Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Rates of Decline in Alzheimer Disease Decrease with Age

  • Dominic Holland ,

    dominic.holland@gmail.com

    Affiliation Department of Neurosciences, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, United States of America

  • Rahul S. Desikan,

    Affiliation Department of Radiology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, United States of America

  • Anders M. Dale,

    Affiliations Department of Neurosciences, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, United States of America, Department of Radiology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, United States of America

  • Linda K. McEvoy,

    Affiliation Department of Radiology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, United States of America

  • for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative

Abstract

Age is the strongest risk factor for sporadic Alzheimer disease (AD), yet the effects of age on rates of clinical decline and brain atrophy in AD have been largely unexplored. Here, we examined longitudinal rates of change as a function of baseline age for measures of clinical decline and structural MRI-based regional brain atrophy, in cohorts of AD, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and cognitively healthy (HC) individuals aged 65 to 90 years (total n = 723). The effect of age was modeled using mixed effects linear regression. There was pronounced reduction in rates of clinical decline and atrophy with age for AD and MCI individuals, whereas HCs showed increased rates of clinical decline and atrophy with age. This resulted in convergence in rates of change for HCs and patients with advancing age for several measures. Baseline cerebrospinal fluid densities of AD-relevant proteins, Aβ1–42, tau, and phospho-tau181p (ptau), showed a similar pattern of convergence with advanced age across cohorts, particularly for ptau. In contrast, baseline clinical measures did not differ by age, indicating uniformity of clinical severity at baseline. These results imply that the phenotypic expression of AD is relatively mild in individuals older than approximately 85 years, and this may affect the ability to distinguish AD from normal aging in the very old. Our findings show that inclusion of older individuals in clinical trials will substantially reduce the power to detect disease-modifying therapeutic effects, leading to dramatic increases in required clinical trial sample sizes with age of study sample.

Introduction

Age is the strongest risk factor for sporadic Alzheimer disease (AD) [1], [2], the most common cause of dementia. Due to growth in the proportion of elderly individuals in many societies, the prevalence of AD is increasing at an alarming rate [3][5]. Individuals over 85 years of age comprise the fastest growing segment of US society [6], yet it is not clear whether AD progresses in the same manner among the very old as among individuals who are affected at a younger age.

Pathologically, AD is characterized by the misfolding [7][9] and accumulation of amyloid-beta (Aβ) and tau proteins in insoluble aggregates, leading to the formation of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) [10], [11]. The increase in burden of these hallmark AD lesions in normal aging [12][14], combined with the increasing inability of aging cells to maintain disease proteins in benign states [15] and the exponential growth in the incidence of AD with increasing age (at least through approximately 85 years [16]), could suggest that the rate of decline in AD also increases with age. Emerging literature indicates, however, that the burden of AD lesions in individuals with AD decreases with age [17], while cognitive and structural changes might be more salient in younger rather than older individuals with AD [18]. This may reduce the ability to detect AD among the oldest old, potentially contributing to the controversy over whether AD incidence rates go up, down, or remain stable after age 85 [16].

Since the elderly population is growing, and proportionally growing faster with increasing age, it is important to better understand whether and how age interacts with the disease process to affect rates of decline. Here, we investigated whether rates of clinical decline and structural MRI-based measures of regional brain atrophy differ according to age among healthy controls (HCs) and individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or AD. We also assessed age-dependence of baseline clinical measures, and morphometric and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers of AD pathology. We explored the implication of the observed age-related differences in atrophic and clinical rates of decline for clinical trial design, and for disease biomarker trajectories.

Methods

We examined participants from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI, www.adni-info.org). Relevant details of ADNI, including participant enrollment criteria, MR image acquisition, and CSF collection and analysis methods are provided in File S1.

Participants

Participants enrolled in ADNI are not depressed, have a modified Hachinski score of 4 or less, and have a study partner able to provide an independent evaluation of functioning. HC participants have a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) of 0 [19]. Participants with MCI have a subjective memory complaint, objective memory loss measured by education-adjusted scores on Wechsler Memory Scale Logical Memory II, a CDR of 0.5, preserved activities of daily living, and absence of dementia. Participants with AD have a CDR of 0.5 or 1.0 and meet National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke and Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria for probable AD [20]. Further details on inclusion/exclusion criteria are described in the ADNI 1 protocol (available at www.adni-info.org/Scientists/ProceduresManuals.aspx).

We evaluated 723 older participants (222 HC, 345 MCI, and 156 AD, Table 1), who were tested at 6- or 12-month intervals for up to 24 (AD) or 36 (HC and MCI) months. The research protocol was approved by each local institutional review board, and written informed consent was obtained from each participant. To exclude patients with early-onset AD [21], and to have a balanced age range of HC and MCI individuals with respect to the AD cohort, we restricted analysis to participants aged 65 years or older at baseline.

thumbnail
Table 1. Number of participants in each age group, for each class of measure analyzed, separately for longitudinal and baseline analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.t001

MRI Image Processing

We preprocessed all MRI scans using image correction procedures for site-specific distortion effects updated for recent scanner changes [22]. We quantified anatomical regional change in serial MRI using Quarc [23], [24], a recently developed method from our laboratory. The longitudinal outcome measure of change with respect to baseline was calculated by directly registering each followup scan to the baseline scan. To evaluate baseline region of interest (ROI) measurements, we used a structural MRI post-processing technique that automatically delineates subcortical [25] and cortical [26] ROIs. We analyzed data from all available time points that passed local quality control (total = 2204). The number of followup scans was reduced by approximately 15% primarily due to motion artifacts, change in scanner model, or change in RF coil, as described in [22].

Methodological bias in image registration leading to elevated effect sizes remains a concern in the AD structural neuroimaging literature [24], particularly given recent reports [27][32] of results that are ostensibly corrected for bias but in fact, as shown in [24], remain significantly biased. Several research groups have developed robust approaches to reducing or eliminating bias [33], [34]. Our explicitly inverse-consistent approach [23] essentially eliminates potential bias by combining forward and reverse image registrations, and has been assessed vis-à-vis other approaches in [24].

Clinical and CSF Measures

We analyzed age-dependence of baseline values and longitudinal rates of change for clinical measures used to assess disease severity–the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale, sum of boxes score (CDR-SB) [19], [35]; the cognitive subscale of the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS-Cog); and the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE). There were 493 AD, 1474 MCI, and 795 HC participant-visits (total = 2762).

We also examined whether values of three previously validated CSF biomarkers of AD pathology, Aβ1–42 (Aβ), tau, and phospho-tau181p (ptau), differed with age at baseline. CSF data were available on approximately half of the participants, as indicated in Table 1.

Mixed Effects Modeling

Longitudinal outcomes (Yij below) in all cases were change measured with respect to baseline (expressed as a percentage of baseline size for cortical thickness change and ROI volume change). MRI change-measures for each participant were calculated from directly registering each followup image to the participant’s baseline image [23].

Using all available time-points per participant, we investigated the relationship between atrophy rate and age, and rate of clinical decline and age using a linear mixed effects model [36] where, for a specific diagnostic group, the longitudinal outcome measurement Yij at time tij for participant i at followup timepoint j is Yij =  mitij + biaitij + εij. Here, εij is the within-participant error, assumed to be independent and identically normally distributed with zero mean and variance σe2; mi = m + γi, where m is the fixed effect slope (mean rate of change for the group at the average age of the group) when controlling for age effects and γi is the corresponding between-participant random effect slope with variance σm2; ai is the mean-centered baseline age (years) of participant i, i.e., the participant’s baseline age minus the average baseline age for the group, and bi = b + δi, where b is the additional, age-coupled, fixed effect contribution to the slope and δi is the corresponding between-participant random effect slope with variance σb2. We estimated the model parameters using the Matlab (R2009b) function nlmefit.

We tested the resulting linear fit for each cohort’s full data set against point estimates of annual rate of change, with 95% confidence intervals (CIs), for successive 5-year intervals in baseline age, calculated without an age-dependent term (setting the regression coefficients bi≡0), to assess the correctness of the estimated age-dependence of group atrophy rates. This means that, in addition to the mixed effects model just described applied to the full data set, we also assessed successive 5-year age ranges using a simpler mixed effects model, i.e., one not controlling for age, on the assumption that if there are any age-effects for 5-year windows they will be small and can be ignored. In other words, for each 5-year interval, we directly calculate the atrophy rate (as previously done in [24]) ignoring age as a covariate. All our regression models are linear in the regression parameters and all covariates, including time.

To determine the significance of the differences, between groups, of the slopes estimated for the linear dependence of atrophy rate on age, as shown in Tables 2 and 3, we used two-sample t-tests for independent samples with unequal variances (Satterthwaite’s method [37]).

We used generalized linear model regression to estimate group baseline values as a function of age for CSF, cognitive, and structural measures. We tested the resulting linear fit against point estimates from successive 5-year intervals in baseline age and plotted the results with 95% CIs.

Sample Size Estimates

The disease effect, or maximum potentially treatable effect, for a patient cohort is the rate of decline calculated from the linear mixed effects model for the patient cohort that is in excess of that calculated for the HCs [24], and is potentially age-dependent. To estimate the sample size required in a two-arm (equal allocation) trial of a hypothetical disease-modifying treatment versus placebo, we assumed the treatment effect size of interest was 25% of the maximum potential treatment effect. We further assumed that the trial was of 24-months duration with a 6-months assessment interval, and that it would have 80% power to detect the treatment effect using a 2-sided significance level of 0.05 [24]. The power calculations, modeling linear change over time for each participant as described above, for a given age, and based on the estimated rate of decline for the patient cohorts relative to the rate of decline experienced at that age by HCs, incorporated the variance parameters for the patient cohort estimated from the mixed effects model [36]. Confidence intervals of 95% for sample size were based on 95% confidence intervals for the treatment effect size of interest.

Results

Annual Rates of Atrophy and Clinical Decline

Figure 1 shows annual atrophy rates versus age for several neuroanatomical ROIs, for HC, MCI, and AD participants. The lines represent group fixed effects linear fits from mixed effects modeling of atrophy rates, including a linear dependence of atrophy rate on baseline age. The point estimates (with 95% CIs) for successive 5-year intervals are derived from independent linear mixed effect modeling without a baseline age term, and thus show the mean change experienced by participants in each 5-year age group, in each diagnostic category. The closeness of fit of the point estimates to the fixed-effects lines throughout the age range of 65 to 90 years demonstrates a strong linear dependence of atrophy rate on baseline age. Of particular significance is the pronounced decrease in atrophy rate with increasing baseline age for patient cohorts, while for HCs rates of decline either remain constant or increase with age (slopes and p-values are in Table 2). The differences in slopes between HC and patient groups were highly significant for all ROIs (p-values are in Table 2). This leads to a pattern of convergence in atrophy rates across the patient and control groups as age increases. Although reduction in atrophy rate for patient cohorts is observable in all ROIs, the degree of convergence across diagnostic groups at later ages is not uniform among ROIs. In particular, ROIs affected early in AD–the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex–maintain a significant difference in atrophy rates between HCs and AD individuals, even at 85 years, while atrophy rates for whole brain and inferior parietal cortex, for example, show almost complete convergence among diagnostic groups at this age.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Mixed effects model fit for annual atrophy rates, allowing for linear change with age.

Data points plotted, with 95% confidence intervals, are independent estimates of the atrophy rates for successive 5-year intervals for a mixed effects model fit without an age-dependent term. Slopes and p-values of the linear fits of atrophy rates with age for each cohort are shown in Table 2. Note that atrophy rate is shown by a signed value; thus a reduction in atrophy rate with age is evidenced by a positive slope in the linear fit. Legend: red  = AD; blue  =  MCI; green  = HC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.g001

thumbnail
Table 2. Slopes and p-values for annual atrophy rates as a function of age shown in Figure 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.t002

Figure 2 shows annual rates of decline versus age for CDR-SB, ADAS-Cog, and MMSE. Rate of decline for CDR-SB did not change with age. However, AD patients showed a decrease in rate of decline for ADAS-Cog and MMSE, whereas HCs showed a small increase in rate of decline for these measures with age, resulting in a pattern of convergence in rates of decline with advancing age (slopes and p-values are in Table 3). The differences in slopes for MMSE between HC and both patient groups, and for ADAS-Cog between HC and AD were highly significant (p-values are in Table 3).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Mixed effects model fit for annual rates of clinical decline, allowing for linear change with age.

Data points plotted, with 95% confidence intervals, are independent estimates of rates of change for successive 5-year intervals for a mixed effects model fit without an age-dependent term. Slopes and p-values of the linear fits for each diagnostic group are shown in Table 3. Legend: red  = AD; blue  =  MCI; green  = HC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Slopes and p-values for annual rates of clinical decline as a function of age shown in Figure 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.t003

Baseline Clinical and CSF Measures

Figure 3, right column, shows the estimated relation of baseline clinical measures to baseline age, modeled with linear fits, along with the independent point estimates, with 95% CI, at successive 5-year intervals for HC, MCI, and AD participants. For all three measures, diagnostic groups are well separated across the age range, with no evidence of age-dependency on baseline clinical score.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Linear fit to baseline CSF and cognitive measures, along with independent estimates of baseline measures (with 95% confidence intervals) at successive 5-year intervals.

Legend: red  =  AD; blue  =  MCI; green  =  HC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.g003

In contrast, Figure 3, left column, indicates an age-dependency for baseline values of CSF Aβ, tau, and especially ptau. At the younger age range, AD patients show higher ptau levels than HCs. However, ptau significantly increases with age in HCs (slope  = 0.64 pg/(mL year); p = 0.035) but significantly decreases with age in AD (slope  = −1.2 pg/(mL year); p = 0.0038).

Plots of the age-dependence of baseline structural measures are shown in Figure 4. As expected, structures continue to atrophy as age advances (negative slopes for the HCs), and as disease advances (in general, smaller structures for more advanced disease stages at any given age). Differences in baseline structural measures between AD and HC groups decrease with advancing age for whole brain, inferior parietal lobule, and retrosplenial cortex.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Linear fit to baseline structural MRI measures, along with independent estimates of baseline measures (with 95% confidence intervals) at successive 5-year intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.g004

Rate of Progression to AD

Of 329 MCI participants with longitudinal clinical data aged 65 years and older, 127 participants were known to progress to AD within 36-months from baseline. Data points in Figure 5 show the percentage of these converters for successive 3-year baseline age intervals. Linear fit to the data has slope = −0.94%/year (standard error = 0.11, p = 0.00016). Between the ages of 65 and 90 years, the estimated progression rate approximately halves: from almost 50% to 25%.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Data points show percentage, for successive 3-year age groups, of participants who progressed to dementia within 36-months from baseline.

Linear fit to data has slope  = −0.94 [%/year] (standard error  = 0.11, p = 0.00016). Ratios give the number of those who developed dementia to the total number of MCI participants, for each 3-year age window.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.g005

Sample Size Estimates

Figures 6 and 7 show sample size estimates (with approximate 95% CIs) as a function of baseline age for MCI and AD participants, respectively, using rates of change relative to age-matched HCs in neuroanatomical ROIs and standard clinical tests as the outcome measures [24]. For neuroanatomical measures, substantially smaller sample sizes are needed to effectively power a clinical trial at younger ages, while at older ages the sample size estimates rise sharply. However, the baseline age at which the sharp increase sets in depends on the ROI: e.g., ∼75 years for whole brain and inferior parietal, ∼80 years for hippocampus, and >85 years for entorhinal, in AD participants. This rapid increase in estimated sample size with age stems from the convergence pattern in atrophy rates seen in Figure 1. A similar pattern is observed with ADAS-Cog as the outcome measure, consistent with the convergence across groups in annual rate of decline shown in Figure 2. The age-dependence of sample size estimates is less marked for the CDR-SB, but estimated sample sizes are still larger for older as compared with younger cohorts. (The effect of reduction in unexplained random slope variance by including age as a random effect is described in File S1.).

thumbnail
Figure 6. Estimated sample sizes with respect to age, per arm, to detect a 25% reduction in rate of change in MCI participants relative to age-matched change in HCs, at the p<0.05 level with 80% power assuming a 24 month trial with scans every six months.

Sample sizes are estimated using a linear mixed effects model with fixed intercepts (no relative change at baseline) and random slopes and linear dependence on age applied to all data available up through 36 months. Dashed lines show the 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.g006

thumbnail
Figure 7. Estimated sample sizes, with respect to age, per arm, to detect a 25% reduction in rate of change in AD subjects relative to age-matched change in HCs.

Other details are as described in the caption to Figure 6.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042325.g007

Potential Bias Due to Differential Dropout

It is possible that results could be affected by differential withdrawal rates. For example, the patterns of observed results could be explained if older patients with more severe symptoms withdrew after baseline from the study at a higher rate than younger subjects (e.g., due to increased morbidity or mortality). We therefore examined study discontinuation rates by age and symptom severity. Across MCI participants, 16 (4.6%) participants withdrew after the baseline session; these dropouts were distributed across the age ranges (2 aged 65–70; 7 aged 70–75; 2 aged 75–80; and 5 aged 80–85), with the exception that all MCI participants in the oldest age group completed at least one additional follow-up. For AD patients, 12 (7.7%) withdrew after the baseline session: 1 each in the 65–70 and 70–75 age groups, and 5 each in the 75–80 and 80–85 age groups.

There was no age-bias among the MCI participants who retained the diagnosis of MCI but who withdrew before the final (36-months) visit. For example, the ratio of the number of stable MCI participants with at least one followup but without a final visit, to the total number of MCI participants with at least one followup, for successive 5-year age groups between 65 and 90 years, was, respectively, 0.42, 0.44, 0.46, 0.46, and 0.57. Thus, differential dropout was unlikely to affect the estimated rate of progression to AD as a function of age.

Individuals who withdrew from the study after the baseline session did not differ significantly in symptom severity from those who completed one or more follow-up sessions. For example, MCI patients who withdrew after baseline showed a mean ADAS-Cog score of 11.1 (standard deviation, SD = 4.4) versus 11.6 (SD = 4.4; p = 0.65) for those who completed one or more follow-ups. For AD subjects, mean ADAS-Cog scores were 18.1 (SD = 4.3) and 19.9 (SD = 5.7; p = 0.24) for those who withdrew after baseline and those who completed at least one follow-up, respectively.

Discussion

In this study, we present three lines of evidence indicating that AD proceeds more aggressively among younger elderly than among older elderly individuals, leading to a blurring of the distinction between AD and HC among the oldest old. First, we found that annual brain atrophy rates for AD and MCI individuals decrease with increasing baseline age, while atrophy rates for clinically normal older individuals remain constant or exhibit a slight increase with age. Second, we found that baseline CSF biomarker levels indicated greater disease burden in younger than in older MCI and AD patients, while disease burden increased with age among HCs. Third, we found that AD patients showed reduced rates of cognitive decline with increasing baseline age, despite uniformity of cognitive impairment at study entry. These findings have important implications for detection of AD among the oldest old, for the design of clinical trials of potentially disease-modifying therapies, and for biomarker and clinical disease trajectories.

All ROIs examined showed a decrease in longitudinal atrophy rate for MCI and AD individuals with increasing age at study entry. This decrease was most apparent in association cortices that tend to be affected later in the disease process, such as the inferior parietal lobule, middle temporal gyrus, and the retrosplenial cortex. The decrease in rate of atrophy with age in patient groups, combined with a trend toward increasing atrophy rates with age for HCs, resulted in a pattern of convergence of atrophy rates in association cortices across all diagnostic groups, with close convergence around 85 years of age, suggesting that in these brain regions neuronal number in patients is increasingly preserved with advancing baseline age [12]. However, the ROIs impacted by NFTs in earliest stages of the disorder (Braak stages I–II, transentorhinal, and stage III, limbic [38], [39]) continued to show significant differences in rates of decline for patients and older controls at more advanced ages, although differences were smaller than those observed at younger ages. Thus the power to discriminate MCI or AD individuals from HCs is differentially retained across ROIs, with the medial temporal ROIs that are particularly vulnerable to early neurofibrillary pathology retaining the strongest discriminative ability with advancing age.

In cross-sectional analyses, levels of disease burden as measured by CSF biomarkers decreased with increased age at study entry for MCI and AD participants, but increased with age for HC participants. The difference in the age-dependence of these biomarkers between patients and controls was particularly strong for ptau, leading to full convergence of ptau levels across patient groups at 85 years. The increase in ptau levels with age in HCs likely reflects increased burden of AD-specific neurofibrillary pathology [40], and may underlie the increase with age in rate of clinical decline [41] and atrophy rate for the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus [42] observed here for HCs.

Consistent with the structural MRI results, rates of longitudinal cognitive decline also showed convergence between patient and HC groups with increasing age at study entry. AD patients showed significant decrease in rate of decline on MMSE and a trend towards reduced rates of decline on ADAS-Cog with increased age, decreasing the difference in rates of decline between AD and HCs with increasing age. Due to uniform enrollment criteria across age, however, baseline cognitive measures did not differ with age within diagnostic groups, and showed constant separation among groups across age. This indicates that within each diagnostic group, individuals were at a uniform cognitive stage irrespective of baseline age. The differences observed in baseline CSF biomarkers and in rates of cognitive and structural change with age therefore do not stem from differences in clinical severity with age at the time of enrollment, but instead appear to reflect a decrease in rate of disease progression with baseline age. Within-cohort rates of decline for CDR-SB, however, were independent of age. CDR-SB measures global clinical or functional change, and may therefore not be as sensitive as ADAS-Cog to small declines in cognition [43]. A similar result was found in a recent study of age and rate of cognitive decline in AD [44], which also observed that older age at baseline was significantly associated with a slower rate of decline in ADAS-Cog 11, ADAS-memory, and MMSE. This decrease in rate of cognitive decline is consistent with our results showing decrease in 3-year rates of progression from MCI to dementia with advancing age: close to 50% of MCI participants aged 65 years developed dementia, whereas only about 25% of those aged 85 years did.

These findings showing a decrease with advancing age in multiple measures of neurodegeneration between AD patient cohorts and HCs are in agreement with earlier research showing that the correlation between dementia severity and NFT burden decreases with advancing age [45], and a more recent study assessing prevalence of moderate or severe pathological lesions in participants aged 69 to 103 years, which found attenuation with advancing age in the association between dementia and the densities of both neuritic plaques and NFTs in all examined ROIs [17]. In contrast, however, the latter study also found that dichotomized measures of neocortical and hippocampal atrophy allowed for distinguishing individuals with dementia from those without dementia, regardless of age, in agreement with the retained discriminative ability of hippocampus and entorhinal cortex measures shown here. Our findings are also in agreement with a recent cross-sectional analysis showing that the AD-related cognitive and structural MRI changes seen in AD patients aged 60–75 years are less salient in patients aged 80–91 years [18], and a recent longitudinal MRI analysis showing that rates of whole brain atrophy were greater in younger than older participants with amnestic MCI [46].

Our results showing smaller baseline structure size in older as compared with younger participants, modeled using linear fits with age, are in broad agreement with earlier manual and automated volumetry analyses [47][49]. In the latter study [49], covering the seventh through tenth decades in age, a generalized additive model (GAM) [50] was used, and nonlinear trends in baseline structure size as a function of age were reported. In the later decades, in particular, the cross-sectionally assessed atrophy accelerated or decelerated with age, depending on ROI and diagnostic group. It should be noted that cross-sectional analyses are not the most accurate approach to estimating longitudinal rates of change. It is also important to note that with GAMs the uncertainty in estimated structure size as a function of age can grow substantially from the middle of the age range toward the younger and older extremes in the age range modeled–which is often where the estimated nonlinearities tend to be most pronounced. This increased uncertainty may help account for discrepancies with other findings. For example, cross-sectional analysis comparing manual with voxel-based morphometry measures of hippocampal volume in healthy individuals [47] indicates acceleration of hippocampal atrophy with age in HCs. This is in agreement with our longitudinal results and in broad agreement with earlier results from manual longitudinal volumetry [51], [52], but is at variance with the deceleration of hippocampal atrophy with age in HCs resulting from the GAM analysis of cross-sectional data [49].

Our findings of reduced rates of clinical and morphometric decline with age in patients with AD may help shed light on the contradictory results that have been reported for incidence of AD dementia for the oldest old, aged 90 years and older. For example, the Cache County Study [53][55] and an autopsy study [56] found that incidence rates of AD decrease, beginning in the early 90 s. In contrast, the 90+ Study examining all-cause dementia, including 60% of participants diagnosed with AD, had a larger contingent of oldest participants, and found that dementia incidence continued to increase with age [57]. Although our results do not directly address the incidence of AD with age, our finding that rate of cognitive decline decreases with baseline age suggests that for the oldest old, clinical detection of AD, which relies on progressive decline in cognitive ability, may be more difficult. Reduction in CSF biomarkers of disease burden, and in atrophic changes in brain structure, with age suggests that the newly revised criteria for diagnosis of AD in research settings, which recommends incorporation of these biomarkers [58], may not help overcome this problem. It should also be noted, more broadly, that since incidence is the number of new cases in a population in a given time period, e.g. a year, non-constant rates of decline with age are likely to play a role in the incidence at different ages of clinically diagnosed AD.

The blurring of the distinction between HCs and patients with MCI or AD with advanced age has important implications for clinical trial design. As shown in Figures 6 and 7, all potential outcome measures evaluated here provide significant power for detecting disease-modifying therapeutic effects for MCI and mild AD participants aged 65–75, with the previously reported advantage for structural measures, such as entorhinal cortex and hippocampus, over clinical measures [24]. However, power is dramatically reduced with increasing age of the study sample. Due to the exponential rise in prevalence of AD with age (through approximately 85 years [16]), older individuals will increasingly be more available for clinical trials as compared with younger individuals. Yet, detecting a decrease in atrophy rate, or in rate of clinical decline, due to a disease-modifying therapy becomes increasingly difficult the older the study cohort. Thus, the extent to which older individuals are represented in the study sample could profoundly affect the power for detecting a therapeutic effect. In particular, a small but significant disease-modifying effect, which could significantly reduce the global burden of the disease [5], might be found in younger cohorts but would likely not be found in older cohorts. Given demographic trends, there is an urgent need to develop disease-modifying therapies to avert what will otherwise be an AD epidemic [59][61]. Since the power to detect therapeutic effects can be reduced dramatically in older cohorts, it is of immediate importance to consider fully cohort age in drug discovery.

With regard to disease trajectories, the observed reduction in rates of decline with advancing age allows for two distinct, plausible scenarios. In the first scenario, a cognitively healthy individual begins to experience a slightly elevated rate of decline at an early point in life, so that over a prolonged period, tissue loss gradually accumulates and cognitive function gradually declines. This slow course continues into advanced ages. At some point, despite the gradual nature of change, enough tissue loss and cognitive impairment has ensued to enable a physician to diagnosis the disorder.

In the second scenario, a cognitively healthy older individual initially experiences the same rates of structural and cognitive change as those of other cognitively healthy individuals of the same age, but the individual begins to experience a more rapid course of decline as symptoms develop and worsen. Rate of decline then slows at some point, so that at an advanced age, the rate of decline again approaches the rate observed for healthy older individuals of the same age. In this scenario, the biomarker trajectory follows a sigmoidal course. Despite the popularity of sigmoidal curves for describing biomarker trajectories in AD [62][65], and the development of mathematical models of neuron death kinetics where neuron death is considered not to arise from cumulative damage [66], [67], there is no conclusive evidence demonstrating a slowing in rate of brain atrophy over time within individual AD patients. Furthermore, slowing in rate of neuronal loss from age and disease progression might not be biologically plausible: arguments from protein homeostasis indicate instead that cell death is more likely to accelerate than to slow with disease progression [15].

A recent investigation [68] of the shapes of AD biomarker trajectories examined whether, as suggested by sigmoidal models, hippocampal atrophy rate slowed with increasing disease severity in four patient cohorts. Employing a liberal significance threshold (p-value  = 0.1), and restricting the sample to amyloid-positive participants, evidence of slowing of the rate of volume loss with increasing disease severity, as assessed with MMSE score, was found in three cohorts when cross-sectional data were analyzed but not when longitudinal data were analyzed. In the fourth cohort the reverse occurred: evidence of slowing of the rate of volume loss with increasing disease severity was found when longitudinal data were analyzed but not when cross-sectional data were analyzed. These inconsistencies do not strongly support the sigmoidal model for AD-related atrophy. Furthermore, when the sample was not restricted to amyloid-positive participants, cross-sectional analyses consistently showed an increase in hippocampal atrophy with increasing disease severity, and longitudinal analyses consistently showed an increase in hippocampal atrophy rate with increasing disease severity–results that do not support the contention that atrophy rates eventually decelerate with advancing disease severity.

Another possible explanation of differences in rates of decline with age observed here is differential dropout, in which older patients with more severe decline dropped out of the study at a higher rate than younger individuals with more severe decline. However, evidence suggests that such differential dropout is unlikely to have affected the observed results. Older participants were not more likely than younger participants to drop out after the baseline session, and there was no difference in symptom severity between those who dropped out after baseline versus those who completed one or more follow-ups. However, differential recruitment into the study may contribute to the observed results. Participation in a trial such as ADNI requires a large commitment of time and effort. It is possible that for the oldest old patients with AD, if they were experiencing a rapid course of decline, they or their caregivers may have been less willing to participate in a burdensome, non-treatment study than those with milder decline. Such differential enrollment, where younger but not older rapidly declining individuals enrolled in the study seems unlikely given the systematic decrease in rates of decline with increasing age for both MCI and AD cohorts.

Limitations of this study include the highly select nature of the study sample: individuals were required to be generally healthy with no evidence of comorbid disorders that could affect study participation, such as depression or vascular dementia. A second limitation is the lack of histopathological verification of AD pathology. It is possible that many participants suffer from subclinical cerebrovascular disease, and the contribution of cerebrovascular disease to dementia symptoms may be greater among the very old. A recent neuropathological study [69], in addition to finding attenuation in the association between dementia and NFT burden in nonagenarians, also found an increase in vascular burden in mixed pathologies in people over 90 years. This increases the possibility of misdiagnosis, particularly since microvascular pathology and AD are synergistic in the development of dementia in old age [70], [71]. Furthermore, and somewhat circularly, the convergent patterns in rates of decline might themselves contribute to increased potential for misdiagnosis at older ages. The small number of AD individuals over the age of 85 years is a further limitation of the study.

Despite these limitations, this study suggests that the association between neuropathological markers and dementia attenuates with age and needs to be taken into account in models of AD. The degree of attenuation is remarkably uniform across diverse markers–CSF AD-related protein densities, atrophy rates in multiple brain regions, and rates of clinical decline–all showing strong convergence patterns across diagnostic groups at older ages, though it is significant that atrophy rates in the hippocampus and particularly the entorhinal cortex indicate slower convergence. As a result, methods for early disease detection and assessment of therapeutic interventions cannot be applied uniformly across the entire elderly age spectrum. Given demographic trends, in particular the rapid growth in the proportion of very old individuals, greater emphasis needs to be placed on further elucidating the effects of age on the disease process to better prepare for the diagnosis, care, and treatment of the oldest old.

Acknowledgments

Data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.ucla.edu). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at: http://adni.loni.ucla.edu/wp-content/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: DH. Performed the experiments: DH. Analyzed the data: DH. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: DH AMD. Wrote the paper: DH RSD LKM. Responsible for the study concept and design, all levels of data analysis, drafting the report, tables, and figures: DH. Contributed critically to data interpretation and revision of the report: DH RSD LKM. Contributed to data acquisition and analysis: AMD.

References

  1. 1. Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Beckett LA, Albert MS, Pilgrim DM, et al. (1995) Age-specific incidence of Alzheimer’s disease in a community population. JAMA : the journal of the American Medical Association 273: 1354–1359.
  2. 2. Kukull WA, Higdon R, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, et al. (2002) Dementia and Alzheimer disease incidence: a prospective cohort study. Arch Neurol 59: 1737–1746.
  3. 3. Brookmeyer R, Gray S, Kawas C (1998) Projections of Alzheimer’s disease in the United States and the public health impact of delaying disease onset. American journal of public health 88: 1337–1342.
  4. 4. Matthews F, Brayne C (2005) The incidence of dementia in England and Wales: findings from the five identical sites of the MRC CFA Study. PLoS Med 2: e193.
  5. 5. Brookmeyer R, Evans DA, Hebert L, Langa KM, Heeringa SG, et al. (2011) National estimates of the prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in the United States. Alzheimers Dement 7: 61–73.
  6. 6. Wan H, Sengupta M, Velkoff VA, De Barros KA (2005) 65+ in the United States: 2005. In: Bureau USC, editor. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. P23–209.
  7. 7. Soto C (2003) Unfolding the role of protein misfolding in neurodegenerative diseases. Nat Rev Neurosci 4: 49–60.
  8. 8. Ross CA, Poirier MA (2004) Protein aggregation and neurodegenerative disease. Nat Med 10 Suppl: S10–17
  9. 9. DeToma AS, Salamekh S, Ramamoorthy A, Lim MH (2012) Misfolded proteins in Alzheimer’s disease and type II diabetes. Chem Soc Rev 41: 608–621.
  10. 10. Querfurth HW, LaFerla FM (2010) Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 362: 329–344.
  11. 11. Braak H, Braak E (1991) Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 82: 239–259.
  12. 12. Freeman SH, Kandel R, Cruz L, Rozkalne A, Newell K, et al. (2008) Preservation of neuronal number despite age-related cortical brain atrophy in elderly subjects without Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 67: 1205–1212.
  13. 13. Price JL, McKeel DW Jr, Buckles VD, Roe CM, Xiong C, et al. (2009) Neuropathology of nondemented aging: presumptive evidence for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging 30: 1026–1036.
  14. 14. Morris JC, Roe CM, Xiong C, Fagan AM, Goate AM, et al. (2010) APOE predicts amyloid-beta but not tau Alzheimer pathology in cognitively normal aging. Ann Neurol 67: 122–131.
  15. 15. Douglas PM, Dillin A (2010) Protein homeostasis and aging in neurodegeneration. J Cell Biol 190: 719–729.
  16. 16. Brumback-Peltz C, Balasubramanian AB, Corrada MM, Kawas CH (2011) Diagnosing dementia in the oldest-old. Maturitas 70: 164–168.
  17. 17. Savva GM, Wharton SB, Ince PG, Forster G, Matthews FE, et al. (2009) Age, neuropathology, and dementia. N Engl J Med 360: 2302–2309.
  18. 18. Stricker NH, Chang YL, Fennema-Notestine C, Delano-Wood L, Salmon DP, et al. (2011) Distinct profiles of brain and cognitive changes in the very old with Alzheimer disease. Neurology 77: 713–721.
  19. 19. Morris JC (1993) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43: 2412–2414.
  20. 20. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, et al. (1984) Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 34: 939–944.
  21. 21. van der Flier WM, Pijnenburg YA, Fox NC, Scheltens P (2011) Early-onset versus late-ons et al zheimer’s disease: the case of the missing APOE varepsilon4 allele. Lancet Neurol 10: 280–288.
  22. 22. Holland D, Brewer JB, Hagler DJ, Fennema-Notestine C, Dale AM (2009) Subregional neuroanatomical change as a biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 20954–20959.
  23. 23. Holland D, Dale AM (2011) Nonlinear registration of longitudinal images and measurement of change in regions of interest. Med Image Anal 15: 489–497.
  24. 24. Holland D, McEvoy LK, Dale AM (2011) Unbiased comparison of sample size estimates from longitudinal structural measures in ADNI. Hum Brain Mapp.
  25. 25. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, et al. (2002) Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33: 341–355.
  26. 26. Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, et al. (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31: 968–980.
  27. 27. Hua X, Gutman B, Boyle C, Rajagopalan P, Leow AD, et al. (2011) Accurate measurement of brain changes in longitudinal MRI scans using tensor-based morphometry. NeuroImage 57: 5–14.
  28. 28. Fox NC, Ridgway GR, Schott JM (2011) Algorithms, atrophy and Alzheimer’s disease: cautionary tales for clinical trials. Neuroimage 57: 15–18.
  29. 29. Jack CR Jr, Barkhof F, Bernstein MA, Cantillon M, Cole PE, et al. (2011) Steps to standardization and validation of hippocampal volumetry as a biomarker in clinical trials and diagnostic criterion for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 7: 474–485 e474.
  30. 30. Weiner MW, Veitch DP, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Cairns NJ, et al. (2012) The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative: a review of papers published since its inception. Alzheimers Dement 8: S1–68.
  31. 31. Jack CR Jr (2012) Alzheimer disease: new concepts on its neurobiology and the clinical role imaging will play. Radiology 263: 344–361.
  32. 32. Lee GJ, Lu PH, Hua X, Lee S, Wu S, et al. (2012) Depressive symptoms in mild cognitive impairment predict greater atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease-related regions. Biol Psychiatry 71: 814–821.
  33. 33. Reuter M, Schmansky NJ, Rosas HD, Fischl B (2012) Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage.
  34. 34. Leung KK, Ridgway GR, Ourselin S, Fox NC (2012) Consistent multi-time-point brain atrophy estimation from the boundary shift integral. Neuroimage 59: 3995–4005.
  35. 35. Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL (1982) A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science 140: 566–572.
  36. 36. Fitzmaurice GM, Laird NM, Ware JH (2011) Applied longitudinal analysis. Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley. xxv, 701 p.
  37. 37. Rosner B (2006) Fundamentals of biostatistics. Belmont, CA: Thomson-Brooks/Cole. xix, 868 p.
  38. 38. Braak H, Braak E (1995) Staging of Alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiol Aging 16: 271–278; discussion 278–284.
  39. 39. Braak H, Alafuzoff I, Arzberger T, Kretzschmar H, Del Tredici K (2006) Staging of Alzheimer disease-associated neurofibrillary pathology using paraffin sections and immunocytochemistry. Acta Neuropathol 112: 389–404.
  40. 40. Buerger K, Ewers M, Pirttila T, Zinkowski R, Alafuzoff I, et al. (2006) CSF phosphorylated tau protein correlates with neocortical neurofibrillary pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 129: 3035–3041.
  41. 41. Desikan RS, McEvoy LK, Thompson WK, Holland D, Brewer JB, et al. (2012) Amyloid-beta-Associated Clinical Decline Occurs Only in the Presence of Elevated P-tau. Arch Neurol.
  42. 42. Desikan RS, McEvoy LK, Thompson WK, Holland D, Roddey JC, et al. (2011) Amyloid-beta associated volume loss occurs only in the presence of phospho-tau. Ann Neurol.
  43. 43. Coley N, Andrieu S, Jaros M, Weiner M, Cedarbaum J, et al. (2011) Suitability of the Clinical Dementia Rating-Sum of Boxes as a single primary endpoint for Alzheimer’s disease trials. Alzheimers Dement 7: 602–610 e602.
  44. 44. Bernick C, Cummings J, Raman R, Sun X, Aisen P (2012) Age and Rate of Cognitive Decline in Alzheimer Disease: Implications for Clinical Trials. Arch Neurol.
  45. 45. Prohovnik I, Perl DP, Davis KL, Libow L, Lesser G, et al. (2006) Dissociation of neuropathology from severity of dementia in late-ons et al zheimer disease. Neurology 66: 49–55.
  46. 46. Jack CR Jr, Weigand SD, Shiung MM, Przybelski SA, O’Brien PC, et al. (2008) Atrophy rates accelerate in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 70: 1740–1752.
  47. 47. Kennedy KM, Erickson KI, Rodrigue KM, Voss MW, Colcombe SJ, et al. (2009) Age-related differences in regional brain volumes: a comparison of optimized voxel-based morphometry to manual volumetry. Neurobiol Aging 30: 1657–1676.
  48. 48. Walhovd KB, Westlye LT, Amlien I, Espeseth T, Reinvang I, et al. (2011) Consistent neuroanatomical age-related volume differences across multiple samples. Neurobiol Aging 32: 916–932.
  49. 49. Schuff N, Tosun D, Insel PS, Chiang GC, Truran D, et al. (2012) Nonlinear time course of brain volume loss in cognitively normal and impaired elders. Neurobiol Aging 33: 845–855.
  50. 50. Hastie T, Tibshirani R (1990) Generalized additive models. London; New York: Chapman and Hall. xv, 335 p.
  51. 51. Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, et al. (2005) Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb Cortex 15: 1676–1689.
  52. 52. Raz N, Ghisletta P, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Lindenberger U (2010) Trajectories of brain aging in middle-aged and older adults: regional and individual differences. Neuroimage 51: 501–511.
  53. 53. Miech RA, Breitner JC, Zandi PP, Khachaturian AS, Anthony JC, et al. (2002) Incidence of AD may decline in the early 90 s for men, later for women: The Cache County study. Neurology 58: 209–218.
  54. 54. Khachaturian AS, Corcoran CD, Mayer LS, Zandi PP, Breitner JC (2004) Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 count affects age at onset of Alzheimer disease, but not lifetime susceptibility: The Cache County Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61: 518–524.
  55. 55. Breitner JC, Wyse BW, Anthony JC, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Steffens DC, et al. (1999) APOE-epsilon4 count predicts age when prevalence of AD increases, then declines: the Cache County Study. Neurology 53: 321–331.
  56. 56. Jellinger KA, Attems J (2010) Prevalence of dementia disorders in the oldest-old: an autopsy study. Acta Neuropathol 119: 421–433.
  57. 57. Corrada MM, Brookmeyer R, Paganini-Hill A, Berlau D, Kawas CH (2010) Dementia incidence continues to increase with age in the oldest old: the 90+ study. Ann Neurol 67: 114–121.
  58. 58. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, et al. (2011) The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 7: 263–269.
  59. 59. Sperling RA, Jack CR Jr, Aisen PS (2011) Testing the right target and right drug at the right stage. Sci Transl Med 3: 111cm133.
  60. 60. Brookmeyer R, Johnson E, Ziegler-Graham K, Arrighi HM (2007) Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 3: 186–191.
  61. 61. Alzheimer’s Association (2010) 2010 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement 6: 158–194.
  62. 62. Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Aisen PS, et al. (2010) Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol 9: 119–128.
  63. 63. Frisoni GB, Fox NC, Jack CR Jr, Scheltens P, Thompson PM (2010) The clinical use of structural MRI in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol 6: 67–77.
  64. 64. Trojanowski JQ, Vandeerstichele H, Korecka M, Clark CM, Aisen PS, et al. (2010) Update on the biomarker core of the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative subjects. Alzheimers Dement 6: 230–238.
  65. 65. Caroli A, Frisoni GB (2010) The dynamics of Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative cohort. Neurobiol Aging 31: 1263–1274.
  66. 66. Lomasko T, Lumsden CJ (2009) One-hit stochastic decline in a mechanochemical model of cytoskeleton-induced neuron death III: diffusion pulse death zones. J Theor Biol 256: 104–116.
  67. 67. Clarke G, Lumsden CJ, McInnes RR (2001) Inherited neurodegenerative diseases: the one-hit model of neurodegeneration. Human molecular genetics 10: 2269–2275.
  68. 68. Jack CR Jr, Vemuri P, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Lesnick TG, et al. (2012) Shapes of the Trajectories of 5 Major Biomarkers of Alzheimer Disease. Arch Neurol.
  69. 69. Sinka L, Kovari E, Gold G, Hof PR, Herrmann FR, et al. (2010) Small vascular and Alzheimer disease-related pathologic determinants of dementia in the oldest-old. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 69: 1247–1255.
  70. 70. Kovari E, Gold G, Herrmann FR, Canuto A, Hof PR, et al. (2007) Cortical microinfarcts and demyelination affect cognition in cases at high risk for dementia. Neurology 68: 927–931.
  71. 71. Gold G, Giannakopoulos P, Herrmann FR, Bouras C, Kovari E (2007) Identification of Alzheimer and vascular lesion thresholds for mixed dementia. Brain 130: 2830–2836.