Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Movie Smoking and Youth Initiation: Parsing Smoking Imagery and Other Adult Content

  • Matthew C. Farrelly ,

    mcf@rti.org

    Affiliation Public Health Policy Research Program, RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, United States of America

  • Kian Kamyab,

    Affiliation Public Health Policy Research Program, RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, United States of America

  • James Nonnemaker,

    Affiliation Public Health Policy Research Program, RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, United States of America

  • Erik Crankshaw,

    Affiliation Community Health Promotion Research Program, RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, United States of America

  • Jane A. Allen

    Affiliation Public Health Policy Research Program, RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina, United States of America

Abstract

Objectives

To isolate the independent influence of exposure to smoking and other adult content in the movies on youth smoking uptake.

Methods

We used discrete time survival analysis to quantify the influence of exposure to smoking and other adult content in the movies on transitioning from (1) closed to open to smoking; (2) never to ever trying smoking; and (3) never to ever hitting, slapping, or shoving someone on two or more occasions in the past 30 days. The latter is a comparative outcome, hypothesized to have no correlation with exposure to smoking in the movies.

Results

Assessed separately, both exposure to smoking imagery and exposure to adult content were associated with increased likelihood of youth becoming open to smoking (OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 1.04–1.15 and OR = 1.10, 95% CI: 1.04–1.17) and having tried smoking (OR = 1.06, 95% CI: 1.00–1.12 and OR = 1.06, 95% CI: 1.00–1.13). Both measures were also separately associated with aggressive behavior (OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 1.04–1.14 and OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 1.04–1.15). A very high correlation between the two measures (0.995, p<0.000) prevented an assessment of their independent effects on smoking initiation.

Conclusion

Although exposure to smoking in the movies is correlated with smoking susceptibility and initiation, the high correlation between exposure to smoking in the movies and other adult content suggests that more research is needed to disentangle their independent influence on smoking.

Introduction

Every year, adolescents around the world are repeatedly exposed to positive images of smoking in media, most notably in movies. A survey of 137 top grossing films released in 2010 revealed that 45% of all movies; 31% of movies rated G, PG, or PG-13; and 71% of movies rated R contained tobacco imagery. [1] Since 1991, more than 650 billion portrayals of tobacco use have been transmitted to audiences in the United States through thousands of films. [2].

The pervasive use of tobacco in movies has led to an emerging literature linking exposure to multiple youth smoking behaviors. In one of the first studies to explore the issue, Dalton et al. [3] found that youth with the highest level of exposure to smoking in the movies were 2.7 times more likely than those with the lowest exposure to initiate smoking. The authors concluded that 52% of smoking initiation among youth in the cohort was attributable to exposure to smoking in the movies. Subsequent studies corroborated this association [4][19] or expanded on it, [20][23] suggesting that exposure to smoking in the movies predicts the risk of becoming an established smoker.

However, as the body of research in this area grows, our understanding of the relationship between smoking in the movies and youth behavior becomes more nuanced. For example, Primack et al. [23] and Choi et al. [24] found that exposure to movie smoking or perceptions of the amount of smoking in movies was associated with smoking behavior among those younger than age 15, but not among older youth. Several studies showed that exposure to smoking imagery in movies had a greater influence on youth who may be considered low risk for smoking based on parental smoking behavior or their level of sensation seeking. [3], [18], [20] Jackson et al. [5] and Tanski et al. [18] have demonstrated racial/ethnic differences in the association between exposure to movie smoking and subsequent youth behavior, with stronger effects among white than black youth. Dalton et al. [25] and Hanewinkel et al. [16] found that the relative risk of smoking among adolescents was significantly lower among youth whose parents restricted their movie viewing based on movie ratings.

Contributing to the complexity of research in this area is the fact that movie smoking imagery is highly correlated with other adult content, such as alcohol and other drug use, sexual situations, and violence.[16], [26][28] Studies report correlations ranging from.83 (for smoking and alcohol imagery) to.99 (for smoking, violence, and sexual content). [27], [28] As a result, it is difficult to disentangle the independent effect of smoking imagery on youth behavior.

Investigators conducting experimental studies have attempted to address this issue by ensuring that movie clips contain no violent or sexual content [29], [30] or by explicitly controlling exposure. [31] Shadel et al. found that exposure to smoking imagery increased the risk of smoking initiation among those with no prior risk when smoking was framed as a “social” behavior and among those already at risk when smoking was framed as a “relaxing” behavior. [29], [30] Shmueli et al. found that exposure to movie clips with smoking imagery was associated with participant smoking during a break in the study period, while exposure to the same clips with the smoking digitally removed was not. [31] A strength of these experimental studies is that they isolate the effects of smoking imagery from other adult content. However, they do not advance our understanding of the long-term effects of exposure to smoking imagery on youth tobacco use initiation and progression to established smoking.

The studies that provide the strongest data on the effect of smoking imagery on youth behavior–such as Dalton et al. [3], Sargent et al., [4] and Jackson et al. [5]–control for differences in personal characteristics such as sensation seeking, and external influences such as peer or familial smoking and parenting style, but they do not control for exposure to adult content, such as violence, profanity, or sex. We know of one study in which the authors attempted to isolate the effect of specific movie content on youth behavior. Hanewinkel et al. [27] conducted a cross-sectional survey in six European countries with 16,000 public school students (mean age of 13). The study was designed to examine the relationship between youth exposure to alcohol imagery in movies and lifetime binge drinking among adolescents. Noting the high correlation between alcohol and smoking imagery in the study movies (.83), the authors conducted a sensitivity analysis to show that while movie exposure to alcohol imagery was associated with binge drinking, exposure to smoking imagery was not. [27] Taken together, this body of work suggests that exposure to smoking imagery in movies is associated with youth smoking; what is less clear is the extent to which this relationship is causal, and the magnitude of any true causal effect.

Isolating the effect of smoking imagery in movies from other adult content would strengthen the body of research being conducted in this area, and would enable us to more precisely quantify the effect of smoking imagery on subsequent youth behavior. In this study, we measure youth’s exposure to smoking in the movies and other adult content in top grossing movies in an attempt to disentangle these separate influences on adolescent smoking initiation.

Methods

Data

The primary data for this study come from the New York Longitudinal Youth Tobacco Evaluation Survey (NY-LYTES). NY-LYTES is designed to capture youth smoking transitions, from never smoking to experimentation and regular smoking, and the factors that influence these transitions, with a special focus on exposure to smoking in the movies. We conducted a random-digit-dial telephone survey of 1,511 New York State youth, aged 13 to 16 at baseline, in the spring and summer of 2005. Subsequent annual follow-up surveys included 1,060 youth in 2006, 809 youth in 2007, and 632 youth in 2008. NY-LYTES and the process of obtaining consent/assent for participation were both approved by the RTI International Institutional Review Board (IRB) and the New York State Department of Health IRB. Informed consent was provided orally by parents of survey respondents; respondents orally provided assent to participate. Oral rather than written consent/assent was obtained because the survey was conducted by telephone only. Oral consent/assent was documented by the interviewer as approved by the IRB. Key constructs are described below.

Outcome Measures

Outcomes are event indicators that indicate transitions from (1) closed to open to smoking; (2) never to ever trying smoking; and (3) never to ever hitting, slapping, or shoving someone on two or more occasions in the past 30 days. An indicator of ever smoking is based on an affirmative response to the question, “Have you ever tried cigarette smoking, even 1 or 2 puffs?” Openness to smoking is assessed only among those who report having never smoked a cigarette, even one or two puffs, and measured using three items on a 5-point Likert scale (definitely yes, probably yes, probably no, definitely no, and no opinion): (1) Do you think you will try a cigarette soon?; (2) Do you think you will smoke a cigarette at any time during the next year?; and (3) If one of your best friends offered you a cigarette, would you smoke it? Youth are considered closed to smoking (0) if they respond “Definitely not” to all three items and open to smoking (1) otherwise. [32] Our final outcome is based on the question, “During the past 30 days how many times did you hit, slap, or shove someone?” with responses of never, once, twice, or three or more times. Based on this question, we created an indicator for hitting, slapping, or shoving someone on two or more occasions (1) or less than two (0) occasions.

Potential Influences

In our analyses, we account for many of the same potential influences used in related literature on smoking in the movies, [25], [33], [34] including sensation seeking, receptivity to tobacco marketing, and parental monitoring of and rules about watching R-rated movies.

Exposure to smoking in the movies.

To estimate youth’s exposure to smoking in the movies, we combined data on top grossing films from www.boxofficemojo.com with rating information on the amount of smoking in movies from SceneSmoking.com, and youth self-reports on seeing individual movies. We first, we identified the top domestic grossing movies for the year preceding the annual wave of the survey. For the baseline wave of the survey, we selected 10 of the top grossing films with no or some tobacco use and 20 of the top grossing films with high levels of smoking. For subsequent waves of the survey, we selected 15 movies: 2 with little or no smoking and 13 with a lot of smoking. Of the 71 unique movies across all years, 68% (48) are PG-13, 18% (13) are R, 11% (8) are PG, and 3% (2) are G. Movies with no smoking received a score of 1 (N = 10); minimal amount of smoking, 2 (N = 4); moderate amount of smoking, 3 (N = 15); and a significant amount of smoking, 4 (N = 42). Based on these ratings, we created the Smoking in the Movies Exposure (SME) index. This index was calculated by multiplying a movie’s smoking rating by a number that indicates whether an individual has never seen the movie (0), has seen the movie only once (1), or has seen it more than once (2) and then summing this score across all movies in each wave. Individuals’ exposure for each given wave was then summed across all preceding waves to create a continuous, cumulative measure of exposure to smoking in the movies. This cumulative measure was scaled down by a factor of 20 to facilitate the interpretation of odds ratios.

Exposure to adult content in the movies.

To estimate youth’s exposure to adult content, we used ratings assigned by Kids-in-Mind.com, a Web site operated by the for-profit company Critics, Inc. The Kids-in-Mind rating system provides more detailed information on adult movie content than the system developed by the Motion Picture Association of America. While the Motion Picture Association assigns movies a single rating such as G, PG, or R, Kids-in-Mind assigns a 0 to 10 rating for three categories of adult content: sex/nudity, violence/gore, and profanity. Based on these data, we constructed the Adult Content Exposure (ACE) index in a similar manner to the SME index. We multiplied the Kids-in-Mind scale (0 to 30) by the frequency indicator (0, 1, 2) and then summed across all movies in each wave. We also created a cumulative sum at each wave to incorporate exposure in previous waves. The cumulative measure was then scaled down by a factor of 100.

Sensation seeking.

We measured sensation seeking, which has been shown to be associated with youth smoking, with the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale items: (1) I would like to explore strange places; (2) I like to do frightening things; (3) I like new and exciting experiences, even if I have to break the rules; and (4) I prefer friends who are exciting and unpredictable. [35] The items use a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly disagree). These responses were then reverse coded such that higher scores indicated stronger agreement. The four items were then summed, and a dichotomous indicator was created to indicate whether a respondent was below the median of this scale (0) or above (1). Youth missing data for any of the four items were considered to have missing data for this measure.

Tobacco marketing receptivity.

To measure youth’s receptivity to tobacco marketing, we constructed a receptivity score with 0 indicating that an individual was unable to name a brand of cigarettes, 1 indicating that they were able to name a brand of cigarettes, 2 indicating that they had a favorite cigarette advertisement (as well as being able to name a cigarette brand), and 3 indicating that they owned and were willing to use merchandise bearing a cigarette company’s name or logo (in addition to being able to name a cigarette brand and having a favorite ad).

Parental rules for and monitoring of R-rated movie and television watching.

We accounted for restrictions imposed by parents on respondents’ viewing of R-rated movies with two measures based on two questions with 4-point Likert scale response options (1 = never, 4 = all the time): (1) How often do your parents let you watch movies or videos that are rated R?; and (2) When you watch an R-rated movie, how often do you watch it with one or both of your parents? The first dichotomous measure equals 1 if the parent allows viewing R-rated movies “most of the time” or “all of the time” and 0 otherwise. The second measure is 1 if the parent “never” or “sometimes” co-views and is 0 otherwise.

Parental oversight and involvement in respondents’ movie viewing was assessed in a manner detailed by Dalton et al. [25] Four items on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = never, 4 = all the time) were used: (1) How often do your parents want to know what a movie is rated before you can see it?; (2) How often do you have to check with your parents before watching a movie?; (3) How often do your parents go into the video store with you when you rent a movie?; and (4) When you go to a friend’s house, how often does your parent check to see what movie you might be watching? The final two items (3, 4) allowed for youth to respond that they “do not go to the movie store” and “do not go to friends’ houses,” respectively. Responses were dichotomized so that responses other than “All the time” equaled 0.

Parental limits on television viewing were measured by responses to the question, “Do your parents limit the amount of time you spend watching TV or movies?” Responses were dichotomous, and respondents were coded with a 1 for “yes” responses and a 0 for “no” responses.

Other variables.

Other measures used for analysis include age, race/ethnicity, gender, residence in New York City, attendance of a public school, a dichotomized indicator for above average academic achievement, church attendance, the presence of an adult at home after school, employment, income (scaled down by a factor of 100), having a friend who smokes, having a friend who smokes marijuana, presence of a smoking ban in respondent’s household, exposure to secondhand smoke, and exposure to tobacco use prevention lessons in school.

Analysis

We estimated the relationship between exposure to smoking in the movies and youth transitions using discrete-time survival analysis. To use discrete-time survival analysis requires creating a person-year data set in which each individual contributes to the estimation as long as he or she is at risk of the event occurring. That is, at each age, we start with all sample members who have not experienced the event but are at risk of the event occurring (e.g., never smoked in the model of the transition to ever smoking) and then estimate the risk of the event occurring (transition to smoking) as the sample of youth ages.

We used a logistic regression model to estimate the probability of event occurrence given that the event has not yet occurred. [36], [37] In these models, once the event in question occurs, the sample member is dropped from subsequent time periods because he or she is no longer at risk. This process allowed the calculation of the odds that an individual will initiate smoking for each age represented in the sample, given that they had not begun smoking previously. Although there is attrition from the cohort over time, the sample size used in these analyses depends on whether the event occurred during any of the surveys in which the youth participated.

The key covariates of interest are the SME and ACE indexes. For each outcome, we estimated separate models using either the SME or ACE index. For all regressions, we present two model specifications: (1) a crude, bivariate model and (2) an adjusted model including the covariates described above. In addition to these models, we examined the degree of correlation between the SME and ACE indexes.

As a test of whether exposure to smoking in the movies is a proxy for youth’s risk more broadly, rather than for smoking initiation specifically, we also examined whether exposure to smoking in the movies is correlated with youth exhibiting aggressive behavior.

Results

Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the baseline sample of youth. At baseline, observed SME scores ranged from 0 to 174, with a mean score of 70.6 out of a total possible score of 180. ACE scores ranged from 0 to 724, with a mean score of 304.1 out of a total possible score of 760. Table 2 illustrates the number of person-years of data available for each outcome, the number who experience the event of interest (i.e., “fail”), and the number of youth who are right censored (i.e., lost to follow-up before experiencing the event/outcome of interest).

thumbnail
Table 1. Baseline Demographics and Summary Statistics from the New York Longitudinal Youth Tobacco Evaluation Survey (NY-LYTES), 2005–2008.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051935.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Life Tables for the New York Longitudinal Youth Tobacco Evaluation Survey (NY-LYTES), 2005–2008.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051935.t002

Exposure to smoking in the movies is significantly associated with increased odds of never smokers becoming open to smoking and initiating smoking (Table 3). We find that as the SME index increases, youth are more likely to be open to smoking (adjusted OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 1.04–1.15) and to have tried smoking (adjusted OR = 1.06, 95% CI: 1.00–1.12). Because the SME index is a continuous variable, another method for understanding the magnitude of the effect is to compare observed smoking initiation with predicted smoking initiation assuming the SME index is zero. This comparison indicates that smoking initiation would have been 21% lower if this cohort was never exposed to smoking in the movies for the fully adjusted model and 66% lower for the simple bivariate model. In addition, increases in the SME index are associated with increases in the likelihood of engaging in aggressive behavior (adjusted OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 1.04–1.14).

thumbnail
Table 3. Influence of Exposure to Smoking in the Movies and Adult Content on Initiation to Smoking, Becoming Open to Smoking, and Exhibiting Aggressive Behavior.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051935.t003

Next, we replaced the SME index with the ACE index and find similar results (see Table 3). As this index increases, youth are more likely to be open to smoking (adjusted OR = 1.10, 95% CI: 1.04–1.17) and to have tried smoking (adjusted OR = 1.06, 95% CI: 1.00–1.13). Additionally, increases in the ACE index are associated with increases in the likelihood of engaging in aggressive behavior (adjusted OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 1.04–1.15).

Given the similarity of the results for the SME and ACE indexes, we examined the correlation between the two indexes and found that it is 0.995 (p<0.001). The similarity between the two measures is illustrated in Figure 1. For this figure, each sample member is categorized into quartiles of exposure to smoking in the movies and adult content. We then display the percentage of the sample that is in the highest quartile of adult content for each of the quartiles of exposure to smoking in the movies. This shows that nearly all the youth who are in the highest quartile for exposure to adult content are also in the highest quartile for exposure to smoking in the movies. Additionally, we estimated the correlation between modified versions of the indexes at baseline by recalculating SME and ACE indexes using a dichotomous, as opposed to a three-level, frequency of exposure indicator. At baseline, the correlation between the recalculated indexes is 0.992 (p<0.001).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Percentage of the New York Longitudinal Youth Tobacco Evaluation Survey (NY-LYTES) 2005–2008 Sample in the Highest Quartile of Exposure to Adult Content in Movies for each Quartile of Exposure to Smoking in Movies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051935.g001

Discussion

This study was designed to build on earlier work examining the relationship between exposure to smoking imagery in movies and youth smoking. Noting the correlation between smoking imagery and adult content in movies, the intent of this analysis was to illustrate that exposure to smoking imagery was correlated with smoking initiation above and beyond any potential influence of other adult content. We sought to disentangle the influence of smoking imagery from other adult content–such as nudity, profanity, and violence–by creating separate exposure measures for each. Based on a sample of 71 top grossing films over a 4-year period, we found that the correlation between smoking imagery and other adult content was so high that it was impossible to disentangle their separate influence. In fact, the two measures are almost perfectly correlated.

The link between youth exposure to smoking in movies and subsequent smoking behavior has been observed in numerous studies, using different designs, and in multiple countries. Based on theories of social learning, [38] it is plausible that exposure to smoking imagery in movies influences youth smoking behavior. However, other theories, such as Problem Behavior Theory, [39], [40] would also lead one to postulate a plausible relationship between exposure to other adult content and smoking. Although studies have attempted to control for variables that might be common to both exposure to smoking in movies and smoking behavior (e.g., sensation seeking), biases in the measurement of these variables (due to youth self-reports for example) could still result in a biased estimate of the relationship between exposure to smoking in the movies and smoking behavior.

This study raises at least three challenges to investigators working in this area. First, we should attempt to disentangle the effects of exposure to smoking imagery from other adult content to better quantify the independent influence of smoking in the movies. Experimental studies such as the one conducted by Shmueli et al. [31] have begun to address this issue. Investigators with more extensive data sets and a greater selection of movies should also be able to improve upon existing research using survey data to disentangle the effects of exposure to smoking in the movies from other adult content.

Second, studies that do not randomly assign adolescents to movie exposure should make every effort to control for youth self-selection of adult content. It is possible that youth who elect to watch movies with smoking and other adult content are also more likely to smoke; in other words, the apparent relationship is due, at least in part, to some unmeasured variable or variables. To date, studies have attempted to control for factors that may be associated with increased exposure to adult content, such as sensation seeking, parenting style, and peer or parent smoking. However, there may be room for improvement in this area; at least one study shows that common measures of sensation seeking do not perform equally well across race-ethnicity, [41] and youth self-reports of parenting variables may be prone to bias. [42] Third, we should begin to develop a better sense of factors that may mediate or moderate the relationship between exposure to smoking imagery and youth behavior. For example, new research suggests that youth who are more highly transported into narratives show greater attitude, belief, and behavior change as a result of those narratives, a finding that may have important implications for our work. [43] Investigators in tobacco control and public health should work collaboratively with colleagues in communication and media research to identify additional potential factors of importance to this body of research.

This study has several limitations. First, the study is based on a smaller set of movies than the seminal work by Sargent, Dalton, and colleagues. Second, the current study examines youth who were older at baseline (aged 13 to 16) than in previous studies, potentially missing earlier initiation. Third, this study relies on data that were not collected or analyzed by the investigators, and for which we have no information related to reliability or validity: boxofficemojo.com (to define top grossing movies), SceneSmoking.com (to develop the SME index), and Kids-in-Mind.com (to develop the ACE index). Fourth, the telephone surveys used in this study likely underestimate the true level of smoking compared to other modes of data collection (e.g., school, household). [44] Despite these limitations, our results are qualitatively similar to many other similar studies, although we draw different conclusions.

This study raises questions about the extent to which existing studies have established a causal link between smoking in movies and youth behavior. Our findings suggest that additional studies are needed to disentangle exposure to smoking imagery and other adult content so as to quantify the magnitude of a true causal relationship between exposure to smoking in the movies and youth smoking.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Kelsey Campbell and Susan Murchie, both of RTI International, for their assistance with this project. We are also grateful to two anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. Their suggestions markedly strengthened this work.

Ethics Statement

The New York Longitudinal Youth Tobacco Evaluation Survey (NY-LYTES) and the process of obtaining consent/assent for participation were both approved by the RTI International Institutional Review Board (IRB) (#11127 Youth Cohort Study) and then by the New York State Department of Health IRB (#04–082 New York State Youth and Young Adult Tobacco Cohort Study). Informed consent was provided orally by parents of survey respondents; respondents orally provided assent to participate. Oral rather than written consent/assent was obtained because the survey was conducted by telephone only. Oral consent/assent was documented by the interviewer as approved by the IRB.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MF KK JN EC JA. Analyzed the data: KK. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: EC. Wrote the paper: MF KK JN EC JA. Conceived of the study and designed and directed the analysis and writing: MF. Developed measures: EC. Conducted the analyses: KK. Assisted with the methods and analysis: MF KK JN. Interpreted results: MF KK JN EC. Wrote the manuscript: MF EC JA. Reviewed draft manuscripts and provided suggestions on changes and additional analyses: MF KK JN EC JA.

References

  1. 1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2011) Smoking in top-grossing movies–United States, 2010. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 60: 910–913.
  2. 2. Titus K PJ, Glantz S. (2009) Smoking presentation trends in U.S. movies 1991–2008. http://escholarship.org/uc/item/30q9j424: UC San Francisco: Center for Tobacco Control Research and Education.
  3. 3. Dalton MA, Sargent JD, Beach ML, Titus-Ernstoff L, Gibson JJ, et al. (2003) Effect of viewing smoking in movies on adolescent smoking initiation: a cohort study. Lancet 362: 281–285.
  4. 4. Sargent JD, Beach ML, Adachi-Mejia AM, Gibson JJ, Titus-Ernstoff LT, et al. (2005) Exposure to movie smoking: its relation to smoking initiation among US adolescents. Pediatrics 116: 1183–1191.
  5. 5. Jackson C, Brown JD, L’Engle KL (2007) R-rated movies, bedroom televisions, and initiation of smoking by white and black adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 161: 260–268.
  6. 6. Dal Cin S, Gibson B, Zanna MP, Shumate R, Fong GT (2007) Smoking in movies, implicit associations of smoking with the self, and intentions to smoke. Psychol Sci 18: 559–563.
  7. 7. Wellman RJ, Sugarman DB, DiFranza JR, Winickoff JP (2006) The extent to which tobacco marketing and tobacco use in films contribute to children’s use of tobacco: a meta-analysis. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 160: 1285–1296.
  8. 8. Song AV, Ling PM, Neilands TB, Glantz SA (2007) Smoking in movies and increased smoking among young adults. Am J Prev Med 33: 396–403.
  9. 9. Hanewinkel R, Sargent JD (2007) Exposure to smoking in popular contemporary movies and youth smoking in Germany. Am J Prev Med 32: 466–473.
  10. 10. Thrasher JF, Jackson C, Arillo-Santillan E, Sargent JD (2008) Exposure to smoking imagery in popular films and adolescent smoking in Mexico. Am J Prev Med 35: 95–102.
  11. 11. Thrasher JF, Sargent JD, Huang L, Arillo-Santillan E, Dorantes-Alonso A, et al. (2009) Does film smoking promote youth smoking in middle-income countries?: A longitudinal study among Mexican adolescents. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 18: 3444–3450.
  12. 12. Titus-Ernstoff L, Dalton MA, Adachi-Mejia AM, Longacre MR, Beach ML (2008) Longitudinal study of viewing smoking in movies and initiation of smoking by children. Pediatrics 121: 15–21.
  13. 13. Wilkinson AV, Spitz MR, Prokhorov AV, Bondy ML, Shete S, et al. (2009) Exposure to smoking imagery in the movies and experimenting with cigarettes among Mexican heritage youth. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 18: 3435–3443.
  14. 14. Sargent JD, Hanewinkel R (2009) Comparing the effects of entertainment media and tobacco marketing on youth smoking in Germany. Addiction 104: 815–823.
  15. 15. Dal Cin S, Stoolmiller M, Sargent JD (2012) When movies matter: exposure to smoking in movies and changes in smoking behavior. J Health Commun 17: 76–89.
  16. 16. Hanewinkel R, Morgenstern M, Tanski SE, Sargent JD (2008) Longitudinal study of parental movie restriction on teen smoking and drinking in Germany. Addiction 103: 1722–1730.
  17. 17. Heatherton TF, Sargent JD (2009) Does watching smoking in movies promote teenage smoking? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 18: 63–67.
  18. 18. Tanski SE, Stoolmiller M, Gerrard M, Sargent JD (2012) Moderation of the association between media exposure and youth smoking onset: race/ethnicity, and parent smoking. Prev Sci 13: 55–63.
  19. 19. Morgenstern M, Poelen EA, Scholte R, Karlsdottir S, Jonsson SH, et al. (2011) Smoking in movies and adolescent smoking: cross-cultural study in six European countries. Thorax 66: 875–883.
  20. 20. Sargent JD, Stoolmiller M, Worth KA, Dal Cin S, Wills TA, et al. (2007) Exposure to smoking depictions in movies: its association with established adolescent smoking. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 161: 849–856.
  21. 21. Dalton MA, Beach ML, Adachi-Mejia AM, Longacre MR, Matzkin AL, et al. (2009) Early exposure to movie smoking predicts established smoking by older teens and young adults. Pediatrics 123: e551–558.
  22. 22. Adachi-Mejia AM, Primack BA, Beach ML, Titus-Ernstoff L, Longacre MR, et al. (2009) Influence of movie smoking exposure and team sports participation on established smoking. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 163: 638–643.
  23. 23. Primack BA, Longacre MR, Beach ML, Adachi-Mejia AM, Titus LJ, et al. (2012) Association of established smoking among adolescents with timing of exposure to smoking depicted in movies. J Natl Cancer Inst 104: 549–555.
  24. 24. Choi K, Forster JL, Erickson DJ, Lazovich D, Southwell BG (2011) Prevalence of smoking in movies as perceived by teenagers longitudinal trends and predictors. Am J Prev Med 41: 167–173.
  25. 25. Dalton MA, Adachi-Mejia AM, Longacre MR, Titus-Ernstoff LT, Gibson JJ, et al. (2006) Parental rules and monitoring of children’s movie viewing associated with children’s risk for smoking and drinking. Pediatrics 118: 1932–1942.
  26. 26. Tickle JJ, Beach ML, Dalton MA (2009) Tobacco, alcohol, and other risk behaviors in film: how well do MPAA ratings distinguish content? J Health Commun 14: 756–767.
  27. 27. Hanewinkel R, Sargent JD, Poelen EA, Scholte R, Florek E, et al. (2012) Alcohol consumption in movies and adolescent binge drinking in 6 European countries. Pediatrics 129: 709–720.
  28. 28. Chapman S, Farrelly MC (2011) Four arguments against the adult-rating of movies with smoking scenes. PLoS Med 8: e1001078.
  29. 29. Shadel WG, Martino SC, Haviland A, Setodji C, Primack BA (2010) Smoking motives in movies are important for understanding adolescent smoking: a preliminary investigation. Nicotine Tob Res 12: 850–854.
  30. 30. Shadel WG, Martino SC, Setodji C, Haviland A, Primack BA, et al. (2012) Motives for smoking in movies affect future smoking risk in middle school students: An experimental investigation. Drug Alcohol Depend 123: 66–71.
  31. 31. Shmueli D, Prochaska JJ, Glantz SA (2010) Effect of smoking scenes in films on immediate smoking: a randomized controlled study. Am J Prev Med 38: 351–358.
  32. 32. Mowery PD, Farrelly MC, Haviland ML, Gable JM, Wells HE (2004) Progression to established smoking among US youths. Am J Public Health 94: 331–337.
  33. 33. Sargent JD, Beach ML, Dalton MA, Ernstoff LT, Gibson JJ, et al. (2004) Effect of parental R-rated movie restriction on adolescent smoking initiation: a prospective study. Pediatrics 114: 149–156.
  34. 34. Sargent JD, Gibson J, Heatherton TF (2009) Comparing the effects of entertainment media and tobacco marketing on youth smoking. Tob Control 18: 47–53.
  35. 35. Stephenson MT, Hoyle RH, Palmgreen P, Slater MD (2003) Brief measures of sensation seeking for screening and large-scale surveys. Drug Alcohol Depend 72: 279–286.
  36. 36. Allison PD (1982) Discrete-time methods for the analysis of event histories. Sociological Methodology 13: 61–98.
  37. 37. Singer JD, Willett JB (1993) It’s about time: using discrete-time survival analysis to study duration and the timing of events. J Educ Stat 18: 155–195.
  38. 38. Bandura A (1986) Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  39. 39. Donovan JE, Jessor R, Costa FM (1988) Syndrome of problem behavior in adolescence: a replication. J Consult Clin Psychol 56: 762–765.
  40. 40. Jessor R (1992) Risk behavior in adolescence: a psychosocial framework for understanding and action. In: Rogers DE, Ginzburg E, editors. Adolescents at risk: Medical and social perspectives. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. 19–34.
  41. 41. Vallone D, Allen JA, Clayton RR, Xiao H (2007) How reliable and valid is the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (BSSS-4) for youth of various racial/ethnic groups? Addiction 102 Suppl 271–78.
  42. 42. Reynolds EK, MacPherson L, Matusiewicz AK, Schreiber WM, Lejuez CW (2011) Discrepancy between mother and child reports of parental knowledge and the relation to risk behavior engagement. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 40: 67–79.
  43. 43. Green MC, Clark JL (2012) Transportation into narrative worlds: implications for entertainment media influences on tobacco use. Addiction. [Epub ahead of print].
  44. 44. Currivan D, Nyman AL, Turner CF, Biener L (2004) Does telephone audio computer-assisted self-interviewing improve the accuracy of prevalence estimates of youth smoking? Evidence from the UMass Tobacco Study. Public Opin Q 68: 542–564.