Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Molecular Implications of Repeated Aggression: Th, Dat1, Snca and Bdnf Gene Expression in the VTA of Victorious Male Mice

Abstract

Background

It is generally recognized that recurrent aggression can be the result of various psychiatric disorders. The aim of our study was to analyze the mRNA levels, in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) of the midbrain, of the genes that may possibly be associated with aggression consistently shown by male mice in special experimental settings.

Methodology/Principal Findings

The genes were Th, Dat1, Snca and Bdnf; the male mice were a group of animals that had each won 20 daily encounters in succession and a group of animals that had the same winning track record followed by a no-fight period for 14 days. Increased Th, Dat1 and Snca mRNA levels were in the fresh-from-the-fight group as compared to the controls. Increased Th and Dat1 mRNA levels were in the no-fight winners as compared to the controls. Significant positive correlations were found between the level of aggression and Th and Snca mRNA levels.

Conclusions

Repeated positive fighting experience enhances the expression of the Th, Dat1 and Snca genes, which are associated with brain dopaminergic systems. The expression of the Th and Dat1 genes stays enhanced for a long time.

Introduction

It is generally recognized that recurrent aggression can be the result of various psychiatric disorders such as manic-depressive disorder, compulsive-obsessive disorder, epilepsy, posttraumatic stress, autism, Alzheimer's disease, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, mental retardation, schizophrenia, drug abuse etc [1]. According to many authors [2][6], aggression is rewarding for both laboratory rodents and humans and any positive reinforcement increases the propensity to behave aggressively. Rats and mice with the prior experience of social victories attack more frequently [6][11]. Mice with repeated positive fighting experience can develop violent behavior patterns [4], [12]. The same refers to humans: the individuals who have once displayed aggressive behavior tend to do so again [5].

It has been experimentally demonstrated that repeated aggression displayed by male mice leads the activation of brain dopaminergic systems. This activation was detected as elevated DOPAC (3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid) levels or/and increased DOPAC/DA (dopamine) ratios in the olfactory bulbs, amygdala, hippocampus, nucleus accumbens, striatum and midbrain observed in the winners as compared to the controls [13], [14]. Reportedly, the dopaminergic systems can be activated in aggressive rats, as DA levels were elevated in the prefrontal cortex during and after fights [15]. A number of papers confirms the involvement of brain dopaminergic systems in the control of aggressive behavior [16].

The aim of our study was to analyze the mRNA levels of the Th, Dat1, Snca and Bdnf genes. These genes were chosen because of the role their products (proteins) have in the brain dopaminergic regulation: tyrosine hydroxylase (TH), which is the rate-limiting enzyme of DA synthesis; the dopamine transporter (DAT), which terminates the DA action on the postsynaptic membrane by rapidly removing it from the synaptic cleft via reuptake [17][19]; alpha-synuclein (SNCA), which plays a role in dopamine compartmentalization in the pre-synaptic terminals [20][21], vesicular dopamine storage, vesicular dopamine release into synapses, and dopamine re-uptake into the dopaminergic neurons [22]; the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which is associated with many diseases [23], [24]. We focused on the ventral tegmental area (VTA) of the midbrain containing the cell bodies of mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons, because mesolimbic dopaminergic projections from the VTA play an important role in the mediation of rewarding processes and are associated with many types of social behavior [25], [26].

The mRNA levels were analyzed in male mice that had a long positive fighting history (20 wins in daily agonistic interactions) and developed behavioral psychopathology, which included the demonstration of abnormal aggression, malignancy, strong hostility, pronounced anxiety, disturbances in social recognition, hyperactivity, stereotypic and hyperkinetic reactions etc [4]. The expression of these genes was also analyzed in a group of 20-time winners who afterwards had not been allowed to fight for 14 days referred to as “the period of aggression deprivation” or “the period of deprivation” throughout; such animals are special in that they are even more aggressive after than before this no-fight period [4]. The comparison of the levels of expression of these genes in the fight-deprived and fight-undeprived winners helps answer the question as to whether the levels of gene expression in the VTA of the “deprived” winners recovers to that in the controls.

Materials and Methods

Animals

Adult male mice of the C57BL/6J strain from a stock maintained in the Animal Facility of the Institute of Cytology and Genetics, SD RAS, (Novosibirsk, Russia) were used. The animals were housed under standard conditions (12∶12 h light/dark regime, switch-on at 8.00 a.m.; food (pellets) and water available ad libitum). Mice were weaned at one month of age and housed in groups of 8–10 in plastic cages (36×23×12 cm). Experiments were performed on mice 10–12 weeks of age. All procedures were in compliance with the European Communities Council Directive of November 24, 1986 (86/609/EEC).

Winners

Aggressive behavior was induced using the sensory contact model [27]. Pairs of weight-matched animals were each placed in a steel cage (28×14×10 cm) bisected by a perforated transparent partition allowing the animals to see, hear and smell each other, but preventing physical contact. The animals were left undisturbed for two or three days to adapt to new housing conditions and sensory contact before they were exposed to encounters. In the second half of the light period, the lid was replaced by a transparent one and five minutes later the partition was removed for 10 minutes to encourage agonistic interactions. The superiority of one of the mice was firmly established within two or three encounters with the same opponent. The superior mouse would be attacking, biting and chasing another, who would be displaying only defensive behavior (sideways postures, upright postures, withdrawal, lying on the back or freezing). The duration of each fight was kept to three minutes, at which point the partition was pulled down. Each defeated mouse (loser) was exposed to the same winner for three days, while afterwards each loser was placed, once a day after the fight, in an unfamiliar cage with an unfamiliar winner behind the partition. Each victorious mouse (winner) remained in its original cage. This procedure was performed once a day for 20 days and yielded an equal number of winners and losers. The controls were animals that had been housed individually for five days. The rationale for this choice is that it gives the best trade-off between group housing and social isolation: five days is sufficient for group housing to no longer be a factor and insufficient for social isolation to become a factor [27].

The design of the current experiment is presented in Figure 1. Three groups of animals were used. (1) Fight-undeprived winners: a group of mice that had each won 20 encounters in succession. (2) Fight-deprived winners: a group of 20-time winners who were allowed to live for 14 days after the last encounter. During this period, each of them shared a cage with a loser; the partition between their compartments being down at all times, to prevent encounters. (3). Controls: the mice that had been housed individually for five days before they were killed for scientific purposes. Each experimental group contained 7–11 animals.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Protocol of the experiment.

Detailed explanations are given in the text. Behavior in “undeprived” and “deprived” winners as recorded during their respective last encounter.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004190.g001

Behavioral study

Each winner was video recorded for 10 min during its last encounter (Figure 1) and the data were documented. Furthermore, we needed to know whether both groups of winners could be considered identical at the time each mouse won its last encounter. If they were, all the differences in gene expression between fight-deprived and fight-undeprived winners, or lack thereof, could solely be accounted for by deprivation. To find out, the groups were compared in terms of behavior.

The following were the behavioral domains analyzed. 1. Attacking. Attacking, biting and chasing. 2. Aggressive grooming. The winner mounts onto the loser's back, holds it down and spends much time licking and nibbling at the loser's scruff of the neck. The loser is wholly immobilized – or sometimes stretches out the neck and again freezes under the winner. 3. Digging. Digging up and scattering the sawdust on the loser's territory (kick-digs: pulling the sawdust forwards with the forepaws; push-digs: pushing the sawdust backwards with the hind paws). 4. Self-grooming. Body care activities (fur licking, head washing, nose washing).

The following were the behaviors measured. a. Latency to attacking; b. Total time spent on any of the four above listed activities; c. Number of events falling under any of the four above listed domains. If an animal did not attack or aggressively groom during the session, the latency to these events was assigned a duration of 600 s, which is how long the session lasted, and the other measures were assigned a value of zero. The total time spent attacking, aggressively grooming and digging was counted as a measure of hostile behavior.

All the mice were decapitated simultaneously (Figure 1). An important point is that the “undeprived” winners were decapitated 24 hours after the last encounter. Brains were removed and chilled rapidly on ice. The VTA was dissected according to the Mouse Brain Atlas [28] and sections were collected from 1.68 mm to −2.12 mm relative to bregma. Obtained tissue was rapidly frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at −70°C until used.

Total RNA extraction and reverse transcription

Total RNA was extracted from each individual brain tissue sample using the Chomczynski and Sacchi method [29] with modifications. Total RNA was quantified by measuring the absorbance at 260 nm. The integrity of total RNA was assessed by agarose gel electrophoresis. 1 µg of total RNA was used for cDNA synthesis by MoMLV reverse transcriptase (Biosan, Novosibirsk, Russia).

Real-time quantitative PCR

Amplification was performed using an iQ5 iCycler (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA, USA). Th, Dat1, Bdnf, β-actin (Actb), and cyclophilin (Cphn) mRNA levels were quantified by TaqMan real-time PCR. PCR was performed in a total volume of 25 µl containing an aliquot of the RT mixture, dNTPs, the appropriate concentrations of sense and anti-sense primers, a TaqMan probe, PCR buffer, and hot-start Taq DNA polymerase (Biosan, Novosibirsk, Russia). Amplification was run for 2 min at 96°C, followed by 37 cycles of 15 s at 96°C, 45 s at 61°C. Fluorescence was monitored for 10 s after the last cycle.

Snca mRNA levels were quantified by SybrGreenI real-time PCR in a total volume of 25 µl containing an aliquot of the RT mixture, dNTPs, the appropriate concentrations of the sense and anti-sense primers, Sybr Green I (Invitrogen), PCR buffer, and hot-start Taq DNA polymerase. Amplification was run for 3 min at 95°C, followed by 40 cycles of 10 s at 92°C, 6 s at 60°C, 6 s at 72°C and 10 s at 85°C. Fluorescence was monitored for 10 s after the last cycle. To check for the presence of non-specific PCR products or primer-dimers, a melting curve analysis was performed after the final PCR cycle.

Amplification efficiencies were calculated a relative standard curve derived from fourfold serial dilutions of pooled cDNA. In all cases, the amplification efficiency was higher than 85%. Each sample was PCR-amplified twice. RT-PCR results were quantified using the relative standard curve method. The level of expression of each gene was normalized to the mean level of expression of the Actb and Cphn genes.

The oligonucleotide primers and probes were designed using Beacon Designer 5.0 (PREMIER Biosoft International, USA). The PCR primer and probe sequences are shown in Table 1.

Statistics

Statistical analysis was performed using the Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with factor groups. A post-hoc pair-wise comparison of the groups was made with the Mann-Whitney test (U test). Correlations were assessed using Spearman's rank correlation coefficient. We searched for correlations between the Th, Dat1, Bdnf, and Snca mRNA levels in each experimental group separately and in combination; each Th, Dat1, Bdnf, Snca mRNA level and each behavior (latency to first attack, the number of attacks, the total amount of time spent attacking) in the “undeprived” winners; post-deprivation Th, Dat1, Bdnf, and Snca mRNA levels and pre-deprivation behavioral parameters in the “deprived” winners. The statistical significance was set at P≤0.05; the tendency level was set at 0.05<P<0.1.

Results

No differences were found between the “undeprived” and the “deprived” group in any of the individual or social behaviors measured after the respective 20-day periods of agonistic interactions (P>0.05, Table 2). Therefore, behaviorally, both groups could be considered identical.

thumbnail
Table 2. Behavioral data from winners in the “undeprived” and the “deprived” group during their respective last encounter.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004190.t002

Kruskal-Wallis analysis revealed a significant influence of the factor groups on the mRNA level of Th [H(2,24) = 7.11, P<0.029] and Dat1 [H(2,25) = 6.45, P<0.040]. The influence of the factor groups on the mRNA level of the Snca gene was not definitely significant, but strongly suggestive [H(2,26) = 5.80, P<0.055]. There was no significant influence of the factor groups on the expression of the Bdnf gene [H(2,24) = 0.16, NS].

Based on the Mann-Whitney test (Figure 2), the “undeprived” winners had increased mRNA levels of Th (U = 10; P<0.021), Dat1 (U = 13; P<0.031) and Snca (U = 16; P<0.028) as compared to the respective levels in the controls; the “deprived” winners had increased mRNA levels of Th (U = 5; P<0.022) and Dat1 (U = 5; P<0.022) as compared to the respective levels in the controls; there was no difference between the “undeprived” and the “deprived” group in the mRNA level of Th (U = 29; NS) or Dat1 (U = 32; NS).

thumbnail
Figure 2. The normalized Th, Dat1, Snca and Bdnf mRNA levels in the VTA of the controls, “undeprived” and “deprived” winners.

* - P<0.05 vs the controls (Mann-Whitney test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004190.g002

Based on Spearman's rank correlation coefficient, there were significant positive correlations between the mRNA levels of the following genes: Th and Dat1 (R = 0.943, P<0.005), Bdnf and Snca (R = 0.893, P<0.007) in the controls; Th and Dat1 (R = 0.891, P<0.001), Dat1 and Snca (R = 0.636, P<0.026) in the “undeprived” winners; Th and Dat1 (R = 0.857, P<0.014) in the “deprived” winners; Th and Dat1 (R = 0.940, P<0.001), Dat1 and Snca (R = 0.456, P<0.022), Snca and Bdnf (R = 0.479, P<0.018) using pooled data from all the groups (Figure 3).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Significant correlations between the mRNA levels of the Th, Dat1, Snca and Bdnf genes in the VTA of the control, the “undeprived” and the “deprived” winners and all groups in combinations.

Positive correlations: * - P<0.05; ** - P<0.01; *** - P<0.001; Spearman's rank correlation coefficient.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004190.g003

Significant positive correlations were found between the Th mRNA level and the number of attacks (R = 0.607, P<0.047), the Th mRNA level and the total time spent attacking (R = 0.655, P<0.029) and the Snca mRNA level and the number of attacks (R = 0.699, P<0.017) in the “undeprived” winners; the Snca mRNA level and the total time spent attacking (R = 0.821, P<0.023), a negative correlation was found between the Snca mRNA level and the latency to first attack (R = −0.964, P<0.001) in the “deprived” winners (Figure 4). Other correlations failed to reach significance.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Significant correlations between the mRNA levels of the Th, Dat1, and Snca genes in the VTA of the “undeprived” and the “deprived” winners and the parameters of aggressive behavior during the 20st confrontation.

Solid lines – positive correlation; dotted line – negative correlations, P<0.05, Spearman's rank correlation coefficient.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004190.g004

Discussion

This experiment demonstrated an increase of the Th and Dat1 mRNA levels in the VTA of C57BL/6J mice, each of whom won 20 encounters in succession (similar results had previously been obtained from CBA/Lac mice, each of whom won 10 encounters in succession [30]). Thus, a chronic manifestation of aggression, which is accompanied by the activation of the brain dopaminergic systems [13], [14], enhances the expression of the Th and Dat1 genes, whose products are responsible for the synthesis and inactivation of DA, respectively. The increase of Snca expression, even though suggestive, may represent a feedback mechanism of DA re-uptake inhibition, which provides increased DA levels in the synaptic cleft under the influence of repeated aggression. No change in Bdnf expression was revealed in the winners. However, the expression of some genes may increase rapidly and decrease abruptly, while that of other genes changes more gradually [31]. As Miczek and the co-workers report [32], continuous subordination stress leads to significantly decreased levels of BDNF protein in the VTA compared to control levels, whereas intermittent social defeat stress episodes result in increased BDNF protein levels. Thus, the lack of changes in Bdnf mRNA levels in the winners could be explained by transient (dynamic) changes of gene expression shown, for example, for the genes of kappa-opioid receptors [33], [34], mu-opioid receptors [35], [36], and proenkephalin [37] in some brain areas in response to exposure to the experimental settings. If this explanation is correct, we cannot completely exclude the involvement of Bdnf in the mechanisms underlying repeated aggression. This expectation is supported by the presence of a positive functional correlation between the Bdnf and Snca mRNA levels.

In the “deprived” winners, Th and Dat1 expression was still enhanced: the respective mRNA levels differed significantly from those in the control mice and did not from those in the “undeprived” winners. On the one hand, it is possible that living close to a male behind the perforated transparent partition alerts the winner and makes it more aggressive even in the no-fighting settings. Another interpretation could be that once the level of expression of these genes was enhanced due to repeated aggression, there might be molecular mechanisms in place to keep these levels enhanced, no matter which settings. The fact that the “deprived” winners are more aggressive than the “undeprived” winners [4] is, if nothing else, amusing. It is possible that the reason for enhanced aggression is the accumulation of DA due to an enhanced level of expression of the Th gene, which codes for TH, the key enzyme in DA synthesis. The Snca mRNA level in the “deprived” winners did not differ from that in the controls.

Significant positive correlations were found between Th and Dat1 mRNA levels in the VTA within each of the experimental groups (the controls, the “undeprived” winners and the “deprived” winners), which suggests a close relationship between dopamine synthesis and inactivation, possibly as a result of overlapping of Th and Dat1 mRNA-positive dopaminergic neurons [17]. The fact itself that there are positive correlations between Th and Dat1 mRNA levels in the VTA is not surprising, because it is obvious that the products of these genes (TH and DAT proteins) are involved in dopaminergic mediation in brain. It is well known that the neurochemical regulation of neurotransmitters metabolism includes feedback mechanisms. Our data provide evidence that the Th and Dat1 are part of these mechanisms. The reason for this correlative relationship might be the common molecular mechanisms of transcriptional regulation of these genes. For example, Nurr1 increases the transcriptional activity of both Th and Dat1 promoters [38], [39]. A significant positive correlation between mRNA levels of the Snca and Bdnf genes was found in the control animals. It is possible that, in intact animals, the transcription factors that regulate the Th and Dat1 genes are other than those that regulate the Snca and Bdnf genes. A positive correlation between mRNA levels of the Dat1 and Snca genes was found in the “undeprived”, but not in the “deprived” winners. The Snca mRNA level in the “deprived” winners showed a tendency to recover to the control level.

Pooled data from all the experimental groups (the controls, the “undeprived” winners, the “deprived” winners) revealed an association of mRNA levels in the following succession: Th----Dat1---Snca----Bdnf. However, the intrinsic molecular mechanisms responsible for the functional association that exists between the experience of behaving aggressively, Th, Dat1, Snca and Bdnf expression and the implications of neurochemical events unfolding in the winners' brains have yet to be revealed.

Thus, a chronic manifestation of aggression, which leads to the activation of the brain dopaminergic systems, enhances the expression of the Th and Dat1 genes, whose proteins are responsible for the synthesis and inactivation of DA, respectively. Mesolimbic dopaminergic projections from the VTA play an important role in the mediation of rewarding processes [reviews 25], [26], [40]. It is therefore possible that the observed changes of gene expression in the winners' VTA result from experiencing positive emotions over social victories [4]. Because consistent defeat leads to the activation of the serotonergic system and negative emotions [41], the lack of significant changes in Th and Dat1 expression in the losers' VTA demonstrated previously [30] lends support to this possibility.

Interestingly, the winners' level of aggression measured as the latency to first attack, the number of attacks and the total time spent attacking is correlated with the Th and Snca mRNA levels in the VTA. It is therefore possible that the higher the level of aggression they display during encounters, the higher the level of Th and Snca gene expression in their brain.

The data obtained so far strongly support the statement that the Th, Dat1 and Snca genes in the VTA are involved in the mechanisms of repeated aggression. It is most likely that these genes have a role in rewarding processes, which can directly underlie the motivation to behave aggressively again. However, these data cannot help answer the question as to whether an increase in the expression of these genes in the VTA is specific for aggressive behavior pathology developed due to repeated aggression and demonstrated in our behavioral experiments. Other neurotransmitter systems, too, may be factors; for example, the opioidergic or serotonergic systems, which were found altered in 20-time winners [4].

There is ample experimental evidence supporting the hypothesis that there are many genes that can change their functional state due to agonistic interactions [23], [33], [42][52]. Repeated exposure to social confrontations and social stress has been shown to be able to develop pathological states (depression, anxiety, abnormal aggression) in animals [4], [23], [41]. It is gradually becoming clear that the development of psychoemotional disorders leads to changes in the transcriptional state of a set of genes, which makes it possible to track changes in gene functioning and to look for possibilities of their pharmacological correction. If this is as it seems to be, we should think of a new-generation therapy that should prevent gene expression from being affected by psychopathogenic factors.

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Vladimir Filonenko for a thorough revision and editing of the original English version of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: NNK. Performed the experiments: NNB UAB ILK MLF. Analyzed the data: NNB UAB MLF NNK. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MLF NNK. Wrote the paper: NNB NNK.

References

  1. 1. DSM-IV (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM-IV), fourth ed. Washington DC: APA.
  2. 2. Baron RA, Richardson D (1994) Human aggression. New York: Plenum Press.
  3. 3. Fish EW, De Bold JF, Miczek KA (2002) Aggressive behavior as a reinforcer in mice: activation by allopregnanolone. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 163: 459–466.
  4. 4. Kudryavtseva NN (2006) Psychopathology of repeated aggression: a neurobiological aspect. In: Morgan JP, editor. Perspectives on the Psychology of Aggression. NOVA Science Publishers, Inc. pp. 35–64.
  5. 5. Moyer KE (1987) Violence and Aggression. N.Y: Paragon House.
  6. 6. Scott JP (1971) Theoretical issues concerning the origin and causes of fighting. In: Eleftheriou BE, Scott JP, editors. The physiology of aggression and defeat. New-York: Plenum. pp. 11–42.
  7. 7. Lagerspetz KMJ (1964) Studies on the aggressive behavior of mice. Ann Acad Sci Fenn Series B 131: 1–131.
  8. 8. Goldsmith JF, Brain OF, Benton D (1976) Effects of age at differential housing and the duration of individual housing/grouping on intermale fighting behaviour and adrenocortical activity in «TO» strain mice. Aggress Behav 2: 307–323.
  9. 9. Van de Poll NE, de Jonge F, Van Oyen HG, Van Pelt (1982) Aggressive behavior in rats; effects of winning and losing on subsequent aggressive interactions. Behav Proc 7: 143–155.
  10. 10. Parmigiani S, Brain PF (1983) Effects of residence, aggressive experience and intruder familiarity on attack shown by male mice. Behav Proc 8: 45–47.
  11. 11. Andrade ML, Kamal KBH, Brain PF (1987) Effects of positive and negative fighting experience on behaviour in adult male mice. Eds. PF Brain, D.Mainardi, S.Parmigiani. “House mouse aggression. A model for understanding the evolution of social behaviour. Harwood Academic Publishers GmbH 223–232.
  12. 12. Caramaschi D, de Boer SF, de Vries H, Koolhaas JM (2008) Development of violence in mice through repeated victory along with changes in prefrontal cortex neurochemistry. Behav Brain Res 189: 263–272.
  13. 13. Kudriavtseva NN, Bakshtanovskaia IV (1991) The neurochemical control of aggression and submission. Zh Vyssh Nerv Deiat Im I P Pavlova 41: 459–466.
  14. 14. Devoino LV, Idova GV, Al'perina EL, Cheido MA (1998) The neurochemical set of the brain–an extra-immune mechanism of psychoneuroimmunomodulation. Vestn Ross Akad Med Nauk 9: 19–24.
  15. 15. Van Erp AM, Miczek KA (2000) Aggressive behavior, increased accumbal dopamine, and decreased cortical serotonin in rats. J Neurosci 20: 9320–9325.
  16. 16. Miczek KA, Faccidomo SP, Fish EW, DeBold JF (2007) Neurochemistry and molecular neurobiology of aggressive behavior. In: Lajtha A, Blaustein JD, editors. Handbook of Neurochemistry and Molecular Neurobiology: Behavioral neurochemistry, neuroendocrinology and molecular neurobiology. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. pp. 285–336.
  17. 17. Chen N, Reith ME (2000) Structure and function of the dopamine transporter. Eur J Pharmacol 405: 329–339.
  18. 18. Hoffman BJ, Hansson SR, Mezey E, Palkovits M (1998) Localization and dynamic regulation of biogenic amine transporters in the mammalian central nervous system. Front Neuroendocrinol 19: 187–231.
  19. 19. Miller GW, Gainetdinov RR, Levey AI, Caron MG (1999) Dopamine transporters and neuronal injury. Trends Pharmacol Sci 20: 424–429.
  20. 20. Abeliovich A, Schmitz Y, Farinas I, Choi-Lundberg D, Ho WH, et al. (2000) Mice lacking alpha-synuclein display functional deficits in the nigrostriatal dopamine system. Neuron 25: 239–252.
  21. 21. Yavich L, Tanila H, Vepsalainen S, Jakala P (2004) Role of alpha-synuclein in presynaptic dopamine recruitment. J Neurosci 24: 11165–11170.
  22. 22. Sidhu A, Wersinger C, Moussa CE, Vernier P (2004) The role of alpha-synuclein in both neuroprotection and neurodegeneration. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1035: 250–270.
  23. 23. Berton O, McClung CA, Dileone RJ, Krishnan V, Renthal W, et al. (2006) Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress. Science 311: 864–868.
  24. 24. Groves JO (2007) Is it time to reassess the BDNF hypothesis of depression? Mol Psychiatry 12: 1079–1088.
  25. 25. Cooper SJ (1991) Interaction between endogenous opioids and dopamine: Implications for reward and aversion. In: Willner P, Scheel-Kruger J, editors. The mesolimbic dopamine system: from motivation to action. London: John Wiley Sons Ltd. pp. 331–366.
  26. 26. Van Ree JM, Gerrits MA, Vanderschuren LJ (1999) Opioids, reward and addiction: An encounter of biology, psychology, and medicine. Pharmacol Rev 51: 341–396.
  27. 27. Kudryavtseva NN (1991) The sensory contact model for the study of aggressive and submissive behaviors in male mice. Aggress Behav 17: 285–291.
  28. 28. Rosen GD, Williams AG, Capra JA, Connolly MT, Cruz B, et al. (2000) The Mouse Brain Library @ www.mbl.org. Int Mouse Genome Conference 14: 166. www.mbl.org.
  29. 29. Chomczynski P, Sacchi N (1987) Single-step method of RNA isolation by acid guanidinium thiocyanate-phenol-chloroform extraction. Anal Biochem 162: 156–159.
  30. 30. Filipenko ML, Alekseyenko OV, Beilina AG, Kamynina TP, Kudryavtseva NN (2001) Increase of tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine transporter mRNA levels in ventral tegmental area of male mice under influence of repeated aggression experience. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 96: 77–81.
  31. 31. Nichols CD, Garcia EE, Sanders-Bush E (2003) Dynamic changes in prefrontal cortex gene expression following lysergic acid diethylamide administration. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 111: 182–188.
  32. 32. Miczek KA, Yap JJ, Covington HE III (2008) Social stress, therapeutics and drug abuse: Preclinical models of escalated and depressed intake. Pharmacol Therap 120: 2: 102–128.
  33. 33. Goloshchapov AV, Filipenko ML, Bondar NP, Kudryavtseva NN, et al. (2005) Decrease of kappa-opioid receptor mRNA level in ventral tegmental area of male mice after repeated experience of aggression. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 135: 290–292.
  34. 34. Spangler R, Ho A, Zhou Y, Maggos CE, Yuferov V, Kreek MJ (1996) Regulation of kappa opioid receptor mRNA in the rat brain by “binge” pattern cocaine administration and correlation with preprodynorphin mRNA. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 38: 71–76.
  35. 35. Azaryan AV, Clock BJ, Rosenberger JG, Cox BM (1998) Transient upregulation of mu opioid receptor mRNA levels in nucleus accumbens during chronic cocaine administration. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 76: 278–283.
  36. 36. Nikulina EM, Hammer RP Jr, Miczek KA, Kream RM (1999) Social defeat stress increases expression of mu-opioid receptor mRNA in rat ventral tegmental area. Neuroreport 10: 3015–3019.
  37. 37. Crespo JA, Manzanares J, Oliva JM, Corchero J, Palomo T, et al. (2001) Extinction of cocaine self-administration produces a differential time-related regulation of proenkephalin gene expression in rat brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 25: 185–194.
  38. 38. Sacchetti P, Mitchell TR, Granneman JG, Bannon MJ (2001) Nurr1 enhances transcription of the human dopamine transporter gene through a novel mechanism. J Neurochem 76: 1565–1572.
  39. 39. Zhang T, Jia N, Fei E, Wang P, Liao Z, et al. (2007) Nurr1 is phosphorylated by ERK2 in vitro and its phosphorylation upregulates tyrosine hydroxylase expression in SH-SY5Y cells. Neurosci Lett 423: 118–122.
  40. 40. Ikemoto S, Panksepp J (1999) The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Res Rev 31: 6–41.
  41. 41. Kudryavtseva NN, Avgustinovich DF (1998) Behavioral and physiological markers of experimental depression induced by social conflicts (DISC). Aggress Behav 24: 271–286.
  42. 42. Kudriavtseva NN, Filipenko ML, Bakshtanovskaia IV, Avgustinovich DF, Alekseenko OV, et al. (2004) Changes in the expression of monoaminergic genes under the influence of repeated experience of agonistic interactions: from behavior to gene. Russ J Genetics 40: 590–604.
  43. 43. Filipenko ML, Beylina AG, Alekseyenko OV, Timofeeva OA, Avgustinovich DF, Kudryavtseva NN (2002) Association between the brain COMT gene expression and aggressive experience in daily agonistic confrontations in male mice. In: McCarty R, Aguelera G, Sabban E, Kvetnansky R, editors. Stress: Neural, Endocrine and Molecular Studies. London & New York: Taylor & Francis. pp. 157–161.
  44. 44. Kollack-Walker S, Don C, Watson SJ, Akil H (1999) Differential expression of c-fos mRNA within neurocircuits of male hamsters exposed to acute or chronic defeat. J Neuroendocrinol 11: 547–559.
  45. 45. Martinez M, Phillips PJ, Herbert J (1998) Adaptation in patterns of c-fos expression in the brain associated with exposure to either single or repeated social stress in male rats. Eur J Neurosci 10: 20–33.
  46. 46. Matsuda S, Peng H, Yoshimura H, Wen TC, Fukuda T, et al. (1996) Persistent c-fos expression in the brains of mice with chronic social stress. Neurosci Res 26: 157–170.
  47. 47. Filipenko ML, Beilina AG, Alekseyenko OV, Dolgov VV, Kudryavtseva NN (2002) Increase in expression of brain serotonin transporter and monoamine oxidase a genes induced by repeated experience of social defeats in male mice. Biochemistry (Mosc) 67: 451–455.
  48. 48. Boer U, Alejel T, Beimesche S, Cierny I, Krause D, et al. (2007) CRE/CREB-driven up-regulation of gene expression by chronic social stress in CRE-luciferase transgenic mice: reversal by antidepressant treatment. PLoS ONE 2: e431.
  49. 49. Abumaria N, Rygula R, Hiemke C, Fuchs E, Havemann-Reinecke U, et al. (2007) Effect of chronic citalopram on serotonin-related and stress-regulated genes in the dorsal raphe nucleus of the rat. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 17: 417–429.
  50. 50. Pizarro JM, Lumley LA, Medina W, Robison CL, Chang WE, et al. (2004) Acute social defeat reduces neurotrophin expression in brain cortical and subcortical areas in mice. Brain Res 1025: 10–20.
  51. 51. Meyer U, van Kampen M, Isovich E, Flugge G, Fuchs E (2001) Chronic psychosocial stress regulates the expression of both GR and MR mRNA in the hippocampal formation of tree shrews. Hippocampus 11: 329–336.
  52. 52. Bartolomucci A, Palanza P, Parmigiani S, Pederzani T, Merlot E, et al. (2003) Chronic psychosocial stress down-regulates central cytokines mRNA. Brain Res Bull 62: 173–178.