Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Reduced Sleep-Like Quiescence in Both Hyperactive and Hypoactive Mutants of the Galphaq Gene egl-30 during lethargus in Caenorhabditis elegans

Abstract

Sleep-like states are characterized by massively reduced behavioral activity. Little is known about genetic control of sleep-like behavior. It is also not clear how general activity levels during wake-like behavior influence activity levels during sleep-like behavior. Mutations that increase wake-like activity are generally believed to also increase activity during sleep-like behavior and mutations that decrease wake-like activity are believed to have decreased activity during sleep-like behavior. We studied sleep-like behavior during lethargus in larvae of Caenorhabditis elegans. We looked through a small set of known mutants with altered activity levels. As expected, mutants with increased activity levels typically showed less sleep-like behavior. Among these hyperactive mutants was a gain-of-function mutant of the conserved heterotrimeric G protein subunit Galphaq gene egl-30. We found, however, that an unusual semidominant hypoactive mutant of egl-30 also had reduced sleep-like behavior. While movement was severely reduced and impaired in the semidominant egl-30 mutant, sleep-like behavior was severely reduced: the semidominant egl-30 mutant lacked prolonged periods of complete immobility, reduced spontaneous neural activity less, and reduced responsiveness to stimulation less. egl-30 is a well-known regulator of behavior. Our results suggest that egl-30 controls not only general activity levels, but also differences between wake-like and sleep-like behavior.

Introduction

Sleep is a complex behavioral state that is found in mammals. But also all other animals that have been carefully studied, display quiescence behavior. Several of these quiescence behaviors fulfill behavioral criteria that define sleep in mammals such as an absence of voluntary movement, reversibility, increased arousal threshold, assumption of a specific posture, homeostatic regulation, and changes in the nervous system [1]. If quiescent behavior fulfills these criteria, it is often called a sleep-like state or sleep-like behavior [2]. It seems like these sleep-like states are much less complex compared with sleep in mammals and it is unclear how sleep and sleep-like states are evolutionarily related. Quiescent behavior can also be found in C. elegans. Larvae go through four larval stages called L1 to L4. At the end of each larval stage, animals molt. Before ecdysis, larvae go through a phase of quiescent behavior lasting two hours that is called lethargus [3]. Recent work has shown that quiescence behavior during lethargus fulfills behavioral criteria that define sleep-like behavior such as absence of voluntary movement, reversibility, increased arousal threshold, a specific posture, homeostatic regulation, and changes in the nervous system [2,4-7].

Unlike sleep in mammals, quiescence behavior in C. elegans is not controlled by a circadian rhythm, but by the molting cycle. In the presence of food, the molting cycle has a periodicity of 8-10 hours, which is shorter than the circadian cycle. Interestingly, the period gene that controls circadian rhythms in other systems has a C. elegans homolog called lin-42. Like the period gene, lin-42 mRNA levels oscillate with the wake-sleep-like cycle [8]. Knockout of lin-42 causes defects in the timing of the molting cycle and the timing of the sleep-like behavior [9]. Thus, circadian rhythm genes control the molting cycle in C. elegans.

Several pathways have already been implicated in the control of sleep-like behavior during lethargus. One of the first mutants found was a gain-of-function mutant in egl-4, a cyclic nucleotide dependent kinase. egl-4gf mutants show ectopic phases of quiescence that are characterized by an absence of locomotion and pharyngeal pumping [2,10]. Ectopic sleep-like quiescence has also been achieved by overexpression of either lin-3 or osm-11, implicating Epidermal Growth Factor signaling and Notch signaling in the control of sleep-like quiescence [11,12]. In addition to the conditions that increase sleep-like behavior, also mutants with decreased sleep-like behavior have been described. Acy-1 gain-of-function mutants are hyperactive and sleep-like behavior is interrupted by phases of high activity [2,6]. Mutation of components of the EGF pathway, such as let-23 or plc-3, does not abolish quiescence, but quiescence is often punctuated by bursts of activity [11]. Also, loss-of-function mutants of the Notch signaling pathway have reduced quiescence [12].

egl-30 is a known regulator of neural function and also of many behaviors such as pharyngeal pumping and locomotion [13]. One of its major functions in neurons appears to be facilitation of neurotransmitter release [14]. Both hypomorphic loss-of-function mutations and semidominant mutation in egl-30 were shown to severely reduce behavioral activity levels [13,15,16].

Here we found that an unusual semidominant mutant in egl-30 was lacking substantial periods of immobility during lethargus. We characterized sleep-like quiescence behavior during L1 lethargus in the semidominant allele egl-30(n715sd), and compared it with the gain-of-function allele egl-30(tg26gf) [17]. We assayed spontaneous movement, neural and muscle calcium activity, and responsiveness to stimulation and found that both a hyperactive and a hypoactive mutant of egl-30 have reduced sleep-like behavior during L1 lethargus.

Materials and Methods

Worm maintenance and strains used

C. elegans was maintained on NGM plates as described [18]. After bombardment into unc-119(ed 3), all insertions described below were backcrossed two times against N2 to remove unc-119(ed 3). The following strains and alleles were used in addition to the strains specified in Figure 1B:

thumbnail
Figure 1. Quiescence in mutants with known alterations of activity levels.

A Correlation between movement during wake-like and sleep-like states in wild type and in several mutant strains. Each black dot represents one strain. We filmed worms with a frame rate of 0.2/s. The red line represents a linear fit. B Strains assayed for the analysis in A. Values displayed show mean ± SEM. ***denotes statistical significance from wild type (two-sample t-test, p<0.001) C-G individual examples of nose speed measurements for one wild type (C), one egl-30(tg26gf) (D), one egl-30(n715sd) (E), one acy-1(pk1279), and one unc-13(e51) example worm (G). The sleep-like state as defined by the absence of feeding is shown in light blue. A black arrow indicates the timing of the cuticle shedding (molt). The beginning of lethargus was arbitrarily set to 0h.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075853.g001

  1. N2: wild type
  2. CG21: egl-30(tg26) I; him-5(e1490) V.
  3. MT1434: egl-30(n686) I.
  4. MT2609: egl-30(n715n1190) I.
  5. DA823: egl-30(ad805) I.
  6. DA1084: egl-30(ad806) I.
  7. HBR352: egl-30(tg26) I. (Created from CG21 by backcrossing 2x into wild type to remove the him background, this strain was used for all tg26 experiments).
  8. MT1520: egl-30(n715) I.
  9. HBR460: egl-30(n715) I,goeIs42[pegl-30::egl30cDNA-mkate2::egl-30-3'utr,unc-119(+)].
  10. HBR569: egl-30(n715) I,goeEx279[myo-3::egl30cDNA-mkate2::egl-30-3'utr,unc-122::rfp,unc-119(+)].
  11. HBR602: egl-30(n715) I,goeIs125[pmyo-3::egl30cDNA-mkate2::egl-30-3'utr,unc-119(+)].
  12. HBR604: egl-30(n715) I,goeIs126[pmyo-3::egl30cDNA-mkate2::egl-30-3'utr,unc-119(+)].
  13. HBR599: egl-30(n715) I,goeIs127[punc-119::egl30cDNA-mkate2::egl-30-3'utr,unc-119(+)].
  14. HBR600: egl-30(n715) I,goeIs128[punc-119::egl30cDNA-mkate2::egl-30-3'utr,unc-119(+)].
  15. HBR4: goeIs3[pmyo-3::gcamp3.35::unc-54-3'utr, unc-119(+)] V.
  16. HBR235: egl-30(tg26) I, goeIs3[pmyo-3::gcamp3.35::unc-54-3'utr, unc-119(+)] V.
  17. HBR418: egl-30(n715) I, goeIs3[pmyo-3::gcamp3.35::unc-54-3'utr, unc-119(+)] V.
  18. HBR204: goeIs24[punc-119::sl1::gcamp3.35::sl2::mkate2-unc-54-3’utr, unc-119(+)].
  19. HBR213: egl-30(n715) I, goeIs24[punc-119::sl1::gcamp3.35::sl2::mkate2-unc-54-3’utr, unc-119(+)].
  20. HBR220: egl-30(tg26) I, goeIs24[punc-119::sl1::gcamp3.35::sl2::mkate2-unc-54-3’utr, unc-119(+)].
  21. HBR205: goeIs22[pmec-4::sl1::gcamp3.35::sl2::mkate2-unc-54-3’utr, unc-119(+)].
  22. HBR216: egl-30(tg26) I, goeIs22[pmec-4::sl1::gcamp3.35::sl2::mkate2-unc-54-3’utr, unc-119(+)].
  23. HBR206: egl-30(n715) I, goeIs22[pmec-4::sl1::gcamp3.35::sl2::mkate2-unc-54-3’utr, unc-119(+)].
  24. AQ2026: ljIs105[sra-6::chr2::yfp, unc-122::gfp].
  25. HBR6: egl-30(tg26) I, ljIs105[sra-6::chr2::yfp, unc-122::gfp].
  26. HBR7: egl-30(n715) I, ljIs105[sra-6::chr2::yfp, unc-122::gfp].

Microcompartment culture

Agarose microcompartment culture was carried out as described [19]. We used 190µm x 190µm compartments. We previously tested whether behavioral parameters change with increasing the microcompartment dimensions and we did not find any differences [19]. We used the slightly smaller version of the microcompartments because they allowed fluorescence imaging [19]. To obtain eggs containing mutants carrying egl-30(n715sd), gravid adults were placed into a 10µl drop of S-Basal placed on a glass coverslip and were cut open using needles, which released the eggs from the animals. The egg suspension was transferred to a plate containing NGM seeded with OP 50 bacteria. After absorption of the liquid into the agar animals were picked with a platinum wire into the microcompartments.

Behavioral observations and calcium measurements

Filming in microcompartments is described in reference [19]. For screening and bout analysis we used time-lapse movies with 5s interval. A bout of immobility was defined as no detectable movement lasting at least 5s. To precisely measure nose speed we used burst type protocols: every 15min we took 40 frames with an interval of 0.5s. Nose speeds were found empirically to be a sensitive measure for quiescence. Both dorsoventral head movements known as foraging and locomotion contributed to nose speed. The spatial resolution of the images was 0.4 µm per pixel. A person that was not blind to the phenotype did the manual tracking of the worm nose using Andor IQ2 software. For averaging data from burst movies, we pooled all burst movies collected during lethargus for each individual worm before averaging across different individuals. The averaged data from one pool of burst movies obtained from one individual animal was regarded as one N. Similarly, we pooled burst movies collected during wake-like behavior for each individual worm before averaging across different individuals. The period of wake-like behavior was much longer than the lethargus period. Thus, we did not use the entire L1 wake-like period for analysis but selected the last ten bursts that were preceding the sleep-like phase. We raised all worms at a temperature of 15°C. The microscope room was temperature-controlled to 19°C using air conditioning. During movie acquisition, we used a lid to prevent drying of the hydrogel microcompartment. The lid was heated to 25.5°C to prevent fogging. The culture temperature was measured with a pt1000 resistance thermometer and was 23°C. After placing pretzel stage eggs of N2 or egl-30(tg26gf) mutants into the microcompartments, we waited four to six hours before we started data acquisition. Because egl-30(n715sd) mutants were egg laying defective and larvae developed more slowly than wild type, we cut open gravid hermaphrodite worms in a drop of S-Basal and placed the eggs onto a fresh NGM plate 12h before preparing the microcompartments. We then transferred freshly hatched L1 larvae into the microcompartments. In addition to staging worms according to their age after hatching, the cuticles of larvae were checked for L1-specific alae. To identify lethargus, we manually scored pharyngeal pumping in bright field or DIC movies acquired at each data point. Shedding of the cuticle was visually identified, and followed each lethargus period. Complete shedding of the cuticle was defined as the completion of the molt. While the growth rate of egl-30(n715sd) worms was reduced, we were able to clearly identify L1 lethargus in every individual worm based on the above criteria. No worms were censored due to error in picking them at the right stage of lethargus. N2 and egl-30(tg26gf) worms were kept inside microcompartments and were imaged for 20h. egl-30(n715sd) mutants were filmed for 40h. 10% of egl-30(n715sd) worms arrested or died during the experiment and did not progress through the molt. We discarded the data for these animals. For aligning the data from several animals, the beginning of pumping cessation was used and was arbitrarily set to 0h. Completion of cuticle shedding was defined as the “molt” in plots. The lethargus was defined as the period of continuous absence of pumping lasting at least 1.5h, followed by shedding of the cuticle. Pumping was scored by visually scoring pharyngeal pumping and pharyngeal structure that appears clear during lethargus. Neural GCaMP3.35 expressing animals, calcium measurements and mechanical stimulation are described in reference [4]. For determining the maximum amplitude of the stimulus-induced calcium transient, first the amplitude for each individual worm was determined and then data from several worms were averaged. Because the maximum of the transient occurred at slightly different times after stimulation in different individual worms, the calculated maximum amplitude appeared slightly higher than the maximum amplitude on the plot showing the averaged data for each time point. For quantification of GCaMP3.35 signals we computed ΔF/F values. F was calculated as the normalized average fluorescence signal of the 2.5h preceding lethargus. Waking intensity before stimulation was used as F for evoked neural activity. Pan-neural signals were analyzed by cutting out a region of interest containing the head region. ALM signals were analyzed by cutting out a small region of interest containing the neuron. As background served a region that did not contain neural signal. For analysis of evoked calcium transients we looked at intensity signals in PLM, because it was easier to keep PLM in focus. Muscle GCaMP3.35 calcium measurements and curvature analysis are described in reference [5].

Rescue construct

We made a synthetic mkate2-tagged version of egl-30. We amplified the putative promoter and 3’ untranslated region of egl-30 using the following primers:

  1. Promoter
  2. Fwd: GGGG ACA ACT TTG TAT AGA AAA GTT GCC GAA AAC AAT GGA AAG AAG CAT TA
  3. Rev.: GGGG AC TGC TTT TTT GTA CAA ACT TGG GCG GCC GAA AAG GTG CCA C
  4. UTR
  5. Fwd: GGGG ACA GCT TTC TTG TAC AAA GTG GGA AGA AGT CGC ATG TCG GAT TG
  6. Rev.: GGGG AC AAC TTT GTA TAA TAA AGT TGG CAT TTAG CTA TAT CAG AAA GCA C

For cloning we used the Multi-Site Gateway Three-Fragment Vector Construction Kit (Invitrogen). After bombardment into unc-119(ed 3), goeIs42 (as well as the other rescue insertions) was first backcrossed two times in to N2 and was then crossed into egl-30(n715sd) afterwards using the hT2 balancer. For muscle and pan-neuronal rescue we obtained more than one transgenic line. We averaged the data from the different transgenic lines: For pan-neuronal rescue we pooled data from HBR599(N = 10) and HBR600(N = 6) and for muscle rescue we pooled data from HBR569(N = 8), HBR602(N = 6), and HBR604(N = 6).

Channelrhodopsin experiments

All trans retinal (ATR, Sigma) was added to the sample. ATR was soaked into the agarose after its solidification. For control experiments that did not contain retinal, only vehicle (50% ethanol) was soaked into the agar. The volume of the agar was 1ml. We added 10µl of ATR solution to the sample so that the final concentration was 250µM. The final concentration of ethanol was 0.5%. Blue light (100 pulses with 15ms duration with an interval of 70ms) was delivered every 30 minutes using a 495nm LED (CoolLED) coupled to the fluorescence port of the microscope. LED power was measured with a light voltmeter. LED intensity scaled approximately linearly with light density. 10% LED intensity corresponded to 0.5mW/mm2. We performed the experiment with LED intensities of 0%, 2%, 5%, 10% 20%, 50%, and 100%. Backward movement of animals was scored manually. A worm was scored as responding, if it moved at least 5µm backwards upon stimulation. Because the movement of egl-30(n715sd) animals was uncoordinated, we scored the presence or absence of backwards movement but not the quality of backwards movements. Each worm was tested four (wild type and egl-30(tg26gf)) to eight (egl-30(n715sd)) times during each sleep-like and wake-like behavior. The measurements for both sleep-like and wake-like behavior were averaged for each individual worm to obtain one N. The experiment was performed with N = 5 worms for each LED condition and genotype. Half-maximum responsiveness was interpolated using Origin software.

Statistical tests and data display

For statistical analysis all data points during the sleep-like states were averaged and compared with averaged data points of the beforehand wake-like state (2-2.5 hours). For statistical tests comparing sleep-like and wake-like behavior, Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests (two tailed) were performed using Origin software. For statistical tests comparing mutants with wild type t-tests were performed using Origin software. Analysis of Variance for Simple Linear Regression (ANOVA, Figure 1A) was used for testing the correlation of wake-like and sleep-like activity in different strains. We used Iglewicz and Hoaglin’s robust test to test the reduction of velocity during sleep-like state compared to wake-like state for multiple outliers. Values were considered as potential outliers, when the modified Z-score was greater than 3.5 [20]. For displaying micrographs we applied false colors to the grayscale image with the standard color map “jet” in Matlab.

Results

Wake-like and sleep-like activity in mutants with altered activity levels

By definition, a sleep-like state is separated from a wake-like state based on differences in behavioral activity. Behavioral activity is massively reduced during the sleep-like state relative to the wake-like state [1,2,6,12,19]. Intuitively, increased activity during wake-like behavior is expected to also cause increased activity during sleep-like behavior: publications describing mutants with altered sleep or sleep-like behavior often also contain measures of general activity levels to assay the specificity of the mutation. Whereas hyperactivity during wake often correlates with decreased sleep, some mutants have been found that have reduced sleep but normal activity levels during wake [2,11,12,21,22]. Using a similar argument, one may expect mutants with generally decreased activity levels during wake-like behavior to also have decreased activity during sleep-like behavior.

In C. elegans, mutants have been found that have generally altered behavioral activity levels [23]. We wanted to test whether hyperactivity is typically associated with reduced sleep-like behavior and whether hypoactivity is typically associated with increased sleep-like behavior. Rather than looking at absolute behavioral activity measures of mutants, we compared their sleep-like behavior with their wake-like behavior and selected those mutants that displayed little difference between these behaviors. We selected an arbitrary and non-exhaustive set of strains that were already described to have altered behavioral activity levels (Figure 1) [10,13,17,23-37]. To analyze these mutants, we filmed the first wake and sleep-like cycle inside microfluidic culture compartments with a frame rate of 0.2 per second and quantified movement by measuring nose speed [19]. We identified L1 lethargus based on two criteria: firstly, pumping cessation lasted at least 1.5h and secondly, shedding of the cuticle followed pumping cessation. Because all of the strains showed cessation of pumping and went through the molt, we could identify lethargus in all mutant strains using these criteria. We compared nose speed during the wake-like state with nose-speed during the sleep-like state. We found that nose speed was strongly reduced during sleep-like behavior in most mutants. The average relative nose speed during the sleep-like state was 60% of the wake-like state. Nose speed during wake-like behavior generally correlated with nose speed during sleep-like behavior (Pearson’s R: 0.79, p < 0.001, ANOVA).

In three mutants, activity levels during sleep-like behavior were higher than 80% of the wake-like state, and were significantly different from wild type according to a two-sample t-test (p < 0.01, still significant after Bonferroni correction). Among these mutants were two hyperactive mutants and one severely hypoactive mutant. One of the hyperactive genes was a hypermorphic gain-of-function allele of egl-30(tg26gf) [17]. The other one was a complete-loss-of-function allele of goa-1(sa734) [24]. The hypoactive mutant was an allele of the conserved heterotrimeric G protein alpha q subunit gene egl-30(n715sd) [13,16]. egl-30(n715sd) was originally described as a semidominant allele that may be antimorphic: Like egl-30 loss-of-function alleles, egl-30(715sd) showed reduced locomotion and egg-laying. Worms that were heterozygous for egl-30(715sd) also showed reduced locomotion and egg-laying, but the phenotypes were less severe than in homozygous mutants. Thus, egl-30(n715sd) may not be a typical hypomorphic loss-of-function allele [15,16]. egl-30(n715sd) and goa-1(sa734) were significant outliers according to Iglewicz and Hoaglin’s robust test [20]. However, the interpretation of these values as outliers is difficult because they are at the extreme ends of the population.

The findings from this analysis suggested that nose speed during wake and sleep-like behavior typically correlated: increased movement during wake-like behavior was associated with increased movement during sleep-like behavior in most mutants. However, we found one hypoactive mutant with less pronounced activity differences between wake and sleep-like behavior. These results suggest a role for egl-30 in regulating activity differences between wake-like behavior and sleep-like behavior. egl-30 is a known regulator of neural function and also of many behaviors such as pharyngeal pumping and locomotion [13].

We further characterized wake-like and sleep-like behavior in both the semidominant hypoactive egl-30(n715sd) mutant as well as the hypermorphic hyperactive egl-30(tg26gf) mutant. We could not study an egl-30 null allele, because complete-loss-of-function of egl-30 is lethal [13].

egl-30 mutants have reduced sleep-like immobility

Complete immobility is a hallmark of sleep-like quiescence during lethargus in C. elegans [2,3]. The nose moved constantly during wake-like behavior, even when the worms were dwelling and did not show long-range locomotion. We thus quantified nose immobility as an approximation for complete immobility: Immobility was defined as no detectable movement of the nose. During wild type sleep-like behavior, larvae were totally immobile for 19% of the sleep-like behavior and bouts of complete quiescence lasted on average 15s (Figure 2A,B). During sleep-like behavior in hyperactive egl-30(tg26gf) mutants, full immobility was seen for only 6% of the sleep-like behavior and bouts of complete quiescence lasted on average only 7s (Figure 2A,B). In hypoactive egl-30(n715sd) mutants, immobility was present only 4% of the sleep-like behavior and bouts of complete quiescence lasted only 6s (Figure 2A,B). Because lethargus and the general development of egl-30(n715sd) lasted twice as long as wild type or egl-30(tg26gf) (Figure 2D,E), we integrated immobility over the entire lethargus period. The integrated immobility of egl-30 mutants was also substantially reduced compared with wild type (Figure 2C). Thus, complete immobility was severely reduced in egl-30 mutants.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Quiescence bout lengths in egl-30 mutants.

A Frequency distribution of quiescence bout durations for wild type and egl-30 mutants calculated as the fraction of lethargus length (or a time window of 2h during wake-like behavior). A bout was defined as immobility lasting 5s or longer. B Quantification of total immobility ≥ 5s calculated as a fraction of lethargus length (or a time window of 2h during wake-like behavior). C Cumulative immobility ≥ 5s integrated over the entire lethargus phase (or a time window of the same length during wake-like behavior). D Mean lethargus duration in hours as defined by the absence of pharyngeal pumping. E Mean development time in hours from 3-fold embryo until L2 larvae in wild type and egl-30 mutants. Error bar represent SEM. A paired sample Wilcoxon test was used for statistical testing of wake-like versus sleep-like behavior. T-test was used for comparing different genotypes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075853.g002

Nose speed distribution in egl-30 mutants

egl-30 loss-of function mutants generally move very little and we wanted to quantify the small movements of the nose as precisely as possible. We thus filmed the egl-30 mutants again and increased the frame rate to two per second. To reduce data amounts to a manageable amount, we filmed the worms discontinuously using a burst mode every 15 minutes for twenty seconds. Probably, burst-type measurements were more precise than the five-second-interval protocol because it less likely missed small movements. We calculated a probability distribution for all nose speeds during sleep-like and wake-like behavior.

During wild type sleep-like behavior, average nose speed was reduced by 78% compared with wake-like behavior (Figure 3A). During sleep-like behavior in hyperactive egl-30(tg26gf) mutants, worms reduced their movement by only 26% (Figure 3B). Average nose speed during sleep-like behavior in hypoactive egl-30(n715sd) mutants was reduced by only 3% (Figure 3C). Nose speed in acy-1(pk1279) and unc-13(e51) was clearly reduced during sleep-like behavior, indicating that nose speed reduction can be detected in hypoactive mutants (Figure 3G,H). The statistics for this experiment can be found in Figure S1. The more precise nose speed measurement showed that during sleep-like behavior, hypoactive egl-30(n715sd) mutants have higher nose mobility than wild type.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Probability distribution of nose speed in egl-30 mutants, egl-30 rescue mutants and hypoactive mutants during wake-like and sleep-like behavior.

Probability distribution of nose speeds during wake-like and sleep-like state. A wild type, B egl-30(tg26gf), C egl-30(n715sd), D egl-30(n715sd) transformed with a rescue construct expressed from its endogenous promoter, E egl-30(n715sd) transformed with a pan-neural rescue construct, F egl-30(n715sd) transformed with a muscle rescue construct, G acy-1(pk1279lf) and H unc-13(e51lf). We can exclude that the rescue of egl-30(n715sd) was caused by the myo-3 or unc-119 promoter regions, because transgenic worms that contained these promoters without the egl-30 cDNA such as pmyo-3::gcamp (Figure 4) or punc-119::gcamp (Figure 5) did not show rescue (Figure 4, 5 and data not shown). Complete immobility of the nose was virtually absent in egl-30 mutants, rescued by the egl-30 transgene and partially rescued by the pan-neural rescue construct. Values displayed show mean nose speed ± SEM. A paired sample Wilcoxon test was used for statistical testing of wake-like versus sleep-like behavior.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075853.g003

Rescue of the egl-30 semidominant phenotype

We wanted to test whether the sleep-like state phenotype in egl-30(n715sd) was actually caused by the mutation in egl-30 or by a background mutation. To do this we tested for rescue of the mutant phenotype using an mkate2-tagged version of egl-30. mkate2 encodes a far-red fluorescent protein [38] and was codon-optimized for expression in C. elegans using codon adapter [39]. The tagging was done according to an established strategy into an internal loop of the G protein [40]. The insertion site was analogous to that of a published, functional gfp-tagged goa-1 transgene [41]. egl-30(n715sd) mutants carrying the egl-30::mkate2 rescue construct showed a clear reduction of nose speed during sleep-like behavior of 66% compared with the wake-like behavior and pronounced immobility during sleep-like behavior (Figure 3D). Because the egl-30::mkate2 transgene rescued the egl-30(n715sd) phenotype we conclude that the phenotype is actually caused by egl-30(n715sd).

egl-30 is expressed in both muscle and neurons [13]. To test in which tissue expression of egl-30 is required for sleep-like state control, we expressed the egl-30::mkate2 transgene in either body wall muscles using a myo-3 promoter or in the entire nervous system using an unc-119 promoter [42,43]. egl-30(n715sd) mutants carrying the punc-119::egl-30::mkate2 rescue construct showed a strong rescue in activity levels, and also a clear reduction of nose speed during sleep-like behavior of 49% (Figure 3E). egl-30(n715sd) mutants carrying the pmyo-3::egl-30::mkate2 rescue construct showed an increase in general activity levels, but only showed a small reduction of nose speed during sleep-like behavior of 12% (Figure 3F). The statistics for this experiment can be found in Figure S1. Thus, we could partially rescue the locomotion and sleep-like phenotype of egl-30(n715sd) by expressing egl-30 only in the nervous system, and we could partially rescue the locomotion phenotype but not the sleep-like state phenotype by expressing egl-30 only in muscle. We conclude that egl-30 acts mostly in the nervous system to control sleep-like behavior.

Reduced muscle relaxation in egl-30 mutants during sleep-like behavior

During lethargus sleep-like behavior, C. elegans reduce their body wall muscle activity and assume a relaxed posture [5,6]. Do egl-30 mutants also relax their muscles and assume a typical posture? To measure muscle activity we expressed GCaMP3.35 in striated body wall muscle cells and filmed worms during wake and sleep-like behavior (Figure 4A). We quantified GCaMP3.35 intensity and quantified postures by measuring the angle change along the body axis. We found that during wild type lethargus, GCaMP3.35 ΔF/F was reduced by 17% and that average angle change was reduced by 20% (Figure 4B,C). During egl-30(tg26gf) lethargus, GCaMP3.35 ΔF/F was not reduced and average angle change was reduced by only 3% (Figure 4B,C). During egl-30(n715sd) lethargus, GCaMP3.35 ΔF/F was reduced by only 1% and average angle change was reduced by 5% (Figure 4B,C). This indicated that egl-30 mutants relaxed their muscles less during lethargus and assumed a less pronounced sleep-specific elongated posture compared with wild type.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Reduced muscle relaxation in egl-30 mutants during sleep-like behavior.

A False color fluorescence images of wild type and egl-30 mutants expressing GCaMP3.35 in body wall muscle during the wake-like state (top) and during the sleep-like state (bottom). Bright signals reflect muscle activity. The angle change is displayed for each example worm. B Mean angle change in wild type and egl-30 mutants during wake-like and sleep-like state. C Reduction of muscle calcium signals in wild type and egl-30 mutants. Note the reduction of angle change and muscle calcium during sleep-like behavior in wild type. Body curvature and muscle calcium are much less reduced in egl-30 mutants. Error bars represent SEM. For each strain ≥ 10 animals were tested (two-sample t-test for comparisons between different genotypes, paired sample Wilcoxon test for comparison of wake-like and sleep-like behavior, *** denotes statistical significance at p<0.001)..

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075853.g004

Reduced decrease of spontaneous neural activity in egl-30 mutants during sleep-like behavior

During C. elegans lethargus, spontaneous neural activity is reduced globally as well as in individual sensory neurons [4,6]. How does spontaneous neural activity change during lethargus in egl-30 mutants? We measured neural activity using transgenic animals expressing GCaMP3.35 either in all neurons or in mechanosensitive neurons and quantified GCaMP3.35 intensity. During lethargus in wild type animals, pan-neural GCaMP3.35 ΔF/F was reduced by 14% and mechanosensitive neuron GCaMP3.35 ΔF/F was reduced by 9% (Figure 5 A, B). During lethargus in egl-30(tg26gf) mutants, global GCaMP3.35 intensity was not measurably reduced and ALM intensity was reduced by only 6% (Figure 5 C, D). During lethargus in egl-30(n715sd) mutants, global GCaMP3.35 intensity was reduced by only 7% and ALM intensity was reduced by only 1% (Figure 5 E, F). We conclude that spontaneous neural activity in egl-30 mutants was less reduced during lethargus compared with wild type.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Reduced decrease of spontaneous neural activity in egl-30 mutants during sleep-like behavior.

A Spontaneous pan-neural activity was reduced during wild type lethargus. B Spontaneous ALM activity was reduced during wild type lethargus. C Spontaneous pan-neural activity and D ALM activity in egl-30(tg26gf) was reduced less than in wild type during lethargus. E Spontaneous pan-neural activity and F ALM activity in egl-30(n715sd) was reduced less than in wild type during lethargus. Error bars represent SEM. The beginning of lethargus was arbitrarily set to 0h. Baseline fluorescence during the two hours of wake-like behavior preceding lethargus was set to zero. Statistical test used was Wilcoxon Signed Ranks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075853.g005

Responsiveness of the nociceptive ASH circuit in egl-30 mutants during sleep-like behavior

During sleep-like behavior in C. elegans, responsiveness to stimulation is reduced [2]. Nociceptive stimulation is sensed mainly by the polymodal ASH neuron [44]. Optogenetic activation of ASH causes backwards movement [45]. How is the responsiveness of the ASH nociceptive circuit altered in egl-30 mutants? We optogenetically stimulated ASH and measured the behavioral response. We used transgenic worms that expressed Channelrhodopsin2 under the control of the sra-6 promoter. This promoter drives strong expression in ASH but also in PVQ and weakly in ASI [46]. The backwards movement that is induced by blue-light stimulation of psra-6::Channelrhodopsin2-expressing animals was shown to be caused by activation of ASH [45,47].

Worms were cultured inside microfluidic microcompartments from the early L1 stage until the L2 stage and were filmed repeatedly using a burst protocol similar to the burst protocols described above. Short movies were collected every 30 minutes. During each burst movie, worms were exposed to blue LED light of a defined intensity. Movies were analyzed by manually scoring backwards movement. In order to obtain a dose response relationship for backwards movement and LED intensity, the experiment was repeated several times with different LED intensities. For each LED intensity a set of different individual worms was assayed. The fraction of larvae responding generally increased with LED intensity. We plotted the fraction of events in which worms showed backwards movements during LED illumination against the LED intensity and used the resulting dose response curve to estimate the LED intensity required for backwards movement in 50% of illumination events. We defined this value as the response threshold of the nociceptive ASH circuit.

During wake-like behavior in wild type the response threshold was 1%. During sleep-like behavior in wild type the response threshold was 6%. At LED powers of 20% or higher, larvae during both wake and sleep-like behavior responded. Thus, during sleep-like behavior, the response threshold was increased (Figure 6A). Control animals that were raised without all-trans-retinal also showed some responsiveness to the LED stimulation, albeit much less than in the presence of all-trans-retinal (Figure 6B). egl-30(tg26gf) mutants were generally more sensitive to stimulation: During wake-like behavior the response threshold was 0.3%, during sleep-like behavior the response threshold was 0.7% (Figure 6C). egl-30(n715sd) mutants were less sensitive to stimulation: During wake-like behavior the response threshold was 2%, during sleep-like behavior the response threshold was 1% (Figure 6D).

thumbnail
Figure 6. Reduced decrease of sensory responsiveness during sleep-like behavior in egl-30 mutants.

A Light dose-dependent fraction of wild type worms reversing upon optogenetic ASH activation. Note that worms need higher LED intensity for half-maximal responsiveness during sleep-like behavior. B Wild type control without retinal. C Light dose-dependent fraction of worms reversing upon ASH activation during wake-like and sleep-like states in egl-30(tg26gf). D Light dose-dependent fraction of worms reversing upon ASH activation during wake-like and sleep-like states in egl-30(n715sd). Note that the intensity required for half-maximal responsiveness is not reduced during lethargus in egl-30 mutants. Error bars represent SEM. * represents p < 0.05, Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075853.g006

We tested response thresholds to aversive stimulation using blue light-induced Channelrhodopsin2 activation. The majority of this stimulation is likely to be caused by ASH activation but also included blue-light avoidance [48]. While responsiveness to aversive stimulation was reduced during sleep-like behavior in wild type, responsiveness was not reduced during sleep-like behavior in egl-30 mutants.

Reduced decrease of responsiveness to mechanical stimulation in egl-30 mutants during lethargus

Upon gentle mechanical stimulation, C. elegans typically first undergo a short rapid backwards movement [49]. During the backwards movement, dorsoventral nose speed is reduced [50]. During lethargus, C. elegans have reduced responsiveness to gentle mechanical stimulation [2]. Is reduced responsiveness to stimulation during lethargus altered in egl-30 mutants? We stimulated worms using dish tapping and filmed their behavior before and after stimulation. We cultured mutant and wild type worms in microcompartments. Every 30 minutes we filmed the animals for 20 seconds, then started dish tapping, and filmed the response for another 20 seconds. During the wake-like state, after stimulation, wild type larvae showed the typical early backwards response and suppression of head movements. Stimulation increased nose speed by 87%. During the sleep-like state, after stimulation, wild type larvae almost never responded with a backwards movement. If the nose was slowly moving during lethargus, stimulation typically caused a reduction of this movement. Thus, during lethargus stimulation average nose speed even decreased by 14% (Figure 7A). During wake-like behavior, upon stimulation, egl-30(tg26gf) larvae showed the typical early backwards response and suppression of head movements. Stimulation increased nose speed by 75%. During lethargus, upon stimulation, egl-30(tg26gf) showed a backwards movement that was indistinguishable from the wake-like state: Stimulation increased nose speed by 76% (Figure 7C). egl-30(n715sd) larvae did not show a stimulus-induced backward response. However, upon stimulation, they showed a suppression of lateral head movements. During wake-like behavior, stimulation decreased nose speed by 16%. During lethargus, stimulation decreased nose speed by 21% (Figure 7E). We conclude that egl-30(tg26gf) mutants always responded with a typical backwards movement to stimulation during both wake and lethargus. egl-30(n715sd) mutants always responded to stimulation, albeit with a limited behavioral response. The different behavioral response makes a direct comparison with wild type difficult. Nevertheless, these results are consistent with the idea that behavioral differences between wake and lethargus are reduced in these mutants.

thumbnail
Figure 7. Reduced decrease of responsiveness to mechanical stimulation in egl-30 mutants during lethargus.

A Evoked dish tapping-induced behavioral response was reduced during wild type lethargus. “evoked response” was defined as nose speed during “mild tapping” minus nose speed before tapping (“no tapping”). B Dish tapping-induced calcium transients in mechanosensory neurons were reduced during wild type lethargus. C Evoked behavioral response to dish tapping in egl-30(tg26gf) mutants was not reduced during sleep-like behavior. Compared with wild type, the evoked response of egl-30(tg26gf) was not reduced during wake-like behavior, but was significantly reduced during sleep-like behavior (p < 0.001, t-test). D Evoked calcium transients in mechanosensory neurons were not reduced during lethargus in egl-30(tg26gf). E Evoked behavioral response to dish tapping in egl-30(n715sd) mutants was not reduced during sleep-like behavior. Compared with wild type, the evoked response of egl-30(n715sd) was not reduced during sleep-like behavior, but was significantly reduced during wake-like behavior (p < 0.001, t-test). F Evoked calcium transients in mechanosensory neurons were less reduced during lethargus in egl-30(n715sd). G Slope of the tapping-induced calcium transient in mechanosensory neurons during the first five seconds. While the initial slope was significantly decreased during sleep-like behavior in wild type (paired sample Wilcoxon test), the difference between wild type and egl-30 mutants was not statistically significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075853.g007

Reduced decrease of excitability of a mechanosensory neuron in egl-30 mutants during the sleep-like state

Mechanosensory neurons show a decrease in excitability during lethargus, which corresponds to a decrease in behavioral responsiveness [4]. Is excitability of mechanosensory neurons altered during lethargus in egl-30 mutants? We mechanically stimulated animals during wake and lethargus and measured evoked calcium transients in PLM. We cultured individual worms expressing GCaMP3.35 in microcompartments. Every 30 minutes we stimulated worms by dish tapping and filmed GCaMP3.35 intensity. During lethargus in wild type worms, stimulus-evoked calcium transients were reduced by 53% (Figure 7B,G). During lethargus in egl-30(tg26gf) mutants, transients were not reduced (Figure 7D,G). During lethargus in egl-30(n715sd) mutants, transients were decreased by only 13% (Figure 7F,G). Taken together, stimulation-induced calcium transients were much less reduced during lethargus in egl-30 mutants compared with wild type.

Discussion

Surprisingly, we found a severely hypoactive egl-30(n715sd) mutant that is less immobile during the developmental time sleep-like behavior should occur. Compared with wild type wake-like behavior, egl-30(n715sd) larvae moved very little, and their behavioral and neural responsiveness to stimulation was generally low. The locomotion behavior and sensory responsiveness of egl-30(n715sd) did not change dramatically between lethargus and wake-like behavior.

Both the gain-of-function and the semidominant egl-30 mutants had defects in sleep-like behavior. We do not know why egl-30 mutations result in these complex defects. We can only speculate that egl-30 mediates activating, wake-promoting signaling pathways as well as inhibiting, sleep-promoting signaling pathways.

Additional alleles of egl-30 exist. There are several alleles, including potential null alleles, that produce stronger loss-of-function than egl-30(n715sd) and that result in embryonic lethality. There are several hypomorphic loss-of-function alleles that produce weaker loss-of-function than egl-30(n715sd) judged by their egg-laying and movement phenotypes [13]. We have not characterized any of these mutants in detail because these loss-of-function mutants clearly displayed lethargus quiescence when visually inspected on an NGM plate. Loss-of-function alleles of egl-30 that we inspected and that showed quiescence were egl-30(n686), egl-30(ad805), and egl-30(ad806).

egl-30(n715sd) lack a splice consensus site which results in an EGL-30 protein that is lacking its C-terminus [13]. An intragenic suppressor mutation of egl-30(n715sd), egl-30(n715n1190), rescues the egg-laying phenotype and has a new splice site which results in a protein that has a small deletion but a normal C-terminus [13]. We found that egl-30(n715n1190) also partially rescued the sleep-like behavior phenotype (nose speed during wake-like behavior was 9.4 µm/s and nose speed during sleep-like behavior was 5.2 µm/s, N = 10). Thus, egl-30(n715sd) appears to be a fairly unusual allele: it is the strongest available hypoactive allele of egl-30 that still produces viable offspring. We do not know why egl-30(n715sd) has this unusual phenotype. One hypothesis would be that the C-terminal part of egl-30, which is lacking in egl-30(n715sd), is specifically required for sleep-like behavior control. The truncated protein could inappropriately interact with other molecules. These inappropriate interactions could be masked if normal egl-30 is in excess during rescue experiments. A different hypothesis would be that egl-30 loss-of-function has to be severe enough to produce a detectable defect in sleep-like behavior. Loss-of-function also results in lethality if it is too severe. Thus, egl-30(n715sd) may represent a rare allele that is strong enough to cause sleep-like state defects, yet weak enough not to cause lethality. Because we do not know the exact nature of egl-30(n715sd), care should be taken when interpreting results obtained with this allele.

How does egl-30 relate to other genes that have been implicated in the control of sleep-like behavior in C. elegans? The egl-30 pathway is well understood in C. elegans and additional factors of this pathway are known. Previous work suggested that Galphaq acts with Galphas and Galphao to control locomotion. Galphas acts through cAMP produced by an adenylate cyclase that is encoded by acy-1 and gain-of-function mutant that have increased cAMP signaling have been described [23,51]. cAMP signaling has been implicated in the control of sleep-like behavior in C. elegans: While acy-1gf mutants still show quiescence, this quiescence is interrupted by periods of activity [2,6]. We have measured sleep-like behavior in acy-1gf during L1 lethargus and found that nose activity during sleep-like behavior was higher than in wild type, consistent with previous reports. However, nose speed was strongly reduced during sleep-like behavior compared with wake-like behavior (Figure 1B). Thus, the phenotype of acy-1gf differs from both egl-30gf and egl-30sd, suggesting that Galphas and Galphaq signaling have different roles in sleep-like state regulation. Galphao is encoded by goa-1. Previous studies showed that goa-1 negatively regulates locomotion by inhibiting egl-30. Thus, goa-1 mutants have a hyperactive egl-30 pathway [31,52]. This could explain why the goa-1 null mutant has a similar sleep-like state phenotype as the egl-30 gain-of-function mutation. We only assayed the locomotion phenotype of a goa-1 mutant during lethargus. Previous work assayed response latency to the aversive stimulant octanol and concluded that a goa-1 null mutant has reduced responsiveness to octanol during lethargus [2]. A goa-1 gain-of-function mutation, however, has more sleep-like behavior (Figure 1B). The goa-1 sleep-like phenotypes thus are not the opposite of the egl-30 phenotypes, suggesting that Galphao and Galphaq have overlapping yet distinct roles in sleep-like state regulation. Our initial analysis also included a hypomorphic allele of ric-8, which encodes the guanine nucleotide exchange factor for egl-30 [23,26]. ric-8 mutants were almost as hypoactive as egl-30sd mutants and also had less relative reduction of activity during sleep-like behavior, consistent with a role of egl-30 in sleep-like state regulation. EGL-30 has been shown to activate two downstream effectors, the phospholipase C, EGL-8, and the Rho-GEF domain of TRIO/UNC-73 [14,31]. However, we could not find a sleep-like phenotype in neither egl-8 nor unc-73 mutants (Figure 1B). The pathway that controls sleep-like behavior downstream of egl-30 thus remains unknown. egl-30 may require both pathways redundantly, or may signal through a yet unknown pathway. We could not test worms with mutations in both egl-8 and unc-73, because this condition is embryonic lethal [31].

Is there any relationship between genetic control of C. elegans sleep-like behavior and sleep-like states in other animals? We do not know the answer yet. But it is interesting that Galphaq is highly conserved and serves a role not only in C. elegans neurons but also in vertebrate brains. In mammals, thalamocortical relay neurons display different modes of firing [53,54], thought to control signal transmission during sleep and wake and arousal [55]. Firing mode change can be achieved by a small shift in membrane potential [53,54]. In one report using brain slices, this small shift has been suggested to be controlled by Galphaq [56]. In C. elegans we also see a small shift in basal neural activity [4] and an involvement of Galphaq. Future work should reveal the relationship of these different systems.

Supporting Information

Figure S1.

Statistical Analysis of nose speed, immobility and reduction of nose speed in egl- 30 mutants, egl-30 rescue mutants and hypoactive mutants during wake-like and sleep-like behavior. Mean velocity of nose speeds in wild type, egl-30 mutants, egl-30 rescue mutants and hypoactive mutants during A wake-like state and B sleep-like state. 
Percentage of immobility < 0.5µm/s in wild type, egl-30 mutants, egl-30 rescue mutants and hypoactive mutants during C wake-like state and D sleep-like state. 
E Percentage of nose speed reduction during lethargus in wild type, egl-30 mutants, egl-30 rescue mutants and hypoactive mutants. Errors are SEM. Statistical test is two- sample t-test. * denotes statistical significance at P<0.05, ** denotes statistical significance at P<0.01, *** denotes statistical significance at P<0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075853.s001

(TIF)

Acknowledgments

We thank A. Zarzycka, E. Friedhoff and M. Pilot for help with manual data processing and I. Rabinowitch and W. Schafer for ljIs105. Some strains were provided by the CGC, which is funded by NIH (P40 OD010440).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JS HB. Performed the experiments: JS. Analyzed the data: JS. Wrote the manuscript: JS HB.

References

  1. 1. Campbell SS, Tobler I (1984) Animal sleep: a review of sleep duration across phylogeny. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 8: 269-300. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(84)90054-X. PubMed: 6504414.
  2. 2. Raizen DM, Zimmerman JE, Maycock MH, Ta UD, You YJ et al. (2008) Lethargus is a Caenorhabditis elegans sleep-like state. Nature 451: 569-572. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06535. PubMed: 18185515.
  3. 3. Cassada RC, Russell RL (1975) The dauerlarva, a post-embryonic developmental variant of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Dev Biol 46: 326-342. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0012-1606(75)90109-8. PubMed: 1183723.
  4. 4. Schwarz J, Lewandrowski I, Bringmann H (2011) Reduced activity of a sensory neuron during a sleep-like state in Caenorhabditis elegans. Curr Biol 21: R983-R984. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.046. PubMed: 22192827.
  5. 5. Schwarz J, Spies J, Bringmann H (2012) Reduced muscle contraction and a relaxed posture during sleep-like Lethargus. Worm 1: 12-14.
  6. 6. Iwanir S, Tramm N, Nagy S, Wrigth C, et al DI. (2012) The microarchiteture of C. elegans behavior during lethargus: homeostatic bout dynamics, a typical body-posture and regulation by a central neuron. Sleep.
  7. 7. Singh K, Huang H, Hart AC (2013) Do C. elegans Sleep? A Closer Look. Sleep 36: 307-308. PubMed: 23450901.
  8. 8. Jeon M, Gardner HF, Miller EA, Deshler J, Rougvie AE (1999) Similarity of the C. elegans developmental timing protein LIN-42 to circadian rhythm proteins. Science 286: 1141-1146. doi:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.286.5442.1141. PubMed: 10550049.
  9. 9. Monsalve GC, Van Buskirk C, Frand AR (2011) LIN-42/PERIOD controls cyclical and developmental progression of C. elegans molts. Curr Biol 21: 2033-2045. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.054. PubMed: 22137474.
  10. 10. Avery L (1993) The genetics of feeding in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 133: 897-917. PubMed: 8462849.
  11. 11. Van Buskirk C, Sternberg PW (2007) Epidermal growth factor signaling induces behavioral quiescence in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Neurosci 10: 1300-1307. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1981. PubMed: 17891142.
  12. 12. Singh K, Chao MY, Somers GA, Komatsu H, Corkins ME et al. (2011) C. elegans Notch Signaling Regulates Adult Chemosensory Response and Larval Molting Quiescence. Curr Biol 21: 825-834. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.07.043. PubMed: 21549604.
  13. 13. Brundage L, Avery L, Katz A, Kim UJ, Mendel JE et al. (1996) Mutations in a C. elegans Gqalpha gene disrupt movement, egg laying, and viability. Neuron 16: 999-1009. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80123-3. PubMed: 8630258.
  14. 14. Lackner MR, Nurrish SJ, Kaplan JM (1999) Facilitation of synaptic transmission by EGL-30 Gqalpha and EGL-8 PLCbeta: DAG binding to UNC-13 is required to stimulate acetylcholine release. Neuron 24: 335-346. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80848-X. PubMed: 10571228.
  15. 15. Trent C, Tsuing N, Horvitz HR (1983) Egg-laying defective mutants of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 104: 619-647. PubMed: 11813735.
  16. 16. Park EC, Horvitz HR (1986) Mutations with dominant effects on the behavior and morphology of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 113: 821-852. PubMed: 3744028.
  17. 17. Doi M, Iwasaki K (2002) Regulation of retrograde signaling at neuromuscular junctions by the novel C2 domain protein AEX-1. Neuron 33: 249-259. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00587-6. PubMed: 11804572.
  18. 18. Brenner S (1974) The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 77: 71-94. PubMed: 4366476.
  19. 19. Bringmann H (2011) Agarose hydrogel microcompartments for imaging sleep- and wake-like behavior and nervous system development in Caenorhabditis elegans larvae. J Neurosci Methods 201: 78-88. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.07.013. PubMed: 21801751.
  20. 20. Iglewicz B, Hoaglin DC (1993) How to Detect and Handle Outliers. Milwaukee, WI: Amercian Society for Quality Control.
  21. 21. Rogulja D, Young MW (2012) Control of sleep by cyclin A and its regulator. Science 335: 1617-1621. doi:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1212476. PubMed: 22461610.
  22. 22. Koh K, Joiner WJ, Wu MN, Yue Z, Smith CJ et al. (2008) Identification of SLEEPLESS, a sleep-promoting factor. Science 321: 372-376. doi:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1155942. PubMed: 18635795.
  23. 23. Schade MA, Reynolds NK, Dollins CM, Miller KG (2005) Mutations that rescue the paralysis of Caenorhabditis elegans ric-8 (synembryn) mutants activate the G alpha(s) pathway and define a third major branch of the synaptic signaling network. Genetics 169: 631-649. doi:https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.104.032334. PubMed: 15489510.
  24. 24. Robatzek M, Thomas JH (2000) Calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II regulates Caenorhabditis elegans locomotion in concert with a G(o)/G(q) signaling network. Genetics 156: 1069-1082. PubMed: 11063685.
  25. 25. Koelle MR, Horvitz HR (1996) EGL-10 regulates G protein signaling in the C. elegans nervous system and shares a conserved domain with many mammalian proteins. Cell 84: 115-125. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-8674(00)80998-8. PubMed: 8548815.
  26. 26. Miller KG, Emerson MD, McManus JR, Rand JB (2000) RIC-8 (Synembryn): a novel conserved protein that is required for G(q)alpha signaling in the C. elegans nervous system. Neuron 27: 289-299. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00037-4. PubMed: 10985349.
  27. 27. Avery L, Bargmann CI, Horvitz HR (1993) The Caenorhabditis elegans unc-31 gene affects multiple nervous system-controlled functions. Genetics 134: 455-464. PubMed: 8325482.
  28. 28. Maruyama IN, Brenner S (1991) A phorbol ester/diacylglycerol-binding protein encoded by the unc-13 gene of Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 88: 5729-5733. doi:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.88.13.5729. PubMed: 2062851.
  29. 29. Tabuse Y, Nishiwaki K, Miwa J (1989) Mutations in a protein kinase C homolog confer phorbol ester resistance on Caenorhabditis elegans. Science 243: 1713-1716. doi:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2538925. PubMed: 2538925.
  30. 30. Weinshenker D, Garriga G, Thomas JH (1995) Genetic and pharmacological analysis of neurotransmitters controlling egg laying in C. elegans. J Neurosci 15: 6975-6985. PubMed: 7472454.
  31. 31. Williams SL, Lutz S, Charlie NK, Vettel C, Ailion M et al. (2007) Trio’s Rho-specific GEF domain is the missing Galpha q effector in C. elegans. Genes Dev 21: 2731-2746. doi:https://doi.org/10.1101/gad.1592007. PubMed: 17942708.
  32. 32. Hajdu-Cronin YM, Chen WJ, Patikoglou G, Koelle MR, Sternberg PW (1999) Antagonism between G(o)alpha and G(q)alpha in Caenorhabditis elegans: the RGS protein EAT-16 is necessary for G(o)alpha signaling and regulates G(q)alpha activity. Genes Dev 13: 1780-1793. doi:https://doi.org/10.1101/gad.13.14.1780. PubMed: 10421631.
  33. 33. Moorman C, Plasterk RH (2002) Functional characterization of the adenylyl cyclase gene sgs-1 by analysis of a mutational spectrum in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 161: 133-142. PubMed: 12019229.
  34. 34. Schafer WR, Kenyon CJ (1995) A calcium-channel homologue required for adaptation to dopamine and serotonin in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 375: 73-78. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/375073a0. PubMed: 7723846.
  35. 35. Schafer WR, Sanchez BM, Kenyon CJ (1996) Genes affecting sensitivity to serotonin in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 143: 1219-1230. PubMed: 8807295.
  36. 36. Sze JY, Victor M, Loer C, Shi Y, Ruvkun G (2000) Food and metabolic signalling defects in a Caenorhabditis elegans serotonin-synthesis mutant. Nature 403: 560-564. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/35000609. PubMed: 10676966.
  37. 37. Jansen G, Weinkove D, Plasterk RH (2002) The G-protein gamma subunit gpc-1 of the nematode C.elegans is involved in taste adaptation. EMBO J 21: 986-994. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/emboj/21.5.986. PubMed: 11867526.
  38. 38. Shcherbo D, Murphy CS, Ermakova GV, Solovieva EA, Chepurnykh TV et al. (2009) Far-red fluorescent tags for protein imaging in living tissues. Biochem J 418: 567-574. doi:https://doi.org/10.1042/BJ20081949. PubMed: 19143658.
  39. 39. Redemann S, Schloissnig S, Ernst S, Pozniakowsky A, Ayloo S et al. (2011) Codon adaptation-based control of protein expression in C. elegans. Nat Methods 8: 250-252. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1565. PubMed: 21278743.
  40. 40. Jose AM, Bany IA, Chase DL, Koelle MR (2007) A specific subset of transient receptor potential vanilloid-type channel subunits in Caenorhabditis elegans endocrine cells function as mixed heteromers to promote neurotransmitter release. Genetics 175: 93-105. PubMed: 17057248.
  41. 41. Hughes TE, Zhang H, Logothetis DE, Berlot CH (2001) Visualization of a functional Galpha q-green fluorescent protein fusion in living cells. Association with the plasma membrane is disrupted by mutational activation and by elimination of palmitoylation sites, but not be activation mediated by receptors or AlF4. J Biol Chem 276: 4227-4235. doi:https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M007608200. PubMed: 11076942.
  42. 42. Maduro M, Pilgrim D (1995) Identification and cloning of unc-119, a gene expressed in the Caenorhabditis elegans nervous system. Genetics 141: 977-988. PubMed: 8582641.
  43. 43. Fire A, Waterston RH (1989) Proper expression of myosin genes in transgenic nematodes. EMBO J 8: 3419-3428. PubMed: 2583105.
  44. 44. Kaplan JM, Horvitz HR (1993) A dual mechanosensory and chemosensory neuron in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 90: 2227-2231. doi:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.90.6.2227. PubMed: 8460126.
  45. 45. Guo ZV, Hart AC, Ramanathan S (2009) Optical interrogation of neural circuits in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Methods 6: 891-896. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1397. PubMed: 19898486.
  46. 46. Troemel ER, Chou JH, Dwyer ND, Colbert HA, Bargmann CI (1995) Divergent seven transmembrane receptors are candidate chemosensory receptors in C. elegans. Cell 83: 207-218. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-8674(95)90162-0. PubMed: 7585938.
  47. 47. Ezcurra M, Tanizawa Y, Swoboda P, Schafer WR (2011) Food sensitizes C. elegans avoidance behaviours through acute dopamine signalling. EMBO J 30: 1110-1122. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/emboj.2011.22. PubMed: 21304491.
  48. 48. Edwards SL, Charlie NK, Milfort MC, Brown BS, Gravlin CN et al. (2008) A novel molecular solution for ultraviolet light detection in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLOS Biol 6: e198. doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0060198. PubMed: 18687026.
  49. 49. Chalfie M, Sulston JE, White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN et al. (1985) The neural circuit for touch sensitivity in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci 5: 956-964. PubMed: 3981252.
  50. 50. Alkema MJ, Hunter-Ensor M, Ringstad N, Horvitz HR (2005) Tyramine Functions independently of octopamine in the Caenorhabditis elegans nervous system. Neuron 46: 247-260. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.024. PubMed: 15848803.
  51. 51. Berger AJ, Hart AC, Kaplan JM (1998) G alphas-induced neurodegeneration in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci 18: 2871-2880. PubMed: 9526004.
  52. 52. Miller KG, Emerson MD, Rand JB (1999) Goalpha and diacylglycerol kinase negatively regulate the Gqalpha pathway in C. elegans. Neuron 24: 323-333. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80847-8. PubMed: 10571227.
  53. 53. Llinás R, Jahnsen H (1982) Electrophysiology of mammalian thalamic neurones in vitro. Nature 297: 406-408. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/297406a0. PubMed: 7078650.
  54. 54. Zhan XJ, Cox CL, Rinzel J, Sherman SM (1999) Current clamp and modeling studies of low-threshold calcium spikes in cells of the cat’s lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurophysiol 81: 2360-2373. PubMed: 10322072.
  55. 55. Steriade M, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ (1993) Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science 262: 679-685. doi:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.8235588. PubMed: 8235588.
  56. 56. Coulon P, Kanyshkova T, Broicher T, Munsch T, Wettschureck N et al. (2010) Activity Modes in Thalamocortical Relay Neurons are Modulated by G(q)/G(11) Family G-proteins - Serotonergic and Glutamatergic Signaling. Front Cell Neurosci 4: 132. PubMed: 21267426.