Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Personal Traits Underlying Environmental Preferences: A Discrete Choice Experiment

  • Mario Soliño ,

    solino.mario@inia.es

    Affiliations National Institute for Agriculture and Food Research and Technology (INIA), Forest Research Centre (CIFOR), Madrid, Spain, Sustainable Forest Management Research Institute, University of Valladolid & INIA, Palencia, Spain

  • Begoña A. Farizo

    Affiliation Institute for Public Goods and Policies (IPP), Spanish National Research Council (CSIC), Madrid, Spain

Abstract

Personality plays a role in human behavior, and thus can influence consumer decisions on environmental goods and services. This paper analyses the influence of the big five personality dimensions (extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism and openness) in a discrete choice experiment dealing with preferences for the development of an environmental program for forest management in Spain. For this purpose, a reduced version of the Big Five Inventory survey (the BFI-10) is implemented. Results show a positive effect of openness and extraversion and a negative effect of agreeableness and neuroticism in consumers' preferences for this environmental program. Moreover, results from a latent class model show that personal traits help to explain preference heterogeneity.

Introduction

It is broadly recognized that the specific behavior of individuals is conditioned by individual factors, home-site factors [1], [2], [3], [4], [5] and a set of formal rules and socially accepted informal rules [6], such as those of family or culture. Personality also plays a role in human behavior and thus, can influence environmental concerns [7], environmental engagement [8] and consumer decisions on environmental goods and services [9].

Several environmental studies have considered the influence of attitudes or tastes [10], [11], [12], [13] and psychological constructs [14], [15] on individual preferences. The influence of personality dimensions on environmental preferences has been less examined. To correct this gap, a reduced version of the BFI such as the BFI-10 is a suitable instrument to incorporate personality traits in environmental valuation, since it is a recommended approach for research in which participant time is limited and when personality assessment would otherwise be impossible. Environmental economics and more specifically, environmental valuation, uses survey-based instruments to analyze consumer behavior among natural and environmental resources. In this case, the BFI-10 shows acceptable psychometric properties [16] and is a suitable instrument to introduce the personality traits into the economic analysis.

The Big Five Inventory (BFI) is a personality survey based on a set of phrases used to measure the Big Five dimensions of personality, i.e., extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism and openness [17]. Although BFI can be answered in less than 10 minutes, there is a growing demand for shorter instruments [18], [19], [20]. Reduced scales of BFI cannot be used as a common approach to assess personality [16], [20], but is a good alternative to implement personality in other scientific disciplines different from psychology, such as economics.

The paper is structured as follows. Section 2 presents the theoretical underpinnings of Discrete Choice Experiments and the BFI-10. Section 3 outlines the questionnaire used to analyze the influence of personality traits on environmental valuation. In Section 4 we present the main results of the study. Finally, Section 5 is devoted to conclusions and discussion.

Materials and Methods

The BFI-10

The Big Five Traits allow us to classify people by analyzing their ratings in a set of short phrases characterizing five independent personality dimensions. Following [16], the BFI-10 was implemented in order to analyze the influence of personality traits in environmental valuation. Ten short phrases were used to represent the big five personality dimensions, with just 2 items per dimension (extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism and openness). Individuals rate these 10 short phrases (Table 1) describing their personality on a five-step scale from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly).

A comprehensive analysis of different interpretations of these dimensions is presented in [21], [22]. For the purpose of this article, we summarize the focal interpretation of the five personality dimensions:

  1. Extraversion: people classified as “extroverts” show social adaptability and interpersonal involvement, and they are talkative, assertive, active, outgoing, and outspoken. Therefore, we expect a positive likelihood of these people contributing to the development of a new environmental program.
  2. Agreeableness: this dimension implies pro-social behavior, and people scoring high on agreeableness tend to be sympathetic, kindly, appreciative, affectionate, soft-hearted, warm and generous. There is no prior expectation for the influence of this dimension on choices for the environmental program. Nevertheless, a significant positive effect could be interpreted as a warm-glow bias (friendly behavior).
  3. Conscientiousness: they are organized, thorough, playful, efficient, responsible and reliable. They are “global-thinkers” and, due to the regional character of the proposed environmental program, we expect a negative or non-significant relationship between high scores in this dimension and the probability of accepting a small (not far-reaching) environmental program.
  4. Neuroticism: typically, people classified as “neurotics” are tense, anxious, nervous, moody, and tend to worry. Therefore, a negative relationship is expected between high scores in this dimension and the probability of accepting a novel environmental program. These personalities might tend to choose the status quo option rather an option of change [9]. Moreover, we expect extreme behaviors and a high dispersion of responses from neurotics.
  5. Openness: these are people open to experience; they have wide interests and are imaginative, intelligent and original. Therefore, we expect a positive likelihood of these people contributing to the development of a new environmental program.

Discrete choice experiments

Discrete Choice Experiments (DCEs) simulate markets in which different environmental goods and services compete in a realistic trade-off manner. Individuals choose between different alternatives involving environmental attributes, according to their own preferences and budget constraints. DCEs allow researchers to infer consumers' preferences and implicit prices for several characteristics embedded in a prospective policy or environmental program. Since accounting for unobserved heterogeneity is a matter of importance in the estimation of behavioral models, two mixed approaches are applied: a Random Parameter Logit (RPL) model [23] and a Latent Class (LC) Model [24].

The RPL model introduces unobserved preference heterogeneity, i.e., allows the coefficients of observed variables to vary randomly among people rather than being fixed. In RPL models, the individual's i indirect utility function (Vi) is usually represented as a linear additive expression:(1)where αj is an alternative specific constant (ASC) for each option (j =  1,2,...,J) in the choice set, Sij is the associated attribute vector, is the vector of population mean preference values, θi represents the deviations in individual preferences with respect to the mean values, and is an i.i.d. type I extreme value random component of utility which cannot be observed by the researcher. Coefficients β vary in the population with density ƒ(β|Ω), with Ω denoting the parameters of density. If we assume that the individual's preferences, as represented by βi  =  +θi, follow the same decision heuristics for all choice (t =  1,2,..,T), the probability of individual i's observed sequence of choices [y1, y2,...,yT] is calculated by solving the integral:(2)where j is the alternative chosen in choice occasion t and μ is a scale parameter.

As pointed above, another way to capture taste variations is through LC or by a combination of LC and RPL. In this study we apply a latent class model with common and specific random parameters. Latent class has been long applied in environmental modeling, but not many studies combine both mixed approaches. Some examples of the dynamism of LC can be seen in [25], describing the choice of excursions in natural protected areas; [26] for transport issues; or [4], [27] for valuing environmental goods.

Following [24], the model has several classes (x) for the probability that case i selects alternative j at the replication t, given attribute value (characteristics of alternatives), covariates , used to predict class membership (x), and random parameters (RP) denoted by Fdi being d the number of factors. The Fdi are assumed to be standard normally distributed and mutually independent () where I is the identity matrix.

In a general case, with a conditional logit model, the response probability is:(3)where is the systematic component in the utility of alternative j at replication t given that case i belongs to latent class x. The linear model is:

(4)The term is a linear function of an alternative specific constant , attribute effects and random effects. The first term () defines random effects for the alternative specific constants, and the following terms define random effects for the attributes.

In a latent class choice model with random parameters, the probability density has this form:(5)where

(6)A multinomial logit is specified in which class membership is regressed on covariates;(7)with linear term , being the constant corresponding to latent class x and the effect of the rth covariate for the Class x.

In our study, personal traits are included as covariates to explain belonging to a specific class, assuming that members of the same class share a pattern of choice. On the other hand, we assume that attributes from the discrete choice experiment behave randomly. This specification allows us to identify whether one or more attributes are not random to a specific class, improving the accuracy of the model.

The environmental valuation questionnaire

The choice experiment aims to quantify the population's preferences for several externalities associated with a program developing resin-tapping in Spain, i.e., biodiversity of flora, employment generated by the strengthening of the sector, reduced risk of fire, the presence in the market of “certified products” that incorporate natural resin, and the duration and the annual cost of the program. The levels presented are detailed in Table 2.

The discrete choice experiment is implemented in an environmental valuation questionnaire structured in 4 sections. Section 1 contains questions relating to the relationships between the interviewee and forests. The second section examines knowledge about resin-tapping activities. Section 3 contains the discrete choice experiment. The choice sets were designed following an optimal in difference design as proposed by [28]. From an optimal design size of 18 choice cards, a blocking strategy was performed and each respondent was confronted with nine choice cards containing two prospective programs and the status quo scenario (where the program is not implemented and the individual does not pay any extra fee). The experimental design is balanced, i.e. all the attribute levels are shown the same number of times to each individual. In general, unbalanced designs should be avoided where possible because statistical power differs within attribute levels and/or between attributes, and artificial correlations with grand means or model intercepts are introduced [29].

Finally, the fourth section is focused on socio-economic characteristics and includes the BFI-10 questions. The questionnaire was administered in September and October 2012 to 2,224 individuals. The target population was adult dwellers from Castilla y León, since resin-tapping activities in Spain are concentrated in this region (located in central Spain). The database used for this analysis is available on request from the authors.

Results

From the individuals' ratings on the BFI-10 short-phrases items, the average score is calculated for each of the big five personality dimensions. The results are shown in Table 3. Conscientiousness shows the higher value and neuroticism shows the lower one, whereas extraversion, agreeableness and openness show similar values. Low correlations are detected for the five dimensions of personality. This is a good indicator of the independence of these personality dimensions.

thumbnail
Table 3. Average score and correlations for the big five personality dimensions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089603.t003

Table 4 presents the results of the RPL estimations using 500 Halton draws (Train, 2003) and NLOGIT 5.0 software. All attributes, apart from the cost and the Alternative Specific Constant (ASC) were assumed to be normally distributed. Differences of the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) between a baseline model (without including personality traits) and the expanded-BFI model favored the selection of the last one. The purpose of using these explanatory variables in our analysis is focused on getting a better understanding on the relation between personality traits and choices, to help us explaining why individuals prefer certain things over others. The ASC was introduced in the models as a dummy variable taking value 1 to indicate that the individual does not choose the status quo scenario. The estimated coefficient for the ASC is statistically non-significant. This result suggests that individual heterogeneity is correctly captured by the model.

From these results (Table 4) we can state that personality dimensions have an effect on the choices individuals make. Our results show that the most influential trait is openness, that is, individuals showing a high score in openness (people open to new experiences) are more prone to choose the options for protection than people with other dominant traits. All covariates have the expected sign except agreeableness, for which we did not have any a priori expectations about the sign. Since stated preference techniques are prone to get a positive answer from individuals whatever they are offered, our hypothesis was that individuals who show this trait could favor the yea-saying bias.

It would thus be worthwhile to know which aspects of the personality make us value certain attributes over others; that is, the influence of personality traits on preferences for environmental programs. For this we run a latent class model using Latent Gold 4.5 software. In order to benefit from the virtues of the RPL models, we consider that some attributes may have just one source of variability (for FireL, FireM, Time, CertM and CertH) while for others (Employ, BioM, BioH and Cost) we identified two, one dependent on the class and another constant for all the classes. In other words, we assume that for some attributes dispersion around the mean estimates is the same for all individuals, like the RPL shown above, while for some other attributes we also have identified variability in the class (intra-class).

Table 5 shows the mixed LC-RPL model, where the SDPD column contains the Standard Deviations of the estimates for the classes and the last column, Common SDPD shows the Standard Deviations for the variables, regardless of the class. This decomposition of variability has the advantage of informing us about possible sources of heterogeneity. For example, the estimates for medium risk of fire (FireM) are positive for all classes and show high variability for everybody, while for low risk (FireL) the variation is due to the composition of the classes. For some, this is something desirable, while for others it seems it is enough to achieve the medium level; some risk of fire might be acceptable for certain groups of the society because low risk could not be credible or because the cost of achieving that goal may be disproportionate. Fire variables were expected to behave quite homogeneously and have low variability. On the contrary, BioM and BioH show great inter- and intra-class variability, as shown by the common and by-class SD. Cost is another variable with great variation. Traditionally, the cost of the program parameter is considered as fixed, since the elicitation of willingness to pay (WTP) measures complicates the computation. In this study we are not interested in getting the benefits (in terms of willingness to pay) of the program but to control and explain heterogeneity of preferences, thus Cost is considered to have a random nature. Again, two sources of variability have been identified, one common to all individuals no matter the class, and another for the classes. As with the BioM and BioH, for some classes the decisions on this attribute are not random, as is the case for classes 3 and 4. If we go into the composition of the classes attending to their personality traits, we can see that class 4 is the one with lower random behavior and Openness is the trait of their members (around 13% of the sample). The most valued aspect of the program for this class is biodiversity followed by fires, both a medium level but valuing positively high levels of biodiversity and negatively low levels of risk of fires.

Classes 1, 2 and 6 are at the other end, showing high variation of their mean parameters. Attending to the personality traits, class 6, which only represents 3% of the sample, has none that could describe their mean personality. Class 2, with almost 22% of the sample, groups the people with low extraversion and positive and high scores on agreeableness and conscientiousness. Class 1 is the biggest with almost 38% of the participants. They have high scores on openness (positive) and on agreeableness (negative). For people in this group cost is the least important attribute and FireL the most important, and they will choose any improvement for the management of these areas. As expected, this group has a high variability on all the parameters. This may be due, among other things, to the fact that is the most populated class.

A trait that deserves attention is neuroticism, which is significant only for class 3 (14% of the sample) together with a negative openness. Our results confirm [30], indicating that neuroticism could be a weaker predictor of a range of outcomes. As expected, few aspects of the environmental program are appreciated by this group. Key attributes like fire are not even significant. Class 5 (around 11% of the sample) is pro-environmental and the positive sign of cost could have an explanation in the positive and significant traits for this class: extraversion and openness.

Finally, in Section 2 we hypothesized that a positive and significant Agreeableness could lead to a warm glow behavior, but our results shows that the assertive aspect of Extraversion, combined with other traits could be more prone to this kind of bias (see Class 5). Our experiment is not big enough to provide evidence or establish these relations; thus only further research could lead us to find if those relations are possible.

Conclusions

This paper relates personality traits directly with environmental preferences. It shows that personality plays a role in human behavior and thus, it can influence consumer decisions on environmental goods and services. In environmental valuation with stated preference methods a hypothetical market is simulated through a survey instrument. In order to avoid fatigue effects, the survey time is very limited, thus a key issue for implementing personality traits in environmental economics is the time required to answer the psychological questions. For this reason, a reduced version of Big Five Inventory with acceptable psychometric properties (the BFI-10) was applied in order to analyze the influence of personality traits in a discrete choice experiment. Therefore, this paper uses a psychometric approach to highlight the relevance of personality traits in consumer decisions, relating directly personality to economic behavior. Other approaches such as the New Ecological Paradigm scale [31] have connected pro-environmental behavior with economic behavior (i.e. a higher willingness to pay). Moreover, in the literature are numerous examples connecting different types of behavior and, what it is very interesting too, connecting the “values” such as the Schwartz Value Theory [32], [33] to explain individual preferences [34]. Discussions on these issues can be found in several papers from the 2011 International Choice Modelling Conference [35]. We have tried with this experiment to go a step backwards, since we agree in that values are partly inspired by personality. But we recognize that this study is a short piece of the research on the influence of personality on individual preferences, and other relations will be tested in the future.

This study brings new insights on the relationship between personality and environmental values. Results show a positive effect of openness and extraversion and a negative effect of agreeableness and neuroticism in consumers' preferences for this environmental program. Nevertheless, these effects are diverse and affect the heterogeneity of preferences, the preferred characteristics and the desirability of the program itself. Further research is necessary to explain, for example, why estimates from individuals with high scores on conscientiousness have higher random behavior than those showing a neuroticism trait. That is, to better explain the relationship between personality traits and tastes and preferences. In summary, this work constitutes a new step to understanding the involvement of individuals in environmental conservation and could help to better design appropriate ways to reach certain groups and ensure the success of environmental and social goals.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MS BAF. Performed the experiments: MS BAF. Analyzed the data: MS BAF. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MS BAF. Wrote the paper: MS BAF.

References

  1. 1. Fishbein M, Azjen I (1975) Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley Publishing.
  2. 2. Bateman IJ (1996) An economic comparison of forest recreation, timber and carbon fixing values with agriculture in Wales: A geographical information systems approach. PhD Thesis, Department of Economics, University of Nottingham.
  3. 3. Soliño M, Farizo BA, Campos P (2009) The influence of home-site factors on residents' willingness to pay: An application for power generation from scrubland in Galicia, Spain. Energy Policy 37 (10): 4055–4065.
  4. 4. Farizo BA, Joyce J, Soliño M (2014) Dealing with heterogeneous preferences using multilevel mixed models. Land Econ 90(1): 181–198.
  5. 5. Farizo BA, Louviere JJ, Soliño M (2014) Mixed integration of individual background, attitudes and tastes for landscape management. Land Use Policy 38: 477–486.
  6. 6. North DC (1990) Institutions, institutional change and economic performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  7. 7. Hirsh JB (2010) Personality and environmental concern. J Environ Psychol 30: 245–248.
  8. 8. Milfont TL, Sibley CG (2012) The big five personality traits and environmental engagement: Associations at the individual and societal level. J Environ Psychol 32: 187–195.
  9. 9. Grebitus C, Lusk JL, Nayga RM Jr (2013) Explaining differences in real and hypothetical experimental auctions and choice experiments with personality. J Environ Psychol 36: 11–26.
  10. 10. Morey E, Thacher J, Breffle W (2006) Using angler characteristics and attitudinal data to identify environmental preference classes: A latent-class model. Environ Resour Econ 34: 91–115.
  11. 11. Johnston RJ (2007) Choice experiments, site similarity and benefits transfer. Environ Resour Econ 38: 331–351.
  12. 12. Breffle WB, Morey ER, Thacher JA (2011) A joint latent-class model: Combining likert-scale preference statements with choice data to harvest preference heterogeneity. Environ Resour Econ 50: 83–110.
  13. 13. Choi AS, Fielding KS (2013) Environmental attitudes as WTP predictors: A case study involving endangered species. Ecol Econ 89: 24–32.
  14. 14. Nocella G, Boecker A, Hubbard L, Scarpa R (2012) Eliciting consumer preferences for certified animal-friendly foods: Can elements of the theory of planned behavior improve choice experiment analysis?. Psychology and Marketing 29(11): 850–868.
  15. 15. López-Mosquera N, Sánchez M (2013) Direct and indirect effects of received benefits and place attachment in willingness to pay and loyalty in suburban natural areas. J Environ Psychol 34: 27–35.
  16. 16. Rammstedt B, John OP (2007) Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the big five inventory in English and German. J Res Pers 41: 203–212.
  17. 17. Costa PT Jr, McCrae RR (1985) The NEO personality inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychol. Assess. Resources.
  18. 18. Robins RW, Hendin HM, Trzesniewski KH (2001) Measuring global self-esteem: Construct validation of a single-item measure and the Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Pers Soc Psychol B 27: 151–161.
  19. 19. Rammstedt B, Rammsayer T (2002) Gender differences in self-estimated intelligence and their relation to gender-role orientation. Eur J Personality 16: 369–382.
  20. 20. Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB Jr (2003) A very brief measure of the big-five personality domains. J Res Pers 37: 504–528.
  21. 21. Digman JM (1990) Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annu Rev Psychol 41: 417–440.
  22. 22. John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ (2008) Paradigm shift to the integrative big five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New York: Guilford Press 114–158.
  23. 23. Train K (2003) Discrete choice methods with simulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  24. 24. Vermunt JK, Magidson J (2005) Technical guide for Latent Gold Choice 4.0: Basic and advanced. Belmont Massachusetts: Statistical Innovations Inc.
  25. 25. Boxall PC, Adamowicz WL (2002) Understanding heterogeneous preferences in random utility models: A latent class approach. Environ Resour Econ 23(4): 421–446.
  26. 26. Greene WH, Hensher DA (2003) A latent class model for discrete choice analysis: Contrasts with mixed logit. Transport Res B-Meth 37(8): 681–698.
  27. 27. Birol E, Karousakis K, Koundouri P (2006) Using a choice experiment to account for preference heterogeneity in wetland attributes: The case of Cheimaditida wetland in Greece. Ecol Econ 60(1): 145–166.
  28. 28. Street DJ, Burgess L (2007) The construction of optimal stated choice experiments. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.
  29. 29. Louviere JJ, Hensher DA, Swait JD (2000) Stated choice methods: Analysis and application. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  30. 30. Almund M, Duckworth AL, Heckman J, Kautz T (2011) Personality psychology and economics. In: Hanushek EA, Machin SJ, Woessmann L, editors. Handbook of the economics of education volume 4. Amsterdam: North Holland. 1–181.
  31. 31. Dunlap RE, Van Liere KD, Mertig AG, Jones RE (2000) Measuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: A revised NEP scale. J Soc Issues 56: 425–442.
  32. 32. Schwartz SH (1992) Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In: Zanna M, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology 25. New York: Academic Press. 1–65.
  33. 33. Schwartz SH (1994) Are there universal aspects in the structure and content of human values? J Soc Issues 50: 19–45.
  34. 34. Lee JA, Soutar GN, Louviere JJ (2008) An alternative approach to measuring Schwartz's values: The best-worst scaling approach. J Pers Assess 90(4): 335–347.
  35. 35. Hess S, Daly A (2013) Choice modelling: The state of the art and the state of practice. Chelthenham, UK/Massachusetts, USA: Edward Elgar.