Organisation, Edinburgh, Australia

  • James Barker,

    Affiliation School of Pharmacy and Chemistry, Kingston University London, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, United Kingdom

  • Miklós Tóth,

    Affiliation Faculty of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary

  • Declan P. Naughton

    Affiliation School of Life Sciences, Kingston University London, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, United Kingdom

Abstract

Background

Social psychology research on doping and outcome based evaluation of primary anti-doping prevention and intervention programmes have been dominated by self-reports. Having confidence in the validity and reliability of such data is vital.

Methodology/Principal Findings

The sample of 82 athletes from 30 sports (52.4% female, mean age: 21.48±2.86 years) was split into quasi-experimental groups based on i) self-admitted previous experience with prohibited performance enhancing drugs (PED) and ii) the presence of at least one prohibited PED in hair covering up to 6 months prior to data collection. Participants responded to questionnaires assessing a range of social cognitive determinants of doping via self-reports; and completed a modified version of the Brief Implicit Association Test (BIAT) assessing implicit attitudes to doping relative to the acceptable nutritional supplements (NS). Social projection regarding NS was used as control.

PEDs were detected in hair samples from 10 athletes (12% prevalence), none of whom admitted doping use. This group of ‘deniers’ was characterised by a dissociation between explicit (verbal declarations) and implicit (BIAT) responding, while convergence was observed in the ‘clean’ athlete group. This dissociation, if replicated, may act as a cognitive marker of the denier group, with promising applications of the combined explicit-implicit cognitive protocol as a proxy in lieu of biochemical detection methods in social science research. Overall, discrepancies in the relationship between declared doping-related opinion and implicit doping attitudes were observed between the groups, with control measures remaining unaffected. Questionnaire responses showed a pattern consistent with self-reported doping use.

Conclusions/Significance

Following our preliminary work, this study provides further evidence that both self-reports on behaviour and social cognitive measures could be affected by some form of response bias. This can question the validity of self-reports, with reliability remaining unaffected. Triangulation of various assessment methods is recommended.

Introduction

Epidemiological and social science research assessing social cognitions linked to doping behaviour has been constrained by the almost exclusive use of self-report methodology [1]. Anti-doping prevention programmes are also evaluated via self-reported changes in attitudes and willingness to use doping substances, anabolic steroids in particular [2][5]. However, recent research has drawn attention to a potential distorting effect of social desirability observed in self-reported social cognitive measures related to doping [6], [7].

In a recent project, benefitting from a multidisciplinary approach combining social and analytical sciences [8], evidence has emerged that potentially could change the landscape of social science research into doping. We have shown, albeit on a small sample, that taking self-reports at face value could lead to misleading conclusions about the social cognitive processes that underlie doping behaviour. For the first time in social science doping research, our results showed that not only the information on doping behaviour but also self-reported social cognitive measures could also be affected by some form of response bias. Whilst differences in explicit (self-reported) social cognitive measures between user and non-user groups were observed in the expected direction when groups were created from self-report, generally the reverse was evidenced when the user status was based on hair analysis results (i.e. based on the presence of at least one prohibited performance enhancing drug in hair). Implicit measures were consistent with the grouping based on hair analysis. The outcome of this project suggested that respondents may consistently manipulate their answers on all related measures in order to maintain the image they wish to project, although the possibility that this response bias might stem, at least partially, from self-deception (as opposed to strategic responding for impression management) cannot be ruled out.

The strikingly different patterns in self-reports and implicit associations in the context of behavioural data inevitably lead to the question of: Which data should we trust? The self-report methodology has endured a mix of support and criticism in the past. Whilst a plethora of literature suggests that self-report can yield a valid assessment of substance use behaviour, it has also drawn equally strong criticism. For example, the Timeline Follow-Back procedure is one of the widely used methods in clinical and research settings to accrue quantitative retrospective estimates of substance use or risky behaviour covering 7 days to 2 years prior to the interview date [9]. It has demonstrated utility in assessing alcohol, marijuana and tobacco use, as well as sexual behaviour involving taking risks of HIV infection, vis-a-vis face-to-face and over the phone interview or internet application [10]-[12] when covering a shorter period, but produces increasingly less accuracy as the timeframe increases [13]. In a similar vein, alternative questionnaire methods have been developed and reviewed for validity and reliability in identifying substance abuse. These include, but are not limited to, the Lifetime Drug Use History [14], Risk Behavior Assessment of lifetime and recent use of amphetamine [15], Drug Abuse Screening Test [16], the CAGE [17] and Cannabis Use Problems Identification Test [18].

Whilst these tests have provided adequate evidence of validity and reliability, other studies using various biomarkers to validate self-reported behaviour data have put convincing evidence forward for a considerable under-reporting of substance use [19]-[22]. A systematic review [23] revealed that this bias is not limited to socially undesirable behaviours; it also extends to simple measures such as height and weight with a tendency towards over- and under-reporting, respectively [23]. The effect of gender, race, age and contextual contingencies such as drug type and seriousness of the offence on over- and under-reporting substance use has also been investigated [24] with race as the only factor so far demonstrating an effect on admitting drug taking behaviour.

Although laboratory experiments manipulating the conditions from neutral to high demand for socially desirable responding are abundant [25], evaluating the facets of potential response bias in field research is seriously compromised by the absence of available independent objective verification [26]. Therefore, the present project aimed to address this gap by triangulating two behavioural measures and two distinct assessment procedures. Specifically, the current paper repeats the previous analysis [8] on a larger sample to verify our preliminary results and seek further evidence to ascertain whether responses given by the different user groups show a distinct pattern. The importance of this question is underscored by the fact that whilst all of these options distort the data obtained from self-reports, they lead to fundamentally different consequences for the inferences made from these data. Random untruthful answers are likely to lead to statistically non-significant results and potentially low internal consistency in measures. Consistently manipulated answers, regardless if the cause is unconscious or strategic, evidently lead to incorrect, but entirely believable conclusions about doping users vs. non-users.

To further explore issues that emerged from our previous study and were reported as preliminary findings [8], we formulated the following hypotheses.

To examine to what extent self-reported doping use is influenced by strategic responding (response bias), and to what extent the implicit assessment remains unaffected by the same bias, we hypothesise that:

  1. H1: self-reports align with self-declared use, and the doping BIAT aligns better with user grouping based on hair analysis;
  2. H2: we expect strong divergence between explicit and implicit attitude measures among deniers, and small convergence in the clean athlete group.

Based on a previous study showing domain specificity in the behaviour – social projection relationship where self-declared doping users overestimated the prevalence of doping but not social drugs and vice versa [27], we hypothesise that non-doping related variables are unaffected by the behavioural context.

  1. H3: No difference exists in self-reported non-doping variables across doping user groups (including both self-declaration and hair-analysis.

As literature precedence shows gender difference in areas prone to socially desirable responding such as dietary intake [28], [29], it was plausible that the level of honesty/dishonesty about sensitive or discriminating behaviour could be affected by gender. Although findings to date are inconclusive regarding the direction of this effect, females generally find lying less acceptable then males [30][32]. A recent study investigating dental hygiene found that admission of insufficient dental hygiene doubled for females when a less intrusive method was used, but increased only by 46% among males [33], indicating not only gender but interaction between gender- and method effect. Therefore, regarding potential gender differences, we hypothesise that

  1. H4: a gender effect exists on self-reported data for doping behaviour and social cognitive determinants of doping.

Methods

The study utilised a mixed method design to afford triangulation between explicit measures through self-reported questionnaire, implicit associations using a computerised test for latency measures, and bioanalysis via hair specimens.

Participants

We expanded the previously used small sample of 14 [8] to the full sample of 82 male and female athletes using convenience sampling. Inclusion criteria were limited to i) holding registration as an athlete in sport clubs/teams and active current participation in organised sports’ competitions and ii) the ability to provide 3 cm of a head hair sample. No specific athlete group was targeted. Competition levels were ranging from university club to international level.

Recruitment

After securing ethical approval, athletes were recruited via personal contacts to ensure good rapport and trust between the researchers and participants. Athletes received a small payment (value<10€) as compensation for their time and inconvenience. Details on recruitment, inclusion-exclusion criteria, and sample characteristics are provided in Petroczi et al. [8]. The project was approved by the Kingston University Faculty of Science Research Ethics Committee. Participation was voluntary with implied consenting procedure. The Participant Information Sheet, as well as the first page of the questionnaire clearly stated that the completion and return of the questionnaire and submission of the computerised test was taken as consent. The rationale behind omitting written consent was justified on the basis that given the sensitivity (doping use) ensuring complete anonymity was necessary. Data were linked via computer-generated alphanumerical codes. The data collection with implied consent was approved by the Faculty Research Ethics Committee.

Measures

Behaviour.

Independent categorical variables were created by dividing the sample into quasi-experimental groups on the basis of i) self-admitted previous experience with prohibited performance enhancing substances and ii) the presence of at least one prohibited performance enhancing drug in hair covering the last 6 months, depending on hair length. This led to four theoretically possible groups as presented in Table 1 along with basic demographic information. Known groups were formed based on self-reports (we refer to this as ‘self-report doping scenario’) or hair analysis (we refer to this as ‘hair analysis doping scenario’). For self-report measure of doping behaviour, athletes were asked whether they have ever used a prohibited performance enhancing substance. The combined approaches (self report and hair analysis together) yield a group where participants denied their experience with doping despite the fact that their hair analysis indicated fairly recent use. For the purpose of this study, we used three groups: self-admitted doping use (group A), self-declared clean athletes (group B) and deniers (group C).

thumbnail
Table 1. Athlete groups and demographic information based on self-reports and hair analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.t001

Self-reported doping behaviour was established from the answers given to the question: Have you ever used a banned substance?, with a dichotomous answer option. At the beginning of the questionnaire, clear and concise definitions were given for doping, social drugs and nutritional supplements with exemplars given for all three groups. The social drug category excluded caffeine, alcohol and tobacco.

Hair samples were cut at scalp with a blunt tip scissor and stored in paper envelopes at room temperature until analysis. It is generally accepted that hair analysis is not suitable for detecting single (possibly accidental) exposure [34]. Incorporation of these performance enhancing drugs requires sustained use during the period the hair sample (length) covers.

Psychological assessments.

Dependent variables included a wide range of self-reports on social cognitive determinants of doping coupled with a modified version of the Brief Implicit Association Test (BIAT) procedure [35] to doping. The doping BIAT pairs performance enhancement related stimuli in doping (nandrolone, stanozolol, testosterone, amphetamine) and nutritional supplement (vitamins, ginseng, garlic, calcium) target categories with ‘good’ attributes (peace, joy, love, smile). The opposite evaluative term (‘bad’: sick, hell, poison, fail) was non-focal [8]. Categories, attributes and their stimuli were shown to participants before the test. Upon incorrect answer, a large red X appeared for 400 ms at the bottom of the page prompting participants to correct their answers. Mean latency difference and D-score were calculated according to the scoring algorithm recommended by Greenwald et al. [36]. Latency time difference and D-score <0 indicates relatively stronger associations for nutritional supplements with good attributes; whereas latency difference time and D-score >0 suggests the opposite.

Explicit social cognitive measures were: doping attitude, using the Performance Enhancement Attitude Scale, PEAS [37]; social projection of doping and nutritional supplement (NS) use prevalence; beliefs whether doping should be allowed in competition and opinion on what proportion of athletes would be found guilty if samples taken today would be analysed in 10 years time. The PEAS is a 17-item, unidimensional measure of general doping attitude with good reliability across several studies [37]. The PEAS is scored on a 6-point Liker-type scale indicating levels of agreement with the attitude statements. Response options were anchored at each point ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (6). Example items are: “doping is an unavoidable part of competitive sport”, “health problems related to rigorous training and injuries are just as bad as from doping” and “there is no difference between drugs, fibreglass poles, and speedy swimsuits that are all used to enhance performance”. In this study, the Hungarian version of PEAS with previously established psychometric properties was used [27], [37]. Social projection was solicited by using independent single questions asking respondents to estimate the proportion of the general population using nutritional supplements and illicit drugs; as well as the proportion of athletes in their own sport using nutritional supplements and doping. Estimation was given in percentage between 0 (nobody) to 100% (everyone). These, and the other non-standard questions regarding perceived pressure, preferred competitive situation and doping opinion are provided (Table S1) along with their scoring method. Non-doping related dependent variables (social projection of NS and social drug use among members of the general population) were used as control variables.

Bioanalysis.

Approximately 100 mg untreated head hair, cut at scalp, was screened using ELISA kits for the presence of the most commonly used anabolic steroids (stanozolol, nandrolone and boldenone). Positive samples for anabolic steroids were confirmed and quantified using liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) methods [8], [38]. Erythropoietin (EPO) was detected using quantifiable ELISA. Hair digestion and analytical methods using LC-MS/MS were developed in house to increase sensitivity and to reduce the amount of hair required [38].

Data analysis

Group comparisons are first performed by factorial ANOVA to allow the testing of main and interaction effects. Owing to the unequal, and in some cases too small, group sizes, main effects were then confirmed by non-parametric (Mann-Whitney) tests. Owing to the small sample size for ‘user’ groups, inferential statistics should be read with caution. Means and standard deviations are provided for all dependent variables. Relationships were tested using the chi-square test for independent variables and correlation coefficients (Pearson's r and Kendall's tau) for dependent variables. Effect size and minimum required sample size for statistical significance were calculated using G*Power 3.1 software. Interaction terms were calculated by multiplying the z-scores for the two continuous explicit and implicit attitude measures. The BIAT effect was shown by latency difference (raw scores in ms) and D-scores [36] with negative values representing a longer latency time on the good + doping task (vs. good + nutritional supplement task). The doping BIAT task was run on a standard desktop computer (AMD AthlonTM 64X2 Dual Core Processor 4400+) under Windows XP operating system, using a bespoke test developed in-house. Response options were assigned to keyboard letters. Statistical analyses were performed using PASW Statistics v18.

Results

The mean age of the samples was 21.48±2.86 years (range 18–30). Gender distribution of the sample was close to equal (52.4% female). Athletes in the sample represented 30 sports with track & field: 14, triathlon: 7, gymnastics: 7 and soccer: 6 appearing more than five times. Competitive levels ranged from university to national level. Scale reliability (Cronbach alpha) for the PEAS in this study was .806.

Prevalence rate of the use of performance enhancing substances and nutritional supplements

Nutritional supplement use was reported by 60%, whereas those having personal experience with doping constituted 13.4% of the athletes. Admitted doping appears to be independent of self-reports on NS or social drug use. Eight hair samples were positive for stanozolol and two for EPO, giving a 12% prevalence rate for prohibited performance enhancing substance use in the sample. Detected Stanozolol levels in hair are reported in Petroczi et al., 2010 and Deshmukh et al., 2010. EPO levels were 13.35 pg/mg and 12.53 pg/mg for the two positives. Frequencies by gender are shown in Table 2. Interestingly, more males admitted having experience with doping than females (20.5% vs. 7.0%, respectively) with a reversed pattern for positive hair analysis (5.3% vs. 18.6%, respectively).

thumbnail
Table 2. Self-reported use of nutritional supplements and doping in comparison to prevalence of Stanozolol/EPO use based on detection in hair in the sample by gender (presented as frequencies).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.t002

None of the athletes who returned positive hair samples admitted doping use. Conversely, no self-admitted doping was confirmed by current hair sample tests (Table 1). Owing to the evident uncertainty (i.e. whether the mismatch between self-reports and analytical results were due to false reporting or limits in time and/or scope of the hair analysis) around this latter group (group A in Table 1), these athletes were excluded from further analyses but their results are provided in Tables S2 and S3 to inform future research.

Social cognitive factors

Social projections for NS, doping and social drug use showed a positive, statistically significant but relatively weak relationship between fellow athletes using prohibited performance enhancing drugs and NS (r = .385, p<.001) and social drugs by the general population (r = .231, p = .036).

Based on self-declared doping behavior, an interaction effect between user group and gender was only found for social projection of doping (F = 4.454, p = .038) and NS (F = 4.379, p = .040) use by fellow athletes. No gender difference was evidenced for any of the outcome variables except the pressure to use banned substances, where the t-test showed a statistically significant difference (t(57.67) =  −2.093, p = .041), but the non-parametric test failed to confirm this (p = .071). On the scale of 100% representing maximum pressure, the estimation given by males was higher (18.46%, SD = 28.40 vs. 7.72%, SD = 15.58). Gender difference or interaction effect was not detected in comparing ‘clean’ athletes and ‘deniers’ within the self-declared clear group.

A statistically significant difference was found between admitted doping users and non-users in explicit attitude (p<.001) and social projection of doping use (p = .024), with a borderline significance for pressure to use doping (p = .062). As expected, those who admitted having personal experience with doping exhibited more of a lenient attitude (shown by higher PEAS score) towards doping and gave higher estimated proportions of doping users among athletes and reported higher perceived pressure to use doping. Means, SDs and test statistics are provided in Table 3. These findings are in keeping with literature precedents.

thumbnail
Table 3. Groups by self-declared doping behaviour (means, SDs and test statistics for main effects).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.t003

Triangulating explicit and implicit measures with objective behavioural data

Adjusting the criteria for establishing user groups with hair analysis results, a new group emerged within self-declared clean athletes, namely those who denied being involved in doping practices yet the drug was present in their hair. Analysis comparing clean athletes and deniers yield, yet again, a very different picture than the one that was obtained based on self-reports. For comparison, we provide means, SDs and test statistics of the main user group effect for the same set of dependent variables in Table 4. Owing to the uncertainty around the self-admitted doping users, these athletes have been excluded. Figure 1 illustrates the effect of the behavioural information on observed differences in latency measures and D-scores derived from the implicit association task (BIAT). In panels A and B, user groups were based on self reported information on doping. As revealed by subsequent hair analysis, a proportion of athletes (shown in pink) among those who were classified as non-users based on self-declaration (shown in green) were, in fact, users. Consequently, the results of self-declared non-users are confounded by the not-insignificant cohort of athletes who used doping but denied this key information on the self-reported questionnaire.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Implicit doping associations by user groups.

‘Persons’ in the middle represents 100% of the sample with persons in green representing clean athletes (n = 61); blue depicts self-reported doping (n = 11); pink shows the proportion of athletes who denied doping use (n = 10), hence would be classified as non-user according to self-reports. Panels are: (A) latency measure on the doping BIAT task by self-reported user groups; (B) D scores of the doping BIAT task by self reported user groups; (C) latency measure on the doping BIAT task based on hair analysis; (D) D scores of the doping BIAT task by self reported user groups. Circles in panels A and C represents outliers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.g001

thumbnail
Table 4. Means, SD and test statistics in the hair analysis doping behaviour scenario.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.t004

Once a new grouping was used, it was notable that the ‘denier’ group performed quite differently on the BIAT, resulting in a smaller latency difference and diminishing IAT-effect (Figure 1C and 1D). In practical terms, those athletes who denied doping use found the good + doping pairing less difficult (shown by decreased latency). The same pattern holds for the D-scores, which are derived from latency measures, but after having taken the individual variations in cognitive ability into account. Latency measures and D-scores are detailed in Tables 3 and 4. The lack of significance is owing to the relatively large individual variations combined with the small sample size. The effect size for comparing ‘clean’ athletes and ‘deniers’ was d = 0.581 which would require at least 48 athletes in each group to reach statistical significance. The observed difference, however, is notable and it offers a valuable avenue for future research.

As the means indicate in Table 4, athletes who denied doping contrary to the evidence in their hair samples exhibited less lenient attitudes toward doping. These results support our previous observation that athletes who deny their doping use behaviour gave answers on social cognitive measures that are consistent with a typical non-user. In other word, they are ‘faking good’ in a consistent manner. These results are not surprising, considering that the outcome measures were exclusively based on self-declarations. The picture, however, has changed for the implicit associations. Albeit the differences in latency or D-scores did not reach statistical significance, the sample means suggest that performance on the BIAT was revealing for deniers. That is, latency measures and D-scores set deniers apart from clean and self-admitted users, but did not differentiate between the latter two groups. There are several plausible explanations for fact that the IAT did not differentiate between admitted doping users and self-reported clean athletes (Figures 1A and 1B). First of all, the ‘clean’ athletes' group level measure was confounded by those who denied doping, pulling the mean latency and related D score toward a more neutral position. Secondly, the self-reported doping user is a dubious group membership.

In order to find further evidence that triangulating self-reports and implicit assessments could be a rewarding approach to identify dishonest participants, we calculated and compared the interaction terms between these two measures. The interaction effect (Table 5) showed moderate divergence in the denier group and slight convergence among clean athletes. These results are in keeping with the hypothesis that discrepancies between explicit and implicit measures are greater in groups that have ‘something to hide’ by societal standards.

thumbnail
Table 5. Interaction between measured variables (mean, minimum and maximum, respectively).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.t005

Finally, we looked at information on preferred competitive situations, beliefs about the necessity of doping to win, opinion regarding the prohibition of doping for elite and all athletes and a projection of the proportion of athletes who would be found positive if samples taken today would be analysed in 10 years was also looked at in the context of behavioural data. The majority of the athletes (89%) indicated that they would prefer to compete in a doping free environment. The remaining 11% opted for a scenario in which both players use doping. The fact that no athletes opted for a unilateral use of doping suggests that there is a proportion of athletes who might be more motivated about enhancing performance in general (including using prohibited means) than gaining competitive advantage against opponents. Congruent with previous findings, 45% of those who admitted doping expressed a preference for a situation with mutual doping use, followed by 10% of those denied doping and 6% of the clean athletes. Interestingly, 62% of all respondents believed that athletes use performance enhancing substances in training and competition. As expected, athletes who denied doping use consistently expressed opinions regarding doping use, necessity and status similar to or even more rigid than those of clean athletes. Detailed analysis is shown in Table 6.

thumbnail
Table 6. Doping related opinion by user groups (% is the proportion within the respective group, rounded to the closest full number).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.t006

Discussion

The aim of this study was to expand on and re-examine our previous findings suggesting that self-report regarding doping is contaminated by a response bias whereas the implicit doping-related associations are less affected by it. A pattern similar to that described in Petroczi et al [8] was observed in the extended data set, thus confirming our preliminary findings. In terms of the extremes of the user spectrum (clean athletes and those who denied doping but with hair analysis showing the presence of a performance enhancing drug), self-reports aligned well with self-declared use, whereas the implicit association test (doping BIAT) results aligned better with user grouping based on hair analysis. We further hypothesized a strong divergence between explicit and implicit attitude measures among deniers; and small convergence in the clean athlete group. These assumptions have been verified although perhaps owing to the small sample and relatively large variance, the strength of divergence was not high.

In addition to confirming the previously found discrepancies in self-reports as a function of group identification (i.e. whether it was based on self-reports or hair analysis), we have found that whilst males dominated the self-declared doping user group, the number of females were disproportionately high among deniers, suggesting some gender effect on both self-admissions and denials. Intriguingly, gender effect was not found on any of the dependent variables, indicating that despite the observed pattern in the discrepancy between self-reported and verified behaviour, gender had no systematic effect on self-reported data for social cognitive determinants of doping. We have also found further evidence that distorted social projection (false consensus) is only present for the doping related sensitive issues, not for general population social drug use or NS. Non-doping related variables are unaffected.

The similar performance on the implicit association test (BIAT) by clean athletes and self-admitted users is somewhat unexpected. Further investigation is required to untangle this phenomenon, but one possible explanation is that the two groups hold dissimilar beliefs about and attitude toward both NS and doping substances, resulting in different latency on both pairings but similar latency difference and D score. As the implicit test required sorting words into categories where either doping or supplements were, as targets, paired with the ‘good’ attribute category difference in the IAT effect can only be expected if members of one group hold a different view about the pair of interest (e.g. good + doping) and thus perform the task with relative ease in comparison to the other group, but all athletes hold similar views and perform similarly on the comparing task (e.g. good + nutritional supplements). In a theoretical case where implicit associations differ on both targets, it is possible to derive a similar IAT effect score with very different performance on the paired tasks. Group mean latency times on each task pairing appear to support this assumption. Self-admitted doping users performed significantly slower on both task pairs than their self-declared clean counterparts (data not shown). Our implicit association tasks contrasted performance enhancing substances and nutritional supplements. Both are often used by athletes with the only difference being in their legal status (i.e. prohibited in sport competition or not). Future studies looking into contrasting deliberately distorted responses on explicit measures and implicit association among ‘deniers’ should incorporate alternative implicit association tasks independent of potential (non-prohibited) performance enhancements. However, our results suggest, with considerable confidence, that the ‘denier’ group is characterised by a pattern of dissociation between explicit and implicit responding. This dissociation is, in fact, likely to be a cognitive marker for this group, which may lead to a promising application of the combined explicit-implicit cognitive protocol used in this study as a proxy for the less readily available biochemical detection methods for large scale social science research on doping.

General population NS and social drug use estimates were used as controls (i.e. non-sensitive issue). However, whilst it served its main purpose, estimations also showed an interesting pattern, hinting that response bias observed in the socially sensitive domain of doping might be linked to the perception of doping as ‘drug’ (as oppose to functional aid). This is consistent with recent theorising that doping attitudes and behaviours may depend on how doping is represented in the athlete's mind - as a drug use (doping as an illicit behaviour) or as an ergogenic aid (doping as functional use) [39], [40]. Investigating contextual influences, Smith et al. [41] concluded that athletes' views on doping were first and foremost influenced by the ‘legality’ of the substance, then on performance. In this, athletes interviewed considered prohibited performance enhancing substances as cheating but acceptable enhancers as essential. This conclusion maps neatly to our results on nutritional supplements and doping agents but also calls for further investigation as our results on the implicit association task might have been affected by the target stimuli [42]. Following Payne and Gawronski's recommendation [43], future doping research should aim to understand the roots of performance differences in the doping-related explicit and implicit assessment protocols in the context of external validation criteria (e.g., behavioural and bio- markers).

Limitations

Limitations associated with this study include convenience sampling and partly the study design itself. With regard to the prevalence rate, available prevalence statistics are either based on adverse analytical findings or some type of self-reports, which makes direct comparison with our results impossible. Furthermore, owing to the different timeframes and the limited scope of the hair analysis, the extent of a possible overlap between the two subsets of positives is unknown, hence the two prevalence rates (13.4% and 12%) cannot be combined for a lifetime prevalence estimate. We have opened up the self-reports to any prohibited performance enhancing substances in order to obtain a reasonable sample size for self-declared doping users and although this has led to the omission of Group A from part of the analysis, it did not have an effect on the key group (deniers). Future studies may recruit strategically and restrict both the timeframe and the substance list for a complete overlap between self-reports and bioanalysis. Future investigations could also benefit from using more detailed questions and alternative indirect methods regarding doping use. Longer hair samples (if available) may be sectioned by time (e.g. 2-week or 1-month segments) to potentially identify those who may have just started to use doping as this may influence their willingness to admit this behaviour. However, this approach would also require validation studies showing the time for which the drug in question remains in the hair and drug mobility along the hair shaft in vivo.

Conclusion

The results of this study draw attention to the discrepancy in doping related explicit attitudes, beliefs and opinions in the context of behavioural data; and to the unique, but perhaps revealing patterns observed in implicit cognition. The most important contribution that our results can add to drug use research is the observed distinct patterns of explicit and implicit responding among self-declared doping users and deniers which may lead to significant advances in both detection and treatment interventions for these groups. Our findings question the validity of self-reports which may have significant implications in interpreting previous and future doping research. A combination of self-report and implicit cognitive measures seems to hold the strongest promise for future doping research. It is this combination that is likely to produce, with attendant methodology refinements, robust cognitive markers of denial. Objective verification using biomarkers or chemical analysis may not be a feasible approach in all social science research. However, our results suggest that triangulating results obtained on the same or related constructs but using different methodologies could be a cost-effective avenue.

Hence, further research into the methods of combining self-report methodology, with indirect, implicit methods is warranted. Assuming that social desirability has a root in contextual contingencies, research among different user groups could be beneficial. Doping social science research, particularly quantitative research, is seriously lacking in studies using samples drawn from athletes banned from competition owing to doping offences and longitudinal research. Research in this field would benefit from looking beyond doping and having a greater use of direct and indirect methods from social psychology, particularly those used successfully in substance use and addiction research. Incorporating implicit social cognition is one promising avenue for doping social science research. Although it is still debated whether implicit social cognitions reveals something about the individual or the individual's environment, implicit social cognition research is among the thriving areas in social psychology. Doping research, owing to the unique nature of doping (i.e. being positioned between illicit behaviour and functional use of ergogenic aids) provides an excellent testing field for developing a better understanding of the explicit and implicit social cognition and the environment.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Non-standard questions used in the athlete survey.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.s001

(DOC)

Table S2.

Means and SD for the dependent variables in the self-declared doping user group (Group A).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.s002

(DOC)

Table S3.

Doping related opinion (% is the proportion within the respective group, rounded to the closest full number) in the self-declared doping user group (Group A).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018804.s003

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the athletes who participated in this study. AP thanks Professor Paschal Sheeran for the useful comments and suggestions.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AP DPN. Performed the experiments: MU MT. Analyzed the data: AP TN. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ND IS JB. Wrote the paper: AP DPN. Developed the implicit association test: EA AP. Developed the software used in implicit assessment: TN.

References

  1. 1. Backhouse S, McKenna J, Robinson S, Atkin A (2007) Attitudes, behaviours, knowledge and education – Drugs in sport: past, present and future. Available: http://www.wada-ama.org/rtecontent/document/Backhouse_et_al_Full_Report.pdf.
  2. 2. Goldberg L, Elliot DL (2005) Preventing substance use among high school athletes. The ATLAS and ATHENA Programs. J Appl School Psychol 21(2): 63–87.
  3. 3. Elliot DL, Goldberg L, Moe EL, DeFrancesco CA, Durham MB, et al. (2008) Long-term outcomes of the ATHENA (Athletes Targeting Healthy Exercise & Nutrition Alternatives) Program for female high school athletes. J Alcohol Drug Educ 52(2): 73–92.
  4. 4. Elliot DL, Moe EL, Goldberg L, DeFrancesco CA, Durham MB, Hix-Small H (2006) Definition and outcome of a curriculum to prevent disordered eating and body-shaping drug use. J School Health 76(2): 67–73.
  5. 5. Ranby KW, Aiken LS, Mackinnon DP, Elliot DL, Moe EL, et al. (2009) A mediation analysis of the ATHENA intervention for female athletes: prevention of athletic-enhancing substance use and unhealthy weight loss behaviors. J Pediatr Psychol 34(10): 1069–1083.
  6. 6. Gucciardi DF, Jalleh G, Donovan RJ (2010) Does social desirability influence the relationship between doping attitudes and doping susceptibility in athletes? Psych Sport Exerc 11(6): 479–486.
  7. 7. Petróczi A, Nepusz T (2011) Methodological considerations regarding response bias effect in substance use research: is correlation sufficient? Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy 6: 1.
  8. 8. Petróczi A, Aidman EV, Hussain I, Deshmukh N, Nepusz T, et al. (2010) Virtue or pretense? Looking behind self-declared innocence in doping. PLoS One 5(5): e10457.
  9. 9. Sobell LC, Sobell MB (1992) Timeline Follow-back: a technique for assessing self-reported ethanol consumption. In: Allen J, Litten RZ, editors. Measuring alcohol consumption: psychosocial and biological methods. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press. pp. 41–72.
  10. 10. Sobell LC, Brown J, Leo GI, Sobell MB (1996) The reliability of the Alcohol Timeline Followback when administered by telephone and by computer. Drug Alcohol Depend 42: 49–54.
  11. 11. Carney MA, Tennen H, Affleck G, Del Boca FK, Kranzler HR (1998) Levels and patterns of alcohol consumption using Timeline Follow-Back, Daily Diaries and Real-Time “Electronic Interviews. J Studies Alcohol 59: 447–457.
  12. 12. Copersion ML, Meade CS, Bigelow GE, Brooner RK (2010) Measurement of self-reported HIV risk behaviors in injection drug users: Comparison of standard versus timeline follow-back administration procedures. J Subst Abuse Treatment 38: 60–65.
  13. 13. Hoeppner BB, Stout RL, Jackson KM, Barnett NP (2010) How good is fine-grained Timeline Follow-back data? Comparing 30-day TLFB and repeated 7-day TLFB alcohol consumption reports on the person and daily level. Addictive Behav 35: 1138–1143.
  14. 14. Day E, Best D, Cantillano V, Gaston RL, Nambamali A, Keaney F (2008) Measuring the use and career histories of drug users in treatment: reliability of the Lifetime Drug Use History (LDUH) and its data yield relative to clinical case notes. Drug Alcohol Rev 27: 171–177.
  15. 15. Napper LE, Fisher DG, Johnson ME, Wood MM (2010) The reliability and validity of drug users' self reports of amphetamine use among primarily heroin and cocaine users. Addictive Behav 35: 350–354.
  16. 16. Yudko E, Lozkhina O, Fouts A (2007) A comprehensive review of the psychometric properties of the Drug Abuse Screening Test. J Subst Abuse Treat 32: 189–198.
  17. 17. Dhalla S, Kopec JA (2007) The CAGE Questionnaire for alcohol misuse: a review of reliability and validity studies. Clin Invest Med 30(1): 33–41.
  18. 18. Bashford J, Flett R, Copeland J (2010) The Cannabis Use Problems Identification Test (CUPIT): development, reliability, concurrent and predictive validity among adolescents and adults. Addiction 105: 615–625.
  19. 19. Delaney-Black V, Chiodo LM, Hannigan JH, Greenwald MK, Janisse J, et al. (2010) Just say “I don't”: lack of concordance between teen report and biological measures of drug use. Pediatrics 126(5): 887–893.
  20. 20. Legerwood DM, Goldberger BA, Risk NK, Lewis CE, Price RK (2008) Comparison between self-report and hair analysis of illicit drug use in a community sample of middle-aged men. Addictive Behav 33: 1131–1139.
  21. 21. Musshoff F, Driever F, Lachenmeier K, Lachenmeier DW, Banger M, et al. (2006) Results of hair analyses for drugs of abuse and comparison with self-reports and urine tests Forensic Sci Int 156: 118–123.
  22. 22. Reinhard MJ, Hinkin CH, Barclay TR, Levine AJ, Marion S, et al. (2007) Discrepancies between self-report and objective measures for stimulant drug use in HIV: Cognitive, medication adherence and psychological correlate. Addictive Behav 32: 2727–2736.
  23. 23. Connor Gorber S, Tremblay M, Moher D, Gorber B (2007) A comparison of direct vs. self-report measures for assessing height, weight and body mass index: a systematic review. Obesity Rev 8: 307–326.
  24. 24. Rosay B, Skroban Najaka S, Hertz D (2007) Differences in the validity of self-reported drug use across five factors: gender, race, age, type of drug, and offense seriousness. J Quantitative Criminol 23: 41–58.
  25. 25. Uziel L (2010) Rethinking social desirability scales: from impression management to interpersonally oriented self-control. Perspect Psychol Sci 5: 243–262.
  26. 26. McCrae RR, Costa PT (1983) Social desirability scales: more substance than style. J Consulting Clin Psychol 51(6): 882–888.
  27. 27. Uvacsek M, Ránky M, Nepusz T, Naughton DP, Mazanov J, Petroczi A (2011) Self-admitted behaviour and perceived use of performance enhancing versus psychoactive drugs among competitive athletes. Scand J Sci Med Sport 21(2): 224–234.
  28. 28. Hebert JR, Ma Y, Clemow L Ockene IS, Saperia G, et al. (1997) Gender differences in social desirability and social approval bias in dietary self-report. Am J Epidemiol 146: 1046–1055.
  29. 29. Hebert JR, Hurley TG, Peterson KE, Resnicow K, Thompson FE, et al. (2008) Social desirability trait influences on self-reported dietary measures among diverse participants in a multicenter multiple risk factor trial. J Nutr 138: 226S–234S.
  30. 30. Levine TR, McCornack SA, Avery PB (1992) Sex differences in emotional reactions to discovered deception. Communication Quarterly 40: 289–296.
  31. 31. DePaulo BM, Kashy DA, Kirkendol SE, Wyer MM, Epstein JA (1996) Lying in everyday life. J Pers Soc Psychol 70: 979–995.
  32. 32. Serota KB, Levine TR, Franklin J (2010) The prevalence of lying in America: three studies of self-reported lies. Human Communication Res 36(1): 2–25.
  33. 33. Moshagen M, Musch J, Ostapczuk M, Zhao Z (2010) Reducing socially desirable responses in epidemiologic surveys: an extension of the randomized-response technique. Epidemiology 21(3): 379–382.
  34. 34. Kintz P (1998) Hair testing and doping control in sport. Toxicol Lett 102-103: 109–113.
  35. 35. Sriram N, Greenwald AG (2009) The Brief Implicit Association Test. Exp Psychol 56: 283–294.
  36. 36. Greenwald AG, Nosek BA, Banaji MR (2003) Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J Pers Soc Psychol 85: 197–216.
  37. 37. Petroczi A, Aidman EV (2009) Measuring explicit attitude as an indicator of athletes' engagement in doping: Review of the psychometric properties of the Performance Enhancement Attitude Scale. Psychol Sport Exerc 10: 390–396.
  38. 38. Deshmukh N, Hussain I, Barker J, Petroczi A, Naughton DP (2010) Analysis of anabolic steroids in human hair using LC-MS/MS. Steroids 75(10): 710–714.
  39. 39. Dunn M, Mazanov J, Sithartan G (2009) Predicting future anabolic-androgenic steroid use intentions with current substance use: Findings from an Internet-based survey. Clin J Sports Med 19: 222–227.
  40. 40. Petróczi A, Mazanov J, Naughton DPInside athletes' minds: Preliminary results from a pilot study on mental representation of doping and implications for anti-doping (in press, Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy).
  41. 41. Smith ACT, Stewart B, Oliver-Bennetts S, McDonald S, Ingerson L, et al. (2010) Contextual influences and athlete attitudes to drugs in sport. Sp Management Rev 13: 181–197.
  42. 42. Bluemke M, Friese M (2006) Do features of stimuli influence IAT effects? J Exp Soc Psychol 42: 163–176.
  43. 43. Payne BK, Gawronski B (2010) A history of implicit social cognition: where is it coming from? Where is it going? In: Gawronski B, Payne K, editors. Handbook of implicit social cognition: measurement, theory, and applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press. pp. 1–8.