Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Food Inequality Negatively Impacts Cardiac Health in Rabbits

Correction

26 Oct 2012: Heidary F, Vaez Mahdavi MR, Momeni F, Minaii B, Rogani M, et al. (2012) Correction: Food Inequality Negatively Impacts Cardiac Health in Rabbits. PLOS ONE 7(10): 10.1371/annotation/170470ba-f544-43dd-992e-52a06d3b41a4. https://doi.org/10.1371/annotation/170470ba-f544-43dd-992e-52a06d3b41a4 View correction

Abstract

Background

Individuals with lower socioeconomic status experience higher rates of mortality and are more likely to suffer from numerous diseases. While some studies indicate that humans who suffer from social inequality suffer generally worse health, to our knowledge no controlled experiments of this nature have been done in any species. Lipofuscin is a highly oxidized cross-linked aggregate consisting of oxidized protein and lipid clusters. This eminent terminal oxidation outcome accumulates within cells during aging process.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Thirty two rabbits were assigned into four groups randomly of eight each. The first group encountered food deprivation for eight weeks and was kept in an isolated situation. The second group was food deprived for eight weeks but encountered to other groups continuously. The third group suffered two weeks of deprivation and then received free access to food. The fourth group had free access to diet without any deprivation. All hearts were removed for histopathological evaluation. Cross-sections of hearts were examined by light microscopy for the presence of yellow-brown Lpofuscin pigment granules. Here we show that relative food deprivation can cause accumulation of Lipofuscin pigmentation. We find that cardiac Lipofuscin deposition increases the most in the inequitable condition in which food deprived individuals observe well-fed individuals.

Conclusions/Significance

Our findings demonstrate that a sense of inequality in food intake can promote aging more than food deprivation alone. These findings should be considered as a basis for further studies on the physiological mechanisms by which inequality negatively impacts health and well-being.

Introduction

The relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and health is well defined. Individuals with lower SES experience higher rates of mortality and are more likely to undertaking numerous health conditions. This so-called “social gradient” in health has been observed across different time periods and age groups using a extensive range of SES indicators, health measures, and methodologies [1], [2].

The purpose of this experiment was to study the effect of food intake inequality in accumulation of Lipofusion pigmentation in the rabbit heart. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine a specific physiological outcome of social inequitably, in this case, the interaction of food intake inequality status and cardiovascular reactivity.

One of the highlights of postmitotic aging is the intracellular accumulation of highly oxidized and cross-linked proteins, known as lipofuscin [3], [4]. Proteins within Lipofuscin are linked by intramolecular and intermolecular cross-links. Many of these cross-links are caused by nonproteineous compounds including oxidation products [5], [6]. The intracellular formation of Lipofuscin is a complex arrangement of reactions involving numerous cellular compartments and enzymes. The intracellular rate of Lipofuscin formation is negatively correlated with the life expectancy of a postmitotic cell and enhances with age [7], [8]. It is understood that, the higher the rate of intracellular Lipofuscin accumulation over the time, the shorter the potential life time of the cell [9]. There is a considerable body of evidence indicating that oxidative stress is a causal factor both in Lipofuscinogenesis as well as in aging [10].

Results

Statistical analysis by Kruskal Wallis test showed significant differences between all groups (P<0.05). Further analysis using Mann Whitney test showed significant differences between the first and the second group (P<0.01; Table 1). This is a remarkable increase in the Lipofusion pigmentation accumulation and much more significant damage in the animals encountering the inequality situation (second group) rather than deprivation alone (first group; see Figure 1 shows percentage of damage between groups). In the control animals (fourth group), histopatholoical evaluation showed normal structure and none of the histopathologic changes were recorded, which was significantly different from the experimental (inequity) animals (Figure 2 and 3).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Percentage of Lipofuscin pigmentation between groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003705.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Longitudinal section through the heart of a rabbit showing normal structure of cardiac muscle in the control group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003705.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Longitudinal section through cardiac muscle showing Lipofuscin pigmentation, interstitial oedema, infiltration mononuclear, fibrosis and fatty infiltration in the group suffered from inequality.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003705.g003

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean and standard deviation of Lipofuscin pigmentation between groups (P<0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003705.t001

Discussion

Some empirical studies have indicated that animal models, like people, respond negatively to inequity [11]. Brosnan and de Waal showed that monkeys refused to participate if they witnessed a conspecific achieve a more attractive reward for equal endeavor, an effect amplified if the partner obtained such a reward without any effort at all. These feedbacks indicate the presence of an aversion to inequality in animals, and may support an early evolutionary origin of inequity aversion [12]. It has been established that Cynomolgus Macaques monkeys, fed an atherogenic chow and exposed to an emotionally stressful social situation, develop greater coronary heart disease rather than monkeys in a stable social condition. These studies suggest that behavioral factors can promote disease progression [13], [14], [15].

An extensive body of evidence from animal models reveals that chronic psychosocial stress can lead, probably via a mechanism involving excessive sympathetic nervous system establishment, to exacerbation of coronary artery atherosclerosis as well as to transient endothelial dysfunction. Supports from animal models also revealed that psychosocial stress reliably induces hypercortisolemia and excessive adrenergic activation in premenopausal females, leading to accelerated atherosclerosis [16].

Presumably, behavioral situation would potentiate disease through associated perturbations of the body's fundamental axes of neuroendocrine response, such as the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical and sympatheticadrenomedullary systems [14], [17]. Our study expands on this knowledge by demonstrating that such stress may be caused by comparisons of one's own outcomes to those of others. This is potentially critical for the health of social species, including humans, whose outcomes may differ between individuals.

It is well understood that Lipofuscin pigmentation is one of the most important characteristics of aging [9] and is responsible for ‘brown atrophy’ of the heart in gross pathology [18]. It might be worthwhile to mention that neuronal lipofuscin accumulation has been found to be increased in animals placed under restraint stress [19], increasing the plausibility that, in this study, a psychological stressor led to the observed changes.

There have been some studies indicating that humans who suffer from social inequality suffer generally worse health conditions mainly cardiovascular diseases [20], [21], [22], but nothing specific and no controlled experiments of this nature. To our knowledge these results are the first evidence that food inequality in animal models can cause any kind of diseases. Our results demonstrate that a sense of inequality in food intake, rather than food deprivation alone, could promote aging. Given the above facts, it is tempting to speculate that psychological changes may induce these variations, but more studies on this subject are needed.

In conclusion, these findings add to the body of evidence that food deprivation and inequality in food intake are highly important in the presence of Lipofusion pigmentation. Age related changes in food deprivation and food intake inequality may contribute to susceptibility to stressful situations for animals. These findings should serve as a basis for further studies to identify the causes of these variations and the major mechanisms underlying physiological responses to the stress of inequality.

Materials and Methods

All procedures in this study were conducted in accordance with protocols approval by the Animal Care and Ethical Committee of Shahed University, Tehran, Iran. Thirty-two New Zealand rabbits (2.5 months old) obtained from Razi Institute, Hesarak, Karaj, Iran. All animals exposed to 12-hour light and dark conditions (lights on at 7 AM), housed in individual cages (6 Sq ft) and fed standard rabbit chow and tap water.

Experimental design

All animals had free access to water during study. After acclimation procedures, rabbits were assigned into four groups randomly of eight each as below:

The first group encountered food deprivation (access to 33% of normal diet) for eight weeks. This group was kept in an isolated situation. The second group was food deprived for eight weeks but encountered to other groups continuously during study (the third and fourth groups). The third group suffered two weeks of deprivation and then received free access to food for another six weeks. The fourth group had free access to diet without any deprivation. At the end of the experiment, animals were sacrificed under ether anesthesia and their hearts were removed for histopathological evaluation.

Histopathology

After euthanasia, heart were removed through a midline sections and washed once in phosphate-buffered saline to remove blood. Tissues were placed in a 10% solution of buffered formalin. Samples were coded, and all histomorphometric procedures were performed in a blind fashion. After gross examination, tissues were paraffin-embedded, sectioned at 3 micro meters, and stained with hematoxylin-eosin. To assess cardiac Lipofuscin pigmentation, cross-sections of hearts were examined by light microscopy for the presence of yellow-brown Lpofuscin pigment granules. All samples were graded double blindly from 0 to 3 histopathologically for expression of Lipofusion pigmentation by an expert histopathologist. The severity of microscopic lesions graded based on the following numerical scale: 0(absence), 1 (minimal), 2 (mild), 3 (severe).

Statistical Methods

Kruskal Wallis and Mann Whitney tests were used for statistical evaluation through SPSS software version 11. Results of histopathological staining used to characterize cardiac Lipofuscin are listed in Table 1 as the mean and standard deviation between groups.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the contribution of Dr. Mohammad Reza Jalali Nadoushan, Dr Zahra Kiasalari, Dr Mohsen Khalili, Dr MH Gheini, Dr. Somayeh Zamani and Middle East Cancer Institute.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: FH MRVM FM BM MR. Performed the experiments: FH MRVM FM BM RH RG. Analyzed the data: NF. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: FH RH. Wrote the paper: FH RG.

References

  1. 1. Goldman N (2001) Social inequalities in health disentangling the underlying mechanisms. Ann N Y Acad Sci 954: 118–139.
  2. 2. Smith JP (1999) Healthy bodies and thick wallets: the dual relation between health and economic status. J Econ Perspect 13: 144–166.
  3. 3. Jolly RD, Palmer DN, Dalefield RR (2002) The analytical approach to the nature of lipofuscin (age pigment). Arch Gerontol Geriatr 34: 205–217.
  4. 4. Jung T, Bader N, Grune T (2007) Lipofuscin: formation, distribution, and metabolic consequences. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1119: 97–111.
  5. 5. Siems WG, Grune T, Beierl B, Zollner H, Esterbauer H (1992) The metabolism of 4-hydroxynonenal, a lipid peroxidation product, is dependent on tumor age in Ehrlich mouse ascites cells. Exs 62: 124–135.
  6. 6. Siems WG, Grune T, Esterbauer H (1995) 4-Hydroxynonenal formation during ischemia and reperfusion of rat small intestine. Life Sci 57: 785–789.
  7. 7. Taylor A, Davies KJ (1987) Protein oxidation and loss of protease activity may lead to cataract formation in the aged lens. Free Radic Biol Med 3: 371–377.
  8. 8. Beregi E, Regius O, Rajczy K (1991) Comparative study of the morphological changes in lymphocytes of elderly individuals and centenarians. Age Ageing 20: 55–59.
  9. 9. Terman A, Brunk UT (1998) Lipofuscin: mechanisms of formation and increase with age. Apmis 106: 265–276.
  10. 10. Sohal RS, Brunk UT (1989) Lipofuscin as an indicator of oxidative stress and aging. Adv Exp Med Biol 266: 17–26. discussion 27–19.
  11. 11. Brosnan SF, Schiff HC, de Waal FB (2005) Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proc Biol Sci 272: 253–258.
  12. 12. Brosnan SF, De Waal FB (2003) Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature 425: 297–299.
  13. 13. Kaplan JR, Manuck SB, Clarkson TB, Lusso FM, Taub DM (1982) Social status, environment, and atherosclerosis in cynomolgus monkeys. Arteriosclerosis 2: 359–368.
  14. 14. Manuck SB, Marsland AL, Kaplan JR, Williams JK (1995) The pathogenicity of behavior and its neuroendocrine mediation: an example from coronary artery disease. Psychosom Med 57: 275–283.
  15. 15. Adams MR, Kaplan JR, Clarkson TB, Koritnik DR (1985) Ovariectomy, social status, and atherosclerosis in cynomolgus monkeys. Arteriosclerosis 5: 192–200.
  16. 16. Rozanski A, Blumenthal JA, Kaplan J (1999) Impact of psychological factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for therapy. Circulation 99: 2192–2217.
  17. 17. Schneiderman N (1987) Psychophysiologic factors in atherosclerosis and coronary heart disease. Circulation 76: (Suppl. I)141–147.
  18. 18. Roffe C (1998) Ageing of the heart. Br J Biomed Sci 55: 136–148.
  19. 19. Chaudhary A, Sharma S, James T (1995) Lipofuscin accumulation in neurons with restraint stress. Gerontology 41: Suppl 2229–237.
  20. 20. Marmot M (2005) Social determinants of health inequalities. Lancet 365: 1099–1104.
  21. 21. Steptoe A, Shamaei-Tousi A, Gylfe A, Henderson B, Bergstrom S, et al. (2007) Socioeconomic status, pathogen burden and cardiovascular disease risk. Heart 93: 1567–1570.
  22. 22. Cooper RS (2001) Social inequality, ethnicity and cardiovascular disease. Int J Epidemiol 30: Suppl 1S48–52.